Spatial Point Processes and Brown Creepers (Certhia americana): Estimating Habitat Use of a Silvicultural Experiment Maria Mayrhofer A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science University of Washington 2006 Program Authorized to Offer Degree: College of Forest Resources University of Washington Graduate School This is to certify that I have examined this copy of a master’s thesis by Maria Mayrhofer and have found that it is complete and satisfactory in all respects, and that any and all revisions required by the final examining committee have been made. Committee Members: David A. Manuwal John M. Marzluff Mark S. Handcock Date:________________________________________ In presenting this thesis in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a master’s degree at the University of Washington, I agree that the library shall make its copies freely available for inspection. I further agree that extensive copying of this thesis is allowable only for scholarly purposes, consistent with “fair use” as prescribed in the U.S. Copyright Law. Any other reproduction for any purposes or by any means shall not be allowed without my written permission. Signature________________________________________ Date________________________________________ TABLE OF CONTENTS Page List of Figures...............................................................................................................iii List of Tables................................................................................................................iv Introduction ...................................................................................................................1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 4 Study Area and Design............................................................................................ 4 Field Methods.......................................................................................................... 5 Brown Creeper Monitoring ............................................................................... 5 Nest Monitoring................................................................................................. 7 Quantifying DEMO Resources................................................................................ 7 Brown Creeper Territories................................................................................. 7 Landcover .......................................................................................................... 7 Contrast-weighted Edge .................................................................................... 8 Distance to Nest................................................................................................. 8 Linking Bird Locations to Stand-level Resources................................................... 9 Spatial Point Process Model.............................................................................. 9 Describing Stand-level Resource Use .............................................................12 Results ......................................................................................................................... 14 Use of Stand-level Resource Attributes ................................................................ 14 Nest Placement and Success.................................................................................. 16 Discussion.................................................................................................................... 17 Implications for Use of Harvested Forest Patches ................................................ 17 Implications for Nesting at Edges .........................................................................19 Evaluation of the Spatial Point Process Model ..................................................... 21 Conclusions ........................................................................................................... 22 i Page Bibliography ................................................................................................................40 Appendix A: Sample Code for RUF Computation in Program R ............................... 53 Appendix B: Fitting the Spatial Point Process Model................................................. 54 Appendix C: Bark Arthropods as Food Abundance.................................................... 59 ii LIST OF FIGURES Figure Number Page 1. DEMO variable forest retention design................................................................... 24 2. Brown Creeper with color-taped retrices ............................................................... 25 3. Brown Creeper territories ........................................................................................ 26 4. Nesting success on DEMO treatments and surrounding forest ...............................27 5. Nesting success at 85m edge definition................................................................... 28 6. Nesting success at 100m edge definition................................................................. 29 iii LIST OF TABLES Table Number Page 1. Land cover class descriptions.................................................................................. 30 2. Definition of contrast-weighted edge ...................................................................... 30 3. Average estimated RUF for Brown Creepers at DEMO study area........................ 31 4 a-d. Standardized RUF coefficients..........................................................................33 5. RUF of birds nesting on and around the 40% aggregated treatments .....................36 6 a-c. Comparing RUF coefficients and nesting success ............................................ 37 7. Nesting success and territory productivity .............................................................. 39 iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank my source of funding, the USDA Forest Service, PNW Research Station, Forestry Sciences Laboratory, Olympia; especially Keith Aubry and Charlie Peterson for their continued support and faith in my study until completion. There were many people whose invaluable contributions aided in the success of my thesis project: The graduate students of the wildlife sciences lab, especially Dr. Robert Gitzen for his friendly, untiring, and invaluable analytical input; also Scott Horton, Nick Palazzotto, and Sara Gregory. Dr. Charlie Halpern, Shelly Evans, and Rick Abbott for support in various aspects of the DEMO study; my field research assistants Bobby J. Cummins, Michelle Wilcox, Renee Ripley, Zed Rulen, Abram Burkholder, Laura Palasz, Raphael Demers, and Brianna Gary for their aid in the collection of the field research data; Taro Narahashi for GIS support; Sally Lee for extensive assistance in high speed data entry and Korean tea. Special thanks to Dr. Juraj Halaj who conducted the entomological component of this study by looking at food availability for Brown Creepers in form of bark-arthropods. Deepest gratitude to my family in Germany for their continuous loving assistance during the long years of my education abroad; and to Sameh Farah for his faith in me, his understanding and boundless support. Last, I would like to acknowledge wholeheartedly my graduate committee David Manuwal, John Marzluff, and Mark Handcock, whose guidance and support enabled this valuable learning experience. I am indebted to my graduate advisor Dr. Manuwal, who provided untiring assistance for the many aspects and problems of this study and always lend a kind ear in times of troubles. Dr. Handcock, who spent countless hours with the development of the RUF computation code which is the analytical core of this study, his invaluable addition of statistical details to the methods section, and for technical support 24/7. Dr. Marzluff for his insightful guidance, constructive criticism and a revolutionary way of looking at wildlife sciences and research. v 1 Introduction Forest managers are challenged to develop and apply new silvicultural approaches to sustain biodiversity and productivity of forest ecosystems (Hansen 1995, Kohm and Franklin 1997, Lindenmayer and Franklin 2002). Ecologically sustainable management must integrate multiple and often conflicting objectives to meet the needs of all forest organisms and maintain ecological processes, many of which are not well understood (Kohm and Franklin 1997). Silvicultural techniques, such as retention of old trees, snags and logs, can help to introduce or maintain the structural complexity in managed forests that is inherent to natural forests. Variable density retention methods are increasingly being adopted by ecological silviculturists (Kohm and Franklin 1997). This type of retention can make substantial contributions to biodiversity conservation by the preservation of characteristics of latesuccessional forests (Aubry et al. 1999, Halpern et al. 1999). Effective implementation of this goal requires a thorough understanding of how changes in forest structure affect ecological processes and community development. The U.S. Forest Service-sponsored Demonstration of Ecosystem Management Options (DEMO) study is an attempt to provide the needed empirical evidence to evaluate consequences of green-tree retention in the Douglas-fir region of the Pacific Northwest United States (Aubry et al. 1999, Halpern et al. 1999). From an ecological standpoint the DEMO design provides a unique platform to study species and habitat interactions at scales relevant to forest management. Previous studies on experimental silvicultural techniques in Oregon (Hagar et al. 1996, Chambers et al. 1999) and Canada (Beese and Bryant 1999, Lance and Phinney 2000, Schieck et al. 2000, Tittler et al. 2001) have reported positive effects on bird community composition and abundance with increasing retention of forest legacy components. However, recent reviews of forest fragmentation and thinning point out that most studies merely recorded changes in bird abundance but failed to investigate 2 underlying ecological mechanisms (Sallabanks et al. 2000, McGarigal and Cushman 2002). The understanding of biological processes that determine avian population viability can provide insight on the effects of various silvicultural treatments and help to make inferences on forest management strategies (Marzluff et al. 2000). The Brown Creeper (Certhia americana) is a small, forest-dwelling songbird that obtains its diet of bark arthropods by foraging primarily on the boles of mature coniferous trees in the western United States (Mariani and Manuwal 1990, Weikel and Hayes 1999, Hejl et al. 2002). The Brown Creeper occurs most commonly in mature and old-growth forests with a continuous canopy cover and trees of large size classes (Carey et al. 1991, Gilbert and Allwine 1991, Lundquist and Mariani 1991, Hansen et al. 1995). They are sensitive to habitat fragmentation, forest thinning, and the creation of forest edges (Rosenber and Raphael 1986, Hobson and Bayne 2000a, b, Hutto 2000, Tittler et al. 2001, Anderson and Crompton 2002). A close relative to the Brown Creeper, the Eurasian Treecreeper (Certhia familiaris) in Finland, is considered an umbrella species for old-growth forest ecosystems and an indicator species for the effects of forest management activities (Kuitunen and Mäkinen 1993, Virkkala et al. 1994, Huhta et al. 2004, Suorsa et al. 2005). The availability of adequate nest sites in the form of snags might constitute a limiting factor to the abundance of a cavity nesting bird such as the Brown Creeper (Cline et al. 1980, Mannan et al. 1980, Raphael and White 1984). The snags used by cavity-nesting birds are often located in the center of dense tree clumps, surrounded by large diameter trees and snags (Saab and Dudley 1988). Creepers generally create their nest cavity under slabs of peeling or sloughing bark in large diameter (>50cm) snags and trees showing advanced decay or heart rot (Carey et al. 1991, Hejl et al. 2002). Trees with these characteristics occur in older forests. Forest management may affect creepers directly by reducing tree density and removing snags (Mannan et al. 1980), or indirectly by creating edge and reducing interior forest conditions (Hobson and Bayne 2000a, Brand and George 2000, 2001). Food availability, such as bark arthropods, 3 might change due to the effects of forest harvest activities on microclimatic regimes (Huhta 1971, Hoekstra et al. 1995, Moore et al. 2002). Furthermore, songbird nests located close to forest edges are more likely subject to nest predation in some settings (Sandström 1991, Marzluff and Restani 1999, Brand and George 2000). Bird nests in forests are under predation pressure from a large and diverse community of predators (Walters and Miller 2001, Bradley and Marzluff 2003). The causal ecological mechanisms that underlie the Brown Creeper’s habitat needs have not been well-studied (Hejl et al. 2002). Various habitat factors can influence bird abundance such as suitable nest sites, presence of predators, food abundance and the quality of foraging sites (Marzluff et al. 2000). The experimental green-tree retention treatments of the DEMO study can affect these factors to different degrees (Lehmkuhl et al. 1999). Specific habitat resources can be variously important to the animal and can constitute an ultimate threshold that determines the presence or absence of a bird species (Kernohan et al. 2001, Marzluff et al. 2001). A DEMO postharvest assessment of forest bird communities indicated a significant decline of Brown Creeper densities with increased forest harvest compared to the control units (Leu et al., in preparation). Consequently, the Brown Creeper is a good indicator species for evaluating the response to various forest harvest treatments of the DEMO study. Analyzing the variable use of resources that a bird utilizes in its home range can provide insights into the functional weight of specific habitat attributes. This study is part of a larger investigation of the trophic interactions between bark-gleaning birds, arthropods and vegetation conditions within the context of the DEMO research. A separate study investigates the effects of green-tree retention on the abundance and guild composition of corticolous arthropods as potential food resource for bark-gleaning birds (Halaj, in preparation). My study concerns the effects of variable retention harvest on avian resource utilization of the retained green-trees and snags within the DEMO experimental research units. Specifically, the objectives of my study are to: (1) explain the application of a new computational device to analyze 4 spatial point patterns in the light of animal resource use, and (2) relate this new method to the use of stand-level resources by the Brown Creeper. Methods Study Area and Design The Demonstration of Ecosystem Management Options (DEMO) study consisted of a randomized block design with six forest harvest treatments that were replicated in six geographic locations (blocks), four in southwestern Washington and two in southwestern Oregon. My study utilized the experimental blocks Watson Falls and Dog Prairie located in the Umpqua National Forest in the Cascade Range of southwestern Oregon. Watson Falls lies within the western hemlock (Tsuga heterophylla) forest zone, Dog Prairie within the white fir (Abis concolor) zone (Franklin and Dyrness 1988). The Douglas-fir (Pseudotsuga menziesii) dominated forest units were all beyond the stem exclusion phase, ranging in age from 110-165 years with a disturbance history of thinning at Dog Prairie and salvage logging at Watson Falls (Halpern et al. 1999). The study blocks were situated in a landscape matrix of mature and old-growth upland forest interspersed with variable sized patches of harvested forest of diverse management history. Each of my two study areas had six experimental harvest retention treatments which differed in the level and spatial dispersion of retained trees (Aubry et al. 1999, Halpern et al. 1999): 100% retention, 75% aggregated retention, 40% and 15% dispersed retention, and 40% and 15% aggregated retention units (Figure 1). The 40% and 15% aggregated retention treatments contained one hectare-sized, circular forest retention patches (five and two aggregated patches, respectively). The 75% retention treatment was not utilized in this study. In all harvested areas 6.5 snags per ha were created by girdling existing dominant or co-dominant green trees. Each treatment unit was 13 ha in size and had a permanent grid installed with 40 m spacing (for more 5 detailed information on the DEMO experiment please see the special issue of Northwest Science, Volume 73, 1999). Field Methods Brown Creeper Monitoring. – During the breeding season of 2003 and 2004 (May through July), I captured and color-banded 42 male Brown Creepers that established a territory within or in the vicinity of the five DEMO treatment units at the Watson Falls and Dog Prairie blocks. Male Brown Creepers were caught with mist-nets by attracting them with territorial call-backs and bird decoys. Each bird was banded with a US Fish and Wildlife Service aluminum leg band and with two colored leg bands. To improve individual bird identification in the field the creeper’s two central retrices were also color-marked using duct tape in the same color as the leg band combination (Figure 2). Determination of age, sex, weight, body fat and plumage condition followed Pyle (1997). Throughout the breeding season all Brown Creepers were spot-mapped using the existing grid system to determine the location and size of their territories at the DEMO treatment units (Bibby et al. 1992, Ralph et al. 1993). In addition, I also used GPS units (Garmin Etrex Venture) to map Brown Creeper movements beyond the DEMO experimental units. This method ensured that the entire Brown Creeper territory was mapped. The GPS locations were accurate within 5-12 meters, points whose recorded precision was >12 meters were deemed inaccurate and not recorded. Each of the treatment units was visited every 2-4 days for a total of about 23 visits per season starting shortly before sunrise until 12.00 – 14.00 h PST. Special care was taken that all birds were sampled equally in terms of early and late morning sessions throughout the season to prevent a sampling bias of relocation points for specific times of the day (Garton et al. 2001). I did not collect data in the afternoon or evening when bird activity was minimal. To avoid detection bias in treatments with the greatest visibility (little forest retention, aggregate edges, nest trees, and favored perches), I placed special 6 emphasis on sampling areas more frequently that were away from edges and had higher tree density. I specifically covered the forest that surrounded the treatment units to make sure I recorded any habitat use outside the treatment units. The Brown Creeper is a highly mobile species that can visit any part of its relatively small territory (2.3-6.4 ha, Hejl et al. 2002, and 12 ha, Suorsa et al. 2005) within a matter of seconds. Its foraging behavior is systematic (Davis 1978, Weikel and Hayes 1999, Hejl et al. 2002): the bird lands at the lower section of the tree bole and works its way upward foraging on bark arthropods. Once the tree crown area is reached the creeper flies off and starts foraging at the bottom of another tree trunk located often within only a few meters of the previous tree. Given these biological traits, I found that the birds would be most adequately sampled by recording every observed location of the marked males and their mates regardless of the time between relocations (Kernohan et al. 2001). Obtaining a representative sample for an accurate record of animal resource use is more important than to worry about statistical independence of relocation data (Otis and White 1999, Millspaugh and Marzluff 2001). However, care was taken that a tree location was only recorded once per sampling visit as Brown Creepers sometimes utilized the same trees repeatedly for foraging, especially trees in the vicinity of the nest. I never recorded any activity on the actual nest tree. To represent resource selection of a single or paired Brown Creeper during the breeding season, I recorded the following observations: foraging, singing, food collection for feeding the mate or nestlings, and the gathering of nesting material. Since females were unbanded and the Brown Creeper is not sexually dimorphic, I only recorded data when the identity of the bird was assured by other clues (presence of marked male, use of nest tree etc.). Field data were collected up to two weeks post-fledging as the family groups stay within the territory for early fledgling care. The territories only break down late in the fledgling period (Davis 1978). Nest Monitoring. – I conducted nest searches within the DEMO treatment units and the surrounding forest for Brown Creeper nests. The nesting progress was checked 7 every 4-7 days. To determine the breeding status and reproductive success, I calculated the Vickery Index for every territory that was established in the study area, including territories without nests and unpaired males (n=55; Vickery et al. 1992). All Brown Creeper nest locations were recorded using GPS units in the field. The nearest distance of the nest to edge was measured using ArcView GIS 3.2a software. Edge was defined as the boundary between different types of forest cover i.e. between the surrounding forest and a harvested treatment unit, or between the retained circular forest patches and the adjacent cleared areas within the aggregated retention units. Roads were not considered as edge-creating habitat and their presence was ignored (Ortega and Capen 2002). The roads leading to the treatment units were small and unimproved and usually ran parallel to a treatment boundary. Quantifying DEMO Resources Brown Creeper Territories. – All relocations were merged for each bird territory and geo-referenced using ArcView (Figure 3). I only constructed territories for Brown Creepers that made at least one nesting attempt (n=42); birds that never nested or their nest was not found were not considered (n=12). At least 30, but preferably more than 50 observations are recommended for an accurate construction of fixed kernel home range estimates (Seaman et al. 1999, Kernohan et al. 2001). A minimum of 30 locations explained >75% of space use for Steller’s jays home ranges during the breeding season on the Olympic Peninsula, Washington (Marzluff et al. 2004). I created all Brown Creeper home ranges using at least 50 observations, the average sample size was 118 ± 41 points. Landcover. – I quantified forest vegetation of the DEMO study area by creating a classified land cover grid in ArcView. I used a base map of the DEMO harvest units obtained from the US Forest Service, Diamond Lake Ranger District. The cover classes represent the different harvest levels within the DEMO treatments and the 100% cover of the forest surrounding the experimental units (see Table 1 for a description of the 8 cover classes). I chose a land cover grid resolution of 8 meters as a compromise between correctly addressing the grain of resources selection (Wiens 1989) by the Brown Creeper without adding excessive computing burden by constructing a more detailed grid. As a preparation for the computation in program R (R Development Core Team 2005) I generated sampling points with cover class values attached using the ArcView extension FOCAL PATCH (Marzluff et al. 2004). Contrast weighted Edge. – The edge contrast between adjacent land cover classes can differ depending on the forest classes of these neighboring patches. The degree of the edge contrast can influence various effects on organisms and the environment (McGarigal and Marks 1995). I specified a contrast-weighted edge to reflect a potential sensitivity of the Brown Creeper to increased forest harvest among the DEMO treatments (Table 2). The amount of edge contrast between adjacent forest patches of the DEMO study area can be assigned by defining weights between 0 (no contrast) and 1 (maximum contrast). Utilizing the moving window function of the program FRAGSTATS 3.3 I attached contrast-weighted edge values to my 8 m land cover grid (McGarigal et al. 2002). The sampling points for the edge grid were generated once more with extension FOCAL PATCH in ArcView. The contrastweighted edge file was further processed in program R where the covariate was normalized to a maximum value of 1.0 for each bird. Distance to Nest. – I included the covariate distance to nest to examine the potential influence of nest location on the habitat use of Brown Creepers within their territory. The covariate was created in program R as the Euclidian distance from the nest in meters for each breeding pair. 9 Linking Bird Locations to Stand-level Resources Spatial Point Process Model. – A new method relating habitat resources to animal space use, the Resource Utilization Function (RUF), uses the animal’s relocation points and a fixed kernel home range estimator to relate a probabilistic measure of use to habitat resources (Marzluff et al. 2004). The coefficients of the regression function estimate the degree of resource use by the animal within its home range. In this thesis I employ the same conceptual framework, but use an alternative statistical measure of the RUF. To link my observed bird locations to the various resources at the DEMO study area, I modeled them as a spatial point process (Diggle 2003). These models are fitted using the spatial point process package SPATSTAT that operates in the statistical computation program R (Baddeley and Turner 2005). The models that I computed for Brown Creeper relocation data incorporate spatial trends, spatial resource covariates and the interaction between the location points (see Appendix A for the computation code used in R and Appendix B for a graphic output of the fitted model). The process describes both spatial inhomogeneity (‘trend’) and the stochastic dependence between points such as spatial clustering (‘interaction’; Baddeley and Turner 2000). The argument trend can specify the dependence on spatial covariates such as landcover classes or contrast-weighted edge. The conditional intensity is a function of both, the spatial trend (and/or spatial covariate effects) and the dependence between points (Baddeley and Turner 2005). Explicitly I model the n observed locations x = ( x1 , …, xn ) as a realization from a utilization distribution (UD). The UD is modeled as a spatial point process with probability density fUD ( x;θ ) with respect to Poisson process of rate 1. A Poisson process of rate 1 represents uniform utilization at the rate of 1 utilization per square meter. The parameter θ = ( β , γ ) represents the coefficients of the covariates ( β ) and the interaction parameters ( γ ). The model for fUD ( x;θ ) is expressed through the (Papangelou) conditional intensity of utilization at location u : 10 λUD (u; x, θ ) = fUD ( x1 , …, xn , u;θ ) fUD ( x;θ ) which is interpreted as the conditional probability of utilization at u given the rest of the bird’s utilization. For homogeneous and inhomogeneous Poisson processes the conditional intensity and the intensity function coincide. In this paper I model the conditional intensity by: λUD (u; x,θ ) = e β T Z (u ) γ S (u;x ) where Z (u ) is the q-vector of covariates for the location u and S (u; x ) is the spatial interaction function. The parameter coefficient ( β k ) of the k th covariate is interpreted in a way similar to logistic regression. Consider the probabilities of utilization at a location u under two different resource utilization functions fUD ( x;θ ) . The two functions are identical, except the second function has the value of the k th covariate one unit higher than in the first at location u . Then the ratio of the probabilities of utilization at location u is e βk . This holds the utilization at points other than u fixed. In simple sense, e βk is the effect of a unit change in the covariate holding the other covariates and utilization fixed. The package SPATSTAT utilizes a maximum pseudolikelihood method (MPLE) for model fitting instead of the maximum likelihood estimator, which allows for a rapid fitting of a large variety of models (see Geyer 1999 in defense of MPLE, and Baddeley and Turner, 2000, 2005). It is possible to compute maximum likelihood estimates using a Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) algorithm, but this was not regarded as being worth the additional computation burden. MCMC methods do not allow the fast and flexible fitting of a wide range of models such as the MPLE can achieve. The spatial point interaction is modeled as a Geyer saturation process. Under this model the total contribution to the potential from each point (from its pairwise 11 interaction with all other points) is limited to a user-defined maximum value s (the saturation threshold) within a spatial radius r of the point (Geyer 1999). Explicitly, n S (u; x ) = ∑ min(s, #{i :| u − xi |< r}) i =1 The threshold and radius parameters define the shape of the point interaction in the same way the kernel function defines the shape of the interaction of the model in Marzluff et al. (2004). They are specified by the user. Specifically, for the models in this thesis I use a range of 35m and saturation threshold of 5. With both radius and threshold fixed the Geyer process is part of the exponential family of distributions. In contrast to linear and nonlinear regression models, models of the exponential family have the advantage that they do not need to satisfy the assumption of the response variable following the normal distribution (Myers et al. 2002). The limiting effect of the saturation threshold in the clustering region of the spatial point pattern caps the probability density of the model (Geyer 1999). Areas of intense use will exhibit strong clustering of points which can in part be due to autocorrelated data. Hence, the conservative approach of Geyer’s saturation threshold in areas of high point density ensures model robustness to spatial autocorrelation. The user-defined interaction radius r acts like a moving window that examines the animal’s point locations in correlation to the underlying habitat resources (covariates) and in relationship to all other points of the bird’s point pattern. In rapid succession this window will make each bird’s location the center cell of its focal point and examine the pairwise point interactions between the focal and the nearest neighbor points within the radius of the window. Thus the Geyer model relates the probability density of used space to the associated resource covariates in a spatially explicit way. This interaction model serves the same purpose as the spatial covariance model used by Marzluff et al. (2004). The interaction model has a further parameter γ which reflects the strength of the interaction. It serves the same role as the bandwidth in the kernel method. If γ = 1 the model is a Poisson spatial process with the number of observed locations in each 12 spatial area following a Poisson distribution, and independence of resource utilization between disjoint spatial areas. If γ > 1 the resource utilization is correlated between spatial areas that are close, leading to a clustering of resource utilization above and beyond that implied by the covariates. If γ < 1 the resource utilization is also correlated between spatial areas that are close, but in this case the resource utilization follows a more regular pattern. This spatial interaction parameter is estimated jointly with the coefficients of the covariates. The advantage of the spatial point process approach is that it jointly estimates the coefficients of the covariates and the spatial interaction of the relocations, and a separate model fit is not required. The model output of the Geyer saturation process is the estimated intensity function that depicts resource use similar to the concept of the RUF (Marzluff et al. 2004). The RUF quantifies differential resource use within the animal’s home range by relating a probabilistic measure of use, the utilization distribution, to habitat resources. The home range is defined by a 99% fixed kernel estimator. The Geyer saturation process does not utilize the kernel method but estimates the home range with the fitted Geyer intensity function of the bird’s relocation points. Note that this range will contain 100% of the observed locations, and hence 100% of the observed resource utilization and homerange. Hypothetically, further or more intense observation of the bird would have lead to further locations and a probable extension of the observed homerange. The model adapts to this by estimating the theoretical home range. The moving window analysis of the spatial point process is confined within the boundaries of this estimated home range. This is analogous to Manly and McDonald’s Type III design (Manly et al. 2002) where the use of resources is measured separately for each animal within its home range and therefore defines the individual bird as the sampling unit. Describing Stand-level Resource Use. – The degree of dependence on spatial covariates is indicated by the coefficients of the fitted RUF. The magnitude of the coefficient indicates the importance of the covariate for resource utilization. The 13 positive and negative prefix of the coefficient depicts if use will increase or decrease with a change in the quantity of the resource attribute. Since each bird was regarded as an independent sampling unit I was able to generate an average RUF for all nesting pairs of Brown Creepers at the DEMO study area. I also created average RUFs for subgroups of birds that had a comparable choice of resource attributes in their home ∧ ranges (Table 3). I pooled and averaged the unstandardized coefficients ( β *) and estimated the variance for each individual bird’s resource use following the methods outlined in Marzluff et al. (2004). Bootstrapping simulations computed the standard deviation for each covariate (SDcovariate), the standard error (SEcovariate), and the overall RUF standard deviation (SDRUF) for each bird’s RUF estimate. Using SDcovariate and SDRUF I standardized the estimated resource coefficients for a more equal comparison of relative importance of resource attributes. I tested the null hypotheses that the average standardized ∧ coefficients ( β ) per bird group is not different from 0 using t-tests at the 0.05 alpha level including the sample variance of the mean (Zar 1999, Marzluff et al. 2004). The ∧ significance of the β –value and the proportion of Brown Creepers whose use was either positively or negatively correlated with the resource coefficients indicate the consistency in resource selection at the population level (Table 4a-e). I constructed two average RUFs for Brown Creepers that nested on the 40% aggregated treatment units (n=3) vs. birds that chose to nest in the forest surrounding these treatments (n=9; Table 5). I only created average RUFs comparing nest locations between treatment and surrounding forest for the bird group utilizing the area of the 40% aggregated treatments, because overall only few birds chose to nest on the DEMO treatment units (n=6). I tested if Brown Creepers showed a preference for nest location either on a DEMO treatment unit or in the forest surrounding the treatment (birds nesting on the control units were excluded). I used the Fisher’s Exact test (one-tailed; Zar 1999) for 14 count data to assess the null hypothesis that nests placed in the forest surrounding any treatment unit were more successful (Figure 4). I was also interested if edge was a factor in nest placement, where edge was characterized as the border area within a forest patch with continuous canopy cover (can be an aggregated forest patch or surrounding forest) adjacent to an area with reduced cover due to harvest treatments. The edge effect on nest placement was defined at two distances based on a priori knowledge: <85 m (Brand and George 2001) and <100m (Kuitunen and Mäkinen 1993). Using a 1-sample proportions test with continuity correction at the 0.05 alpha level (Zar 1999) I tested the null hypothesis if the proportion of nest site selection is equal for nesting (a) on vs. off treatment units and (b) at edge vs. away from edge. In addition, I used the Fisher’s Exact test (one-tailed) to assess the null hypothesis that nests placed away from edge had a higher proportion of fledging success (Figure 5 and 6). I standardized RUF coefficients for groups of nesting Brown Creepers that either placed their nest within <85m of edge vs. nested >85m away from edge (Brand and George 2001). The birds are further grouped by successful and unsuccessful nesters and their mean coefficients tested for difference using a 2-sample t-test at α=0.05 (Table 6a-c; Zar 1999). I did not create average RUFs for birds nesting >100 from edge (Kuitunen and Mäkinen 1993), because the sample size would have been too small (n=2 for successful and n=1 for unsuccessful nesters). Results Use of Stand-level Resource Attributes A combined total of 54 male Brown Creepers set up their territories in the DEMO study area in the breeding seasons of 2003 (n=25) and 2004 (n=29). For this paper I restricted the spatial point process analysis to territories of paired Brown Creepers who attempted to nest at least once per season (n=42). The average estimated 15 RUF coefficients show that all birds avoided the use of the clearcut area compared to the use of an equal area in the surrounding forest; this is true at the 40% as well as at ∧ ∧ the 15% aggregated treatments ( β *= -2.42 and β *= -0.85, respectively; Table 3). Ttests of the estimated standardized RUF coefficients confirm a highly significantly negative association with clearcut area (p<<0.001 for all groups; Table 4a-c). All ∧ Brown Creeper are positively associated with the use of edge habitat ( β *= 0.28), but not significantly so (p=0.24). However, use of edge is significant for birds that placed their territories around the 40% aggregated retention treatments (p=0.004). The average RUF for all Brown Creepers shows that 40% and 15% retained forest patches are used ∧ less then an equal area in the surrounding forest far away from edge ( β *= -0.74). Conversely, the standardized coefficient for birds at the 40% aggregated unit indicate significantly positive use of the 40% forest patches (p=0.004). Use of 15% forest patches is negative though (p=0.91). A similar pattern of habitat use is indicated by the unstandardized vs. standardized coefficients of the thinned DEMO treatments. The average unstandardized coefficient for all birds shows less use of thinned DEMO treatments than the use of an equal area in the surrounding forest far away from edge ∧ ( β *= -0.25). Specifically, the standardized coefficient indicates a negative association with the 15% dispersed retention treatments, but not significantly so (p=0.10). Then again, use of the 40% dispersed retained forest is positive, but also not significantly (p=0.49). The DEMO control units were located adjacent to other retention treatments and thus there was some edge habitat present at parts of the unharvested units. However, every Brown Creeper who nested at the control units was neither positively nor ∧ negatively influenced in habitat use by this resource attribute ( β *= 0). All groups of birds show a weak clustering in the distribution of their spatial point pattern ∧ (Interaction: β *>0), indicating that resource utilization is correlated between spatial areas that are close. The distance to nest covariate is significantly important for all Brown Creepers (p<<0.001). With increasing distance from the nest the use of a resource in that area declines. 16 The Brown Creepers nesting in the forest surrounding the 40% aggregated units ∧ use the clearcut area less ( β *= -2.82) than the creepers nesting on the treatment (β*= 1.24; Table 5). Forest-nesting birds also use the aggregated patches less compared to an ∧ equal area of forest interior habitat ( β *= -0.94), whereas this relationship is reversed ∧ for birds nesting on the treatment ( β *= 0.86). In addition, treatment-nesting creepers ∧ ∧ use edge habitat more ( β *= 1.26) than forest nesting birds ( β *= 0.86). There was no significant difference between any of the pooled standardized RUF coefficients when comparing the two groups of all Brown Creepers that placed their nests either within <85m or >85m away from edge (Table 6c). Neither were there any significant differences between the coefficients for the four subgroups of successful vs. unsuccessful nesting birds (Table 6a-b). Although the comparison was not significant, birds that nested successfully within <85m from edge were negatively ∧ associated with the use of aggregated forest patches ( β = -0.04) and used edge habitat ∧ less ( β =0.01) vs. unsuccessful nesters showed a positive association with patchy ∧ ∧ habitat ( β =0.12) and used edge more ( β =0.08). A similar pattern can be observed when comparing the two groups of birds nesting >85m from edge: the successful ∧ ∧ nesting creepers did not use patchy habitat ( β =0) and used edge less ( β =0.07), but the ∧ unsuccessful birds had a positive coefficient for use of forest patches ( β =0.17) and ∧ edges ( β =0.17). Nest Placement and Success I located a total of 48 Brown Creeper nests in the DEMO study area for the combined breeding seasons of 2003 and 2004. This total also includes second nesting attempts of 6 Brown Creeper pairs. The average Vickery rank and nesting success for all nests and territories was much higher in the 2003 season than in 2004 (Table 7). The Vickery rank for all territories at my DEMO study area was 3.9 ± 1.4, the average nesting success was 54%. Six out of 48 nests were located at a treatment unit (4 at 40% Aggregated, 1 at 40% Dispersed and 1 and 15% Aggregated); all other nests were 17 placed in the forest surrounding the treatments. Brown Creepers chose to nest outside of treatments with high significance (p<<0.001, 1-sample proportions test). Nests placed in the surrounding forest were not more successful than nests on treatment units (p=0.36, Fisher’s Exact test; Figure 4). When edge was defined within <85m of the forest patch boundary 33 nests out of 48 were placed near the edge area (of which 18 fledged successfully, 15 unsuccessfully), and 15 nests were located away from edge (of which 8 were successful, 7 unsuccessful; Figure 5). When edge was defined within <100m then 43 out of 48 nests were found at edge (21 successful, 22 unsuccessful), and only 5 nests were found in forest away from edge (5 successful, 0 unsuccessful; Figure 6). At <85m edge definition significantly more Brown Creepers chose to nest in the edge area than in the forest (p=0.01, 1-sample proportions test). At <100m edge definition this result is highly significant (p<<0.001). The Fisher’s Exact test indicates that at an edge definition of <85m, nests placed away from edge did not have a significantly higher nesting success than nests placed at edges (p=0.7). However, at the <100m edge definition nests away from edge were significantly more successful (p=0.04). Discussion Implications for Use of Harvested Forest Patches The fitted Geyer intensity function estimates the use of stand-level resources by individual birds. All Brown Creepers showed a higher use of the 40% aggregated forest patches and the edge habitat created by the various forest retention harvests, than the use of the surrounding forest away from edge. This observation is contrary to the classification of the Brown Creeper as a forest interior specialist that avoids edge and is sensitive to habitat fragmentation (Hobson and Bayne 2000a, b, Hutto 2000, Tittler et al. 2001, Anderson and Crompton 2002). However, literature review shows inconsistent responses of Brown Creepers to forest fragmentation in North America (Hejl et al. 18 2002), and also for the Eurasian Treecreeper in Northern Europe (Kuitunen and Helle 1988). Observations of edge avoidance might not be due to a sensitivity to edge rather than a response to differences in habitat types, as edge often consists of different vegetation than forest interior habitat. The preferential use of small forest fragments and edges at the DEMO sites might be due to a higher abundance of bark arthropods as potential food source. Changes in food abundance can affect bird productivity and survival (Hutto 1990, Smith and Rotenberry 1990, Pettersson et al. 1995, Suorsa et al. 2004, Virkkala 2004). A concurrent study at the DEMO sites estimated that there were significantly more bark arthropods at the forest harvest treatments than at the control units, with significantly higher abundance at the 15% retention units than the 40% retention (Halaj, in preparation; also see Appendix C). Forest harvest creates changes in the microclimate and understory vegetation that is often favorable to arthropod abundance (Shure and Phillips 1990, Chen et al. 1993, Bedford and Asher 1994, Peltonen et al. 1997, Heithecker and Halpern, in press). Thus habitat fragmentation due to small scale forest retention harvests, such as the 40% aggregated DEMO treatments, might provide attractive habitat to Brown Creepers due to increased food availability. However, the retention patches must consist primarily of mature, medium to large diameter trees, as these are the preferred foraging substrate of Brown Creepers, and a high density of large snags as nesting substrate (Mariani and Manuwal 1990, Carey et al. 1991, Weikel and Hayes 1999, Hejl et al. 2002). Old-growth and mature trees offer a larger amount of arthropods than smaller trees. In contrast to the positive association with the 40% aggregated and dispersed DEMO resources, breeding pairs of Brown Creepers are negatively associated with the 15% aggregated and dispersed treatments. In addition, all Brown Creepers significantly avoid the clearcut areas of the 40% and 15% aggregated retention units. The near absence of creepers in the clearcut areas is not a surprise as the bark-foraging birds are tree-obligates. The 15% retention units might constitute a threshold at which thinned 19 and fragmented habitat begins to loose its attractiveness to the Brown Creepers despite elevated levels of food abundance. A stand with low tree density can increase traveltime between food patches (tree boles) and thus negatively affect foraging energy expenditures (Perrins and Birkhead 1984, Franzreb 1985, Stephens and Krebs 1986). Brown Creepers and Treecreepers have been found to be either absent or in modest densities at low forest retentions and clearcuts (Hansen et al. 1995, Hutto and Young 1999, Tittler et al. 2001, Anderson and Crompton 2002, Suorsa 2005). In Finland, Treecreepers were found not to be much affected by changes in habitat configuration (more patchiness, more edge), but rather by habitat loss itself (loss of old-growth forests, loss of large diameter trees; Suorsa 2005). This observation might explain the mixed response of Brown Creepers in various fragmentation studies where the effects of habitat fragmentation and habitat loss might have been confounded. Implications for Nesting at Edges The Brown Creepers at the DEMO study area relied heavily on the forest surrounding the treatment units for nest placement (42 out of 48 nests). Although the retention treatments might be attractive for foraging, they are poorly suited for nesting. For all birds, the use of DEMO resource attributes declined sharply with distance from nest. The activity of breeding Brown Creepers appears to concentrate in the area close to the nest location (Davis 1978, Kuitunen and Mäkinen 1993, Hejl et al. 2002). To optimize foraging energetics, central place foraging theory suggests the use of all trees in minimum possible distance in a circular area around the nest (Stephens and Krebs 1986, Kuitunen and Mäkinen 1993). As Brown Creepers prefer mature trees for foraging and large diameter snags for nesting, naturally they preferentially nested in the continuous, mature forests surrounding the DEMO treatment units. However, a significant number of the birds chose to place their nests in the surrounding forest area close to the treatment edges within <85m and <100m, even though there were portions of the bird’s territory far away from edge (Figure 4). This is contrary to an estimated edge sensitivity of 85m for the relative density of Brown Creepers in coast redwood 20 forest patches in Northern California (Brand and George 2001). Experiments with nest boxes determined a distance of a 100m from edge as the first nest site choice by Treecreepers in boreal forest patches of southern Finland (Kuitunen and Mäkinen 1993). The increased food abundance in form of bark arthropods in edge habitat and thus better foraging energetics might explain this preference of nest locations close to forest edges (Shure and Phillips 1990, Kuitunen and Mäkinen 1993, Bedford and Asher 1994, Peltonen et al. 1997; Halaj, in preparation). Bird nests in forests are under predation pressure from a diverse community of predators and nests close to edge are more likely subject to predation (Sandström 1991, Brand and George 2000, Walters and Miller 2001, Bradley and Marzluff 2003). However, creeper nests I observed that were placed at the forest edge were not less successful than nests >85m from the edge. Nests placed >100m from the edge were significantly more successful, although the sample size was small (n=5). The overall nesting success for both field seasons was 54% (63% in 2003 and 48% in 2004). The lower rate of nesting success in 2004 was probably due to unusually cold and wet weather conditions in the spring compared to the previous year. Brown Creepers nest early in the season and are susceptible to cold and rainy weather conditions for survivorship and breeding success (Hejl et al. 2002, Huhta et al. 2004). The abundance and community composition of potential nest predators depends on the type and quality of the habitat and the influence of the surrounding landscape (Andren 1992, Tewksbury et al. 1998, Marzluff and Restani 1999, DeSanto and Willson 2001, Rodewald 2002, Huhta 2004). The degree of nest predation at habitat edges situated in a forested landscape, such as the DEMO study area, can be less than predation rates at edges in proximity to agriculture and urban settlements. Settings in the wildland-urban interface can provide habitat for a large and abundant predator community and thus contribute to higher predation pressure. In contrast, the average nesting success and territory productivity at my study area was very close to Brown Creepers nesting in the suburban landscape around Seattle, Washington (54% and 59%, 21 respectively, Table 7; Blewett and Marzluff 2005). In this fragmented habitat, Brown Creepers were found to breed successfully and had similar densities to populations in managed mature and old-growth forests situated in the wildlands of the Olympic Peninsula. However, the birds were most abundant at the suburban study sites that contained the highest percent of forest cover and largest patch sizes. Brown Creepers were not present at built portions of the sites which indicates their dependency on forested areas. Large areas of remnant continuous forest around the urban region might serve as a population source for the Brown Creepers in the fragmented, suburban landscape. Brown Creepers can breed successfully in the moderately fragmented landscape such as the DEMO study area. A matrix of continuous, mature forest with large trees and snags is important for nesting and foraging, as the harvest treatments are not suitable for nesting. The 40% aggregated retention treatment appears to be most attractive for foraging Brown Creepers and for the placement of their nests in the nearby forest around the harvested units. Evaluation of the Spatial Point Process Model The spatial point process model estimates resource use by fitting an intensity function directly to the animal’s relocation points. This model is an advancement of the Marzluff et al. (2004) approach as it can fit the RUF without separately deriving the animal’s utilization distribution. Another advantage is that the Geyer process automatically caps the probability density of the model in areas of highly clustered observations and thus exhibits robustness to spatial autocorrelation. As the Geyer process model is a member of the exponential family, the fitted data do not need to meet the normality assumption. The model performs well in estimating resource use within small territories such as the Brown Creeper’s. It is a good tool for describing the variable response of 22 individual birds to the micro-fragmentation induced by the DEMO harvest treatments. The statistical computation program R and the spatial point process package SPATSTAT are readily available and free of charge (Baddely and Turner 2005). The SPATSTAT package allows a fast and flexible way of fitting a variety of models for spatial data analysis. Thus spatial point processes are a valuable and economical aid for the evaluation and management of habitat resources. Simulations of the fitted Geyer intensity function indicate a good model fit (see Appendix B for a graphic example of simulations). However, there is a need for more vigorous tests that assess the performance and proper fit of the various spatial point process models. The construction of RUFs for Brown Creepers in the DEMO study area requires the incorporation of several resource covariates into the model. An extensive computing knowledge of program R is necessary to create a code that can handle the construction of complex animal-resource models utilizing the package SPATSTAT and other additional applications in R. The creation of such a code can be demanding on personnel resources. The availability of sample code applied to resource utilization studies can be of great help (see Appendix A for the code used in this study). Baddely and Turner (2005) provide an extensive manual about the application of the SPATSTAT package. A great advancement to the applicability of this spatial point process approach would be the development of a user-friendly GUI surface that allows a quick plug in of spatial data and the construction of resource covariates without the necessity of extensive computer programming. A good example is the program FRAGSTATS for the analysis of landscape metrics (McGarigal et al. 2002). Conclusions (1) Spatial point processes, specifically the Geyer saturation point process model, are an effective tool for the analysis of animal resource use in a spatially explicit way and are proficient in detecting differential use in small home ranges such as Brown Creeper territories. 23 (2) The 40% aggregated forest retention is attractive habitat for foraging Brown Creepers, although the birds depend on a landscape setting of continuous, mature forest to provide suitable nest sites. 24 100 % 75 % 40 % 15 % Aggregated Dispersed Figure 1: Design of the six variable forest retention treatments at the DEMO study area. The percent values indicate the level, and the black color the pattern of retained forest cover (adapted from Leu et al., in preparation). 25 Figure 2: Banded Brown Creeper with color-taped retrices (indicated by the arrow). 26 Figure 3: A total of three birds established territories at the 40% aggregated retention treatment at the Dog Prairie block during the season of 2003. Each color represents a different bird, the dots are the relocations, and the yellow square is the location of the nest. The green surface correspond to areas of 100% forest cover, the brown color indicates harvested sections. The five green circles represent the 40% retained, aggregated forest patches; each patch is about 100m in diameter. The three brown circles represent the harvested forest patches of the adjacent 75% retention treatment. 27 35 30 16 25 20 Unsuccessful Successful 15 10 5 0 17 4 2 Treatment Forest Figure 4: Successful and unsuccessful nests of birds that nested either on any of the DEMO treatment units (n=6) or nested in the forest surrounding the treatment (n=33). I tested the hypothesis that nesting success would be higher for nests placed in the surrounding forest (Fisher’s Exact test, p=0.36). 28 35 30 25 15 20 Unsuccessful Successful 15 10 18 5 7 8 0 Edge No Edge Figure 5: Successful and unsuccessful nests of birds that nested close to the edge (<85m) around the DEMO treatment units (n=33) and that nested in the forest interior, away from edge (>85m, n=15). I tested the hypothesis that nesting success would be higher for nests placed >85m away from edge (Fisher’s Exact test, p=0.56). 29 45 40 35 30 25 22 Unsuccessful Successful 20 15 10 5 21 0 5 Edge No Edge 0 Figure 6: Successful and unsuccessful nests of birds that nested close to the edge (<100m) around the DEMO treatment units (n=43) and that nested in the forest interior, away from edge (>100m, n=5). I tested the hypothesis that nesting success would be higher for nests placed >100m away from edge (Fisher’s Exact test, p=0.04). 30 Table 1. The land cover classes represent the different harvest levels within the DEMO treatments and the 100% cover of the forest surrounding the experimental units. The control units were considered as the same 100% cover class as the surrounding forest. Land Cover Classes 100% forest cover surrounding each treatment 40% forest cover of dispersed retention 15% forest cover of dispersed retention Five circular forest patches within the 40% aggregated retention unit Two circular forest patches within the 15% aggregated retention unit Clearcut area within 40% aggregated retention unit Clearcut area within 15% aggregated retention unit Table 2. Definition of contrast weights per interface of two different land cover classes. The range of weights is between 0 (no contrast) and 1 (maximum contrast; for calculations and definitions see McGarigal et al. 2002). The contrast-weighted edge was specified to reflect a potential sensitivity of the Brown Creeper to increased forest harvest among the DEMO treatments. Landcover Interface Contrast Weights Surrounding Forest to Clearcut Area at 15% Aggregated Retention 1.0 Surrounding Forest to Clearcut Area at 40% Aggregated Retention 0.8 Surrounding Forest to 15% Dispersed Retention 0.6 Surrounding Forest to 40% Dispersed Retention 0.4 40% Dispersed Retention to Clearcut Area at 15% Aggregated Retention 0.3 15% Dispersed Retention to Clearcut Area at 15% Aggregated Retention 0.2 Table 3. Average RUF for all nesting pairs of Brown Creepers and for subgroups of birds with comparable choices of forest ∧ resource attributes in their territory during the breeding season in the DEMO study area. The estimates of unstandardized RUF( β *) coefficients are listed with their standard error (SE) in parenthesis. The SE indicates uncertainty in the estimated resource use by each individual Brown Creeper. A positive coefficient indicates that use increases with increasing quantity of the resource. Group Estimates of RUF coefficients (SE) ∧ All Brown Creepers (n=42) ∧ ∧ β * CWED5 β * Distance Nest β * Interaction6 -6.02 (0.01) -1.75 (0.40) -0.74 (0.31) -0.25 (0.15) 0.28 (0.02) -1.11 (0.001) 0.1 (0.001) β * Forest (Intercept) β *Clearcut 40%A β * Patches 40%A n/a β * CWED β * Distance Nest β * Interaction -5.94 (0.04) -2.42 (0.93) -0.94 (0.14) - 0.96 (0.05) -1.19 (0.01) 0.06 (0.001) β * Patches 15%A n/a β * CWED -1.08 (1.43) - -0.15 (0.03) β * Forest (Intercept) -6.59 (0.03) ∧ ∧ β *Clearcut 15%A -0.85 (0.54) ∧ ∧ ∧ ∧ ∧ ∧ ∧ ∧ β * Distance Nest -0.70 (0.003) ∧ ∧ ∧ β * Interaction 0.15 (0.001) ∧ β * Forest (Intercept) n/a n/a β * Thinned 40%D β * CWED β * Distance Nest β * Interaction -6.35 (0.04) - - 0.05 (0.01) 0.12 (0.04) -1.08 (0.005) 0.16 (0.001) β * Forest (Intercept) n/a n/a β * Thinned 15%D β * CWED β * Distance Nest β * Interaction -5.81(0.04) - - -0.47 (0.43) 0.12 (0.15) -1.17 (0.01) 0.07 (0.001) ∧ ∧ ∧ ∧ ∧ 25 31 15% Dispersed (n=11) ∧ β * Thinned4 ∧ 40% Dispersed (n=8) ∧ β * Patches3 ∧ 15% Aggregated (n=9) ∧ β * Clearcut2 ∧ 40% Aggregated (n=12) ∧ β * Forest (Intercept)1 Table 3 (continued) ∧ 100% Control (n=6) ∧ ∧ ∧ β * Forest (Intercept) n/a n/a n/a β * CWED β * Distance Nest β * Interaction -5.14 (0.06) - - - 0 (0) -1.55 (0.01) 0.09 (0.06) 1 Forest = represents the forested area surrounding the treatment unit Clearcut = clearcut area at the 15% and 40% aggregated harvest treatments 3 Patches = retained forest patches at the 15% and 40% aggregated harvest treatments 4 Thinned = evenly retained trees at the 15% and 40% dispersed harvest treatments 5 CWED = Contrast-weighted edge 6 Interaction = indicates if the spatial point pattern displays clustering in its distribution 2 32 33 ∧ Table 4a-e. Estimates of standardized RUF coefficients ( β ) for all nesting pairs of Brown Creepers and for four subgroups of birds with comparable choices of forest resource attributes in their territories. The null hypothesis tests that the average β=0 per group of birds at α=0.05% (p-values in bold font indicate significance). The magnitude of the β-value shows the relative importance of the resource. Consistency in resource selection at population level is indicated by significance of β and the proportion of birds whose use was either positively or negatively correlated with the resource. (a) All Nesting Brown Creepers at the DEMO Study Area. All Resources at the DEMO Study Area Surrounding Forest 1 1 Mean Standardized ∧ β 95% Confidence Interval P-value Ho: β =0 Number of birds associated with resource (n=10) + - -2.77 (-3.28) - (-2.40) <<0.001 0 10 Clearcut Area2 -0.40 (-0.52) - (-0.29) <<0.001 0 20 Retained Patches3 0.04 (-0.09) - (0.17) 0.53 12 7 Thinning4 -0.10 (-0.28) - (0.07) 0.23 7 12 CWED5 0.07 (-0.05) - (0.19) 0.24 23 16 Distance to Nest -0.78 (-0.95) - (-0.64) <<0.001 2 37 Surrounding Forest = represents the forested area surrounding the treatment unit Clearcut Area = clearcut area at the 15% and 40% aggregated harvest treatments 3 Retained Patches = retained forest patches at the 15% and 40% aggregated harvest treatments 4 Thinning = evenly retained trees at the 15% and 40% dispersed harvest treatments 5 CWED = Contrast-weighted edge 2 34 Table 4 (continued) (b) Brown Creeper Group at the 40% Aggregated Retention Treatment. DEMO Resources at Treatment 40% Aggregated Mean Standardized ∧ β 95% Confidence Interval Number of birds P-value associated with Ho: β =0 resource (n=10) + - Surrounding Forest -2.24 (-2.81) - (-1.68) <<0.001 0 12 Clearcut Area -0.64 (-0.65) - (-0.27) <<0.001 1 11 40% A. Patch 0.08 (0.24) - (0.42) <<0.001 8 4 CWED 0.22 (0.09) - (0.36) 0.004 10 2 Distance to Nest -0.63 (-0.91) - (-0.35) <<0.001 1 11 (c) Brown Creeper Group at the 15% Aggregated Retention Treatment. DEMO Resources at Treatment 15% Aggregated Mean Standardized ∧ β 95% Confidence Interval Number of birds P-value associated with Ho: β =0 resource (n=8) + - Surrounding Forest -3.14 (-3.93) – (-2.34) <<0.001 0 9 Clearcut Area -0.33 (-0.46) – (-0.19) <<0.001 1 8 15% A. Patch -0.01 (-0.26) – (0.24) 0.91 6 3 CWED -0.01 (-0.20) – (0.18) 0.90 4 5 Distance to Nest -0.50 (-0.72) – (-0.29) <<0.001 1 8 35 Table 4 (continued) (d) Brown Creeper Group at the 40% Dispersed Retention Treatment. Mean Standardized Number of birds P-value associated with Ho: β =0 resource (n=8) + - β 95% Confidence Interval Surrounding Forest -2.58 (-3.76) – (-1.40) 0.001 0 8 40% D. Retention 0.07 (-0.17) – (0.31) 0.49 5 3 CWED 0.05 (-0.11) – (0.21) 0.45 4 4 Distance to Nest -0.68 (-1.00) – (-0.36) 0.002 1 7 DEMO Resources at Treatment 40% Dispersed ∧ (e) Brown Creeper Group at the 15% Dispersed Retention Treatment. Mean DEMO Resources Standardized at Treatment ∧ 15% Dispersed β 95% Confidence Interval P-value Ho: β =0 Number of birds associated with resource (n=9) + - Surrounding Forest -3.18 (-4.39) – (-1.96) <<0.001 0 11 15% D. Retention -0.18 (-0.44) – (0.05) 0.10 2 9 CWED 0.05 (-0.14) – (0.24) 0.57 6 5 Distance to Nest -0.74 (-0.87) – (-0.60) <<0.001 0 11 Table 5. Average estimated RUF for breeding pairs of Brown Creeper that nested in the area of 40% aggregated treatment (n=12). Nine birds nested in the forest surrounding the treatment and three birds nested on the treatment. For information on interpretation of coefficients and SE please see caption of Table 1. Birds with comparable resource choice Estimates of RUF coefficients (SE) ∧ β *Forest (Intercept) ∧ β *Clearcut 40%A ∧ ∧ ∧ ∧ β *Patches 40%A β *CWED β *Distance Nest β *Interaction -1.19 (0.01) 0.06 (0.001) All birds nesting in area of 40% Aggregated (n=12) -5.94 (0.04) -2.42 (0.93) -0.94 (0.14) 0.96 (0.05) Nesting in Forest (n=9) -5.75 (0.06) -2.82 (1.65) -0.94 (0.25) 0.86 (0.09) Nesting on Treatment (n=3) -6.49 (0.14) -1.24 (0.05) 0.86 (0.06) 1.26 (0.03) -1.26 (0.01) -0.99 (0.02) 0.06 (0.002) 0.05 (0.003) 36 37 ∧ Table 6a-c. Estimates of standardized RUF coefficients ( β ) for groups of nesting Brown Creepers that either placed their nest within <85m or >85m away from edge. The birds are further grouped by successful and unsuccessful nesters and their mean coefficients tested for difference using a 2-sample t-test at α=0.05. For a definition of the coefficients see Table 4a-c. (a) Brown Creepers nesting within <85m from edge. ∧ Mean Standardized coefficients ( β ) ∧ ∧ ∧ ∧ β Clearcut β Patches β Thinned β CWED All birds nesting within <85m from edge (n=30) -0.44 0.03 -0.12 0.04 Successful Nests (n=17) -0.47 -0.04 -0.21 0.01 Unsuccessful Nests (n=13) -0.40 0.12 -0.01 0.08 0.59 0.25 0.32 0.60 ∧ ∧ P-value (Ho: β S = β U) (b) Brown Creeper nesting >85m away from edge. ∧ Mean Standardized coefficients ( β ) ∧ ∧ ∧ ∧ β Clearcut β Patches β Thinned β CWED All birds nesting >85m away from edge (n=12) -0.18 0.11 -0.05 0.12 Successful Nests (n=7) -0.46 0 0.06 0.07 Unsuccessful Nests (n=5) -0.05 0.17 -0.16 0.17 sample size too small sample size too small 0.82 0.50 ∧ ∧ P-value (Ho: β S = β U) 38 Table 6 (continued) (c) Comparing all Brown Creepers nesting within <85m or >85m from edge. ∧ Mean Standardized coefficients ( β ) ∧ All birds nesting within <85m from edge (n=30) All birds nesting >85m away from edge (n=12) ∧ ∧ P-value (Ho: β S = β U) ∧ ∧ ∧ β Clearcut β Patches β Thinned β CWED -0.44 0.03 -0.12 0.04 -0.18 0.11 -0.05 0.12 0.21 0.73 0.85 0.47 39 Table 7. Average nesting success and territory productivity (Vickery Index ± SE; see Vickery et al. 1992 for a definition of the index) for all Brown Creepers in the DEMO study area during the breeding seasons 2003 and 2004. Productivity and nesting success is compared to a population of Brown Creepers in the suburban area of Seattle, Washington (Blewett and Marzluff 2005). This Study (2003) This Study (2004) This Study (2003 and 2004) Suburban Area Average Vickery Rank (no. of territories) 4.4 ± 1.4 (25) 3.5 ± 1.3 (29) 3.9 ± 1.4 (55) 3.7 ± 0.2 (61) Percent Nesting Success (no. of nests) 63 (27) 48 (21) 54 (48) 59 (27) 40 BIBLIOGRAPHY Adams, E. M., and M. L. Morrison. 1993. Effects of forest stand structure and composition on Red-breasted Nuthatches and Brown Creepers. Journal of Wildlife Management. 57:616-629. Anderson, S. H., and B. J. Crompton. 2002. The effects of shelterwood logging on bird community composition in the Black Hills, Wyoming. Forest Science 48:365-372. Andren, H. 1992. Corvid density and nest predation in relation to forest fragmentation a landscape perspective. Ecology Washington DC 73(3):794-804. Aubry, K. B., M. P. Amaranthus, C. B. Halpern, J. D. White, B. L. Woodard, C. E. Peterson, C. A. Lagoudakis, and A. J. Horton. 1999. Evaluating the effects of varying levels and patterns of green-tree retention: experimental design of the DEMO study. Northwest Science 73:12-26. Baddeley, A. and R. Turner. 2000. Practical maximum pseudolikelihood for spatial point patterns. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Statistics 42(3):283-322. Baddeley, A., and R. Turner. 2005. Spatstat: an R package for analyzing spatial point patterns. Journal of Statistical Software 12(6):1-42. Bedford, S.E., and M.B. Usher. 1994. Distribution of arthropod species across the margins of farm woodlands. Agriculture, Ecosystem and Environment 48:295-305. Beese, W.J., and A.A. Bryant. 1999. Effect of alternative silvicultural systems on vegetation and bird communities in coastal montane forests of British Columbia, Canada. Forest Ecology and Management 115(2-3):231-242. Belisle, M., and A. Desrochers. 2002. Gap-crossing decisions by forest birds: An empirical basis for parameterizing spatially-explicit, individual-based models. Landscape Ecology 17(3):219-231. Belisle, M., and C.C. St. Clair. 2002. Cumulative effects of barriers on the movements of forest birds. Conservation Ecology 5(2):9. Bibby, C.J., N.D. Burgess, and D.A. Hill. 1992. Bird census techniques. Academic Press Inc. San Diego. 257 pp. Bird, G.A., and L. Chatarpaul. 1986. Effect of whole-tree and conventional forest harvest on soil microarthropods. Canadian Journal of Zoology 64:1986-1993. 41 Blewett, C.M., and J.M. Marzluff. 2005. Effects of urban sprawl on snags and the abundance and productivity of cavity-nesting birds. Condor 107:678-693. Bradley, J.E., and J.M. Marzluff. 2003. Rodents as nest predators: Influences on predatory behavior and consequences to nesting birds. Auk 120(4):1180-1187. Brand, L.A., and T.L. George. 2000. Predation risks for nesting birds in fragmented coast redwood forest. Journal of Wildlife Management 64(1):42-51. Brand, L.A., and T.L. George. 2001. Response of passerine birds to forest edge in coast redwood forest fragments. Auk 118(3):678-686. Brenner, G. 2005. Fecal analysis report. Pacific Analytics, L.L.C. Boulet, M., M. Darveau, and L. Belanger. 2000. A landscape perspective of bird nest predation in a managed boreal black spruce forest. Ecoscience 7(3):281-289. Burger, J. C., M. A. Patten, J. T. Rotenberry, and R. A. Redak. 1999. Foraging ecology of the California gnatcatcher deduced from fecal samples. Oecologia 120:304-310. Calver, M. C., and R. D. Wooller. 1982. A technique for assessing the taxa, length, dry weight and energy content of the arthropod prey of birds. Australian Wildlife Research 9:293-301. Carey, A.B., M.M. Hardt, S.P. Horton, and B.L. Biswell. 1991. Spring bird communities in the Oregon coast range USA. USFS-GTR-PNW. 285:123-143. Chambers, C.L., W.C. McComb, J.C. II Tappeiner. 1999. Breeding bird responses to three silvicultural treatments in the Oregon Coast range. Ecological Applications 9(1):171-185. Chen, J., J.F. Franklin, and T.A. Spies. 1993. Contrasting microclimates among clearcut, edge, and interior of old-growth Douglas-fir forest. Agricultural and Forest Meteorology 63:219-237. Christman, B.J., and A.A. Dhondt. 1997. Nest predation in Black-capped Chickadees: How safe are cavity nests? Auk 114(4):769-773. Cline, S.P., A.B. Berg, and H.M. Wight. 1980. Snag characteristics and dynamics in Douglas-fir forests, western Oregon. Journal of Wildlife Management 44(4):773786. Coterill, S.E., and S.J. Hannon. 1999. No evidence of short-term effects of clear-cutting on artificial nest predation in boreal mixedwood forests. Canadian Journal of Forest Research 29(12):1900-1910. 42 Davis, C.M. 1978. A nesting study of the Brown Creeper. Living Bird 17:237-263. DeSanto, T.L., and M.F. Willson. 2001. Predator abundance and predation of artificial nests in natural and anthropogenic coniferous forest edges in southeast Alaska. Journal of Field Ornithology 72(1):136-149. Diggle, P.J. 2003. Statistical analysis of spatial point patterns. Arnold, London. Donovan, T.M, P.W. Jones, E.M. Annand, and F.R. Thompson. 1997. Variation in local-scale edge effects: Mechanisms and landscape context. Ecology Washington DC 78(7):2064-2074. Duguay, J. P., P. B. Wood, and G. W. Miller. 2000. Effects of timber harvests on invertebrate biomass and avian nest success. Wildlife Society Bulletin 28:11231131. Dunning, J.B. jr. 1990. Meeting the assumptions of foraging models: an example using tests of avian patch choice. Studies in Avian Biology 13:462-470. Franklin, J.F., and C.T. Dyrness. 1988. Natural vegetation of Oregon and Washington. Oregon State University Press. Franzreb, K.E. 1985. Foraging ecology of Brown Creepers in a mixed-coniferous forest. J. Field Ornithol. 56(1):9-16. Garton, E.O., M.J. Wisdom, F.A. Leban, and B.K. Johnson. 2001. Experimental design for radiotelemetry studies. Pages 16-42 in J.J. Millspaugh and J.M. Marzluff, eds. Radio tracking and animal populations. Academic Press, San Diego, California, USA. Gilbert, F.F., and R. Allwine 1991. Spring bird communities in the Oregon Cascade Range USA. USDA Forest Service GTR-R-PNW. 285:145-159. Geyer, C. 1999. Likelihood inference for spatial point processes. Pages 79-140 in O.E. Barndorff-Nielsen, W.S. Kendall, and M.N.M. van Lieshout, eds. Stochastic geometry: Likelihood and computation. Monographs on Statistics and Applied Probability 80. Chapman and Hall/CRC, Boca Raton, Florida, USA. Hagar, J.C., W.C. McComb, and W.H. Emmingham. 1996. Bird communities in commercially thinned and unthinned Douglas-fir stands of Western Oregon. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 24:353-366. Halaj, J. In preparation. Effects of green-tree retention treatments on trophic interactions between bark-dwelling arthropods and birds. 43 Halaj, J. 1996. Abundance and community composition of arboreal spiders: the relative importance of habitat structure, prey availability and competition. Ph.D. Thesis, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR. Halaj, J., D. W. Ross, and A. R. Moldenke. 1997. Negative effects of ant foraging on spiders in Douglas-fir canopies. Oecologia 109:313-322. Halpern, C. B., S. A. Evans, C. R. Nelson, D. McKenzie, D. A. Liguori, D. E. Hibbs, and M. G. Halaj. 1999. Response of forest vegetation to varying levels and patterns of green-tree retention: an overview of a long-term experiment. Northwest Science 73:27-44. Hansen, A.J., W.C. McComb, R. Vega, M.G. Raphael and M. Hunter. 1995. Bird habitat relationships in natural and managed forests in the West Cascades of Oregon. Ecological Applications 5(3):555-569. Hanula, J. L., and K. C. P. New. 1996. A trap for capturing arthropods crawling up tree boles. USDA Forest Service Research Note SRS-3. 8 p. Hanula, J. L., and K. Franzreb. 1998. Source, distribution and abundance of macroarthropods on the bark of longleaf pine: potential prey of the red-cockaded woodpecker. Forest Ecology and Management 102:89-102. Hanula, J. L., K. E. Franzreb, and W. D. Pepper. 2000. Longleaf pine characteristics associated with arthropods available for red-cockaded woodpeckers. Journal of Wildlife Management 64:60-70. Harestad, A.S., and D.G. Keisker. 1989. Nest tree use by primary cavity-nesting birds in south central British Columbia Canada. Canadian Journal of Zoology 67(4):1067-1073. Heithecker, , and C.B. Halpern. Variation in microclimate associated with dispersedretention harvests in coniferous forests of western Washington. Forest Ecology and Management. In press. Hejl, S.J., K.R. Newlon, M.E. McFadzen, J.S. Young, and C.K. Ghalambor. 2002. Brown Creeper (Certhia americana). P. 1-32 in A. Poole and F. Gill, eds. The birds of North America, No. 669. The Birds of North America, Inc., Philadelphia, PA. Hinsley, S.A. 2000. The costs of multiple patch use by birds. Landscape Ecology 15(8):765-775. Hobson. K.A., and E. Bayne. 2000a. Breeding bird communities in boreal forest of Western Canada: Consequences of “unmixing” the mixedwoods. Condor 102(4):759-769. 44 Hobson. K.A., and E. Bayne. 2000b. Effects of forest fragmentation by agriculture on avian communities in the southern boreal mixedwoods of Western Canada. Wilson Bulletin 112(3):373-387. Hoekstra, J. M., Bell, R. T., Launer, A. E., Murphy, D. D. 1995. Soil arthropod abundance in coast redwood forest: Effect of selective timber harvest. Environmental Entomology 24:246-252. Holling, C.S. 1965. The functional response to invertebrate predators to prey density. Memoirs of the Entomological Society of Canada 45:1-60. Holling, C.S. 1966. The functional response to invertebrate predators to prey density. Memoirs of the Entomological Society of Canada 48:1-86. Holmes, R. T., J. C. Schultz, and P. Nothnagle. 1979. Bird predation on forest insects: An exclosure experiment. Science 206:462-463. Hooge, P.N., and B. Eichenlaub. 1997. Animal movement extension to ArcView: version 1.1. Alaska Biological Science Center, U.S. Geological Survey, Anchorage, Alaska, USA. Huhta, V. 1971. Succession in the spider communities of the forest floor after clearcutting and prescribed burning. Annales Zoologici Fennici 8:483-542. Huhta, V., E. Karppinen, M. Nurminen, and A. Valpas. 1967. Effect of silvicultural practices upon arthropod, annelid and nematode populations in coniferous forest soil. Annales Zoologici Fennici 4:87-143. Huhta, E., T. Aho, A. Jäntti, P. Suorsa, M. Kuitunen, A. Nikula, and H. Hakkarainen. 2004. Forest fragmentation increases nest predation in the Eurasian Treecreeper. Conservation Biology 18(1):148-155. Hutto, R. L. 1990. Measuring the availability of food resources. Studies in Avian Biology 13:20-28. Hutto, R.L. 2000. Using Landbirds as an indicator species group. Pages 75-92 in J.M. Marzluff and R. Sallabanks, eds. Avian Conservation Research and Management. Island Press. Washington, D.C. Hutto, R.L, and J.S. Young. 1999. Habitat relationships of landbirds in the Northern Region. USFS-GTR. RMRS-32. Jackson, J. A. 1979. Tree surfaces as foraging substrates for insectivorous birds. Pages 69-93 in J. G. Dickson, R. N. Connor, R. R. Fleet, J. A. Jackson, and J. C. Kroll, editors. The role of insectivorous birds in forest ecosystems. Academic Press, New York. 381 pp. 45 James, F. C., and H. H. Shugart. 1970. A quantitative method of habitat description. Audubon Field Notes 24:727-735. Johnson, M.D. 2000. Evaluation of an arthropod sampling technique for measuring food availability for forest insectivorous birds. Journal of Field Ornithology 71(1):88-109. Kernohan, B.J., R.A. Gitzen, and J.J. Millspaugh. 2001. Analysis of animal space use and movements. Pages 126-166 in J.J. Millspaugh and J.M. Marzluff, eds. Radio tracking and animal populations. Academic Press, Inc., San Diego, California, USA. Kohm, K.A., and J.F. Franklin. 1997. Creating a forestry for the 21st century: The science of ecosystem management. Island Press, Washington, D.C. Kuitunen, M. 1989. Food supply and reproduction in the Common Treecreeper (Certhia familiaris). Annuals of Zoolocia Fennici 26:25-33. Kuitunen, M., and P. Helle. 1988. Relationship of the common Treecreeper Certhia familiaris to edge effect and forest fragmentation. Ornis Fennica 65:150-154. Kuitunen, M., and M. Mäkinen. 1993. An experiment on nest site choice of the common Treecreeper in fragmented boreal forest. Ornis Fennica 70:163-167. Kuitunen, M., and T. Törmälä.1983. The food of Treecreeper Certhia f. familiaris nestlings in southern Finland. Ornis Fennica 60:42-44. Lance, A.N, and M. Phinney 2000. Bird responses to partial retention timber harvesting in central interior British Columbia. Forest Ecology and Management 142:267280. Larrison, E.J., and K.G. Sonnenberg. 1968. Washington birds: Their location and identification. Seattle Audubon Society, Seattle. 258 pp. Lehmkuhl, J.F., S.D. West, C.L. Chambers, W.C. McComb, D.A. Manuwal, K.B. Aubry, J.L. Erickson, R.A. Gitzen, and M. Leu. 1999. Assessing Wildlife Response to varying levels and patterns of green-tree retention in Western Oregon and Washington. Northwest Science 73:45-63. Leu, M., D. Manuwal, and S. Pearson. The response of eight small forest singing species to varying levels and patterns of green-tree retention. In preparation. Lindenmayer, D.B., and J.F. Franklin. 2002. Conserving forest biodiversity: A comprehensive multiscaled approach. Island Press, Washington, D.C. 46 Lovette, I.J., and R.T. Holmes. 1995. Foraging behavior of American Redstarts in breeding and wintering habitats: Implications for relative food availability. Condor 97(3):782-791. Luginbuhl, J.M., J.M. Marzluff, J.E. Bradley, M.G. Raphael, and D.E. Varland. 2001. Corvid survey techniques and the relationship between corvid relative abundance and nest predation. Journal of Field Ornithology 72:556-572. Lundquist, R. W., and D. A. Manuwal. 1990. Seasonal differences in foraging habitat of cavity-nesting birds in the southern Washington Cascades. Studies in Avian Biology 13:218-225. Lundquist, R.W., and J.M. Mariani. 1991. Nesting habitat and abundance of snagdependent birds in the southern Washington Cascade Range USA. USDA Forest Service GTR-R-PNW. 285:221-240. Manly, B.F.J., L.L. McDonald, D.L. Thomas, T.L. McDonald, and W.P. Erickson. 2002. Resource selection by animals: Statistical design and analysis for field studies. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Netherlands. Mannan, R.W., E.C. Meslow, and H.M. Wight. 1980. Use of snags by birds in Douglas-fir forests, Western Oregon. Journal of Wildlife Management 44(4):787797. Manuwal, D.A., and M. Leu. In preparation. The response of song birds to varying levels and patterns of green-tree retention. Mariani, J. M., and D. A. Manuwal. 1990. Factors influencing Brown Creeper (Certhia americana) abundance patterns in the southern Washington Cascade Range. Studies in Avian Biology 13:53-57. Martin, K., M.D. Mossop, and K.E.H. Aitken. 2002. Nest webs: The structure and function of cavity nesting and song bird communities in unmanaged stands, and responses to harvesting treatments and forest health dynamics in Cariboo-Chilcotin forests. In preparation. Marzluff, J. M. and M. Restani. 1999. The effects of forest fragmentation on avian nest predation. Pp: 155-169. In: Forest Fragmentation: Wildlife and Management Implications (J. A. Rochelle, L. A. Lehmann, and J. Wisniewski eds.). Brill Academic Publishing, Leiden, The Neatherlands. Marzluff, J. M., M. G. Raphael, and R. Sallabanks. 2000. Understanding the effects of forest management on avian species. Wildlife Society Bulletin 28:1132-1143. 47 Marzluff, J.M., S.T. Knick, and J.J. Millspaugh. 2001. High-tech behavioral ecology: Modeling the distribution of animal activities to better understand wildlife space use and resources selection. Pages 310-326 in J.J. Millspaugh and J.M. Marzluff, eds. Radio tracking and animal populations. Academic Press, Inc., San Diego, California, USA. Marzluff, J.M, J.J. Millspaugh, P. Hurvitz, and M. Handcock. 2004. Resources utilization by an avian nest predator: relating resources to a probabilistic measure of animal space use. Ecology 85(5):1411–1427. McClelland, B.R., and P.T. McClelland. 2000. Red-naped Sapsucker nest trees in northern Rocky Mountain old-growth forest. Wilson Bulletin 112(1):44-50. McGarigal, K., and S.A. Cushman. 2002. Comparative evaluation of experimental approaches to the study of habitat fragmentation effects. Ecological Applications 12(2):335-345. McGarigal, K., S. A. Cushman, M. C. Neel, and E. Ene. 2002. FRAGSTATS: Spatial Pattern Analysis Program for Categorical Maps. Computer software program produced by the authors at the University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Available at the following web site: www.umass.edu/landeco/research/fragstats/fragstats.html McGarigal, K., and B.J. Marks. 1995. FRAGSTATS: spatial pattern analysis program for quantifying landscape structure. Gen. Tech. Report PNW-GTR-351, USDA Forest Service, Pacific Northwest Research Station, Portland, OR. McIver, J. D., G. L. Parsons, and A. R. Moldenke. 1992. Litter spider succession after clear-cutting in a Western coniferous forest. Canadian Journal of Forest Research 22:984-992. Meslow, E.C., and H.M. Wight. 1975. Avifauna and succession in Douglas-fir forests of the Pacific Northwest. Pages 266-271 in D.R. Smith, tech. coord. Management of forest and range habitats for nongame birds. USDA Forest Service GTR-WO-1. Millspaugh, J.J., and J.M. Marzluff. 2001. Radio tracking and animal populations: Past trends and future needs. Pages 310-326 in J.J. Millspaugh and J.M. Marzluff, eds. Radio tracking and animal populations. Academic Press, Inc., San Diego, California, USA. Moore, J., R. Ouimet, C. Camwere, and D. Houle. 2002. Effects of two silvicultural practices on soil fauna abundance in a northern hardwood forest, Quebec, Canada. Canadian Journal of Soil Science 82:105-113. Myers, R.H., D.C. Montgomery, and G.G. Vining. 2002. Generalized linear models: With applications in engineering and the sciences. Wiley Interscience, New York, USA. 48 Noon, B. R. 1981. Techniques for sampling avian habitat. Pages 42-52 in D. E. Capen, editor. The use of multivariate statistics in studies of wildlife habitat. USDA Forest Service GTR-RM-87. Norton, M.R., and S.J. Hannon. 1997. Songbird response to partial-cut logging in the boreal mixedwood forest of Alberta. Canadian Journal of Forest Research 27:4453. Ortega, Y.K., and D.E. Capen. 2002. Roads as edges: Effects on birds in forested landscapes. Forest Science 48(2):381-390. Otvos, I.S., and R.W. Stark. 1985. Arthropod food of some forest-inhabiting birds. Canadian Entomologist 117:971-990. Otis, D.L, and G.C. White. 1999. Autocorrelation of location estimates and the analysis of radio tracking data. Journal of Wildlife Management 63(3):1039-1044. Peltonen, M., K. Heliovaara, and R. Vaisanen. 1997. Forest insects and environmental variation in stand edges. Silva Fennica 31(2):129-141. Perrins, C. M, and T .R. Birkhead. 1984. Avian ecology. Chapman and Hall, New York. Petterson, R.B., J.P. Ball, K.E. Renhorn, P.A. Esseen, and K. Sjöberg. 1995. Invertebrate communities in boreal forest canopies as influenced by forestry and lichens with implications for passerine birds. Biological Conservation 74:57-63. Purcell, K.L., and J. Verner 1999. Nest predation of open and cavity nesting birds in oak woodlands. Wilson Bulletin 111(2):251-256. Pyle, P. 1997. Identification guide to North American birds. Slate Creek Press. Bolinas, California. R Development Core Team (2005). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3900051-07-0, URL http://www.R-project.org. Ralph, C. P., S. E. Nagata, and C. J. Ralph. 1985. Analysis of droppings to describe diets of small birds. Journal of Field Ornithology. 56:165-174. Ralph, C.J., P.W.C. Paton, and C.A. Taylor. 1991. Habitat association patterns of breeding birds and small mammals in Douglas-fir-hardwood stands in northeastern California and southeastern Oregon. USDA Forest Service GTR-R-PNW 285:379393. 49 Ralph, C.J., G.R. Geupel, P. Pyle, T.E. Martin, and D.F. DeSante. 1993. Handbook of field methods for monitoring landbirds. USDA Forest Service PSW-GTR-144. Raphael, Martin G. and M. White. 1984. Use of snags by cavity-nesting birds in the Sierra Nevada. Wildlife Monographs No.86:1-66. Rempel, R.S., A. Carr, and P. Elkie. 1999. Patch analyst and patch analyst (grid) function reference. (http;//www.lakehead.ca/~rrempel/patch/). Center for Northern Forest Ecosystem Research, Ontario Ministry of Natural Resources, Lakehead University, Thunder Bay, Ontario, Canada. Robichaud, I., M.A. Villard, and C.S. Machtans. 2002. Effects of forest regeneration on songbird movements in a managed forest landscape of Alberta, Canada. Landscape Ecology 17(3):247-262. Rodewald, A.D. 2002. Nest predation in forested regions: Landscape and edge effects. Journal of Wildlife Management 66(3):634-640. Rogers, L. E., W. T. Hinds, and R. L. Buschbom. 1976. A general weight vs. length relationship for insects. Annals of the Entomological Society of America 69:387389. Rogers, L. E., R. L. Buschbom, and C. R. Watson. 1977. Length-weight relationships of shrub-steppe invertebrates. Annals of the Entomological Society of America 70:51-53. Rosenberg, K. V., and R. J. Cooper. 1990. Approaches to avian diet analysis. Studies in Avian Biology. 13:80-90. Rosenberg, K. V., and M.G. Raphael. 1986. Effects of forest fragmentation on vertrebrates in Douglas-fir forests. Pages 263-272 in J. Verner, M.L. Morrison, and C.J. Ralph, eds.). Wildlife 2000: Modeling habitat relationships of terrestrial vertebrates. University of Wisconsin Press, Madison. Saab, Victoria A. and Jonathan G. Dudley. 1988. Responses of cavity-nesting birds to stand-replacement fire and salvage logging in ponderosa pine/Douglas-fir forests of southwestern Idaho. Res. Pap. RMRS-RP-11. Fort Collins, CO: U.S. Department of Agriculture, Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Research Station. Sallabanks, R., E. B. Arnett, and J. M. Marzluff. 2000. An evaluation of research on the effects of timber harvest on bird populations. Wildlife Society Bulletin 28:11441155. Sandström, U. 1991. Enhanced predation rates on cavity bird nests at deciduous forest edges an experimental study. Ornis Fennica 68(3):93-98. 50 Schieck, J., K. Stuart-Smith, and M. Norton. 2000. Bird communities are affected by amount and dispersion of vegetation retained in mixedwood boreal forest harvest areas. Forest Ecology and Management 126:239-254. Schluter, D. 1984. Feeding correlates of breeding and social organizations in two Galapagos finches. The Auk 101:59-68. Schoener, T. W. 1980. Length-weight regressions in tropical and temperate forestunderstory insects. Annals of the Entomological Society of America 73:106-109. Seaman, D.E., J.J. Millspaugh, B.J. Kernohan, G.C. Brundige, K.J. Raedeke, and R.A. Gitzen. 1999. Effects of sample size on kernel home range estimates. Journal of Wildlife Management 63:739-747. Shure, D.J., and D.L. Phillips. 1991. Patch size of forest openings and arthropod populations. Oecologia 86:325-334. Smith, K.J., and J.T. Rotenberry. 1990. Quantifying food resources in avian studies: Present problems and future needs. Studies in Avian Biology 13:3-5. Sokal, R. R., and F. J. Rohlf. 1995. Biometry: The principles and practice of statistics in biological research. 3rd edition. W. H. Freeman and Co., New York, New York. 887 pp. Sousa, P.J. 1987. Habitat suitability index models: Hairy Woodpecker. U.S. Fish Wildl. Serv. Biol. Rep. 82(10.146). 19 pp. Suhonen, J., and M. Kuitunen. 1991. Food choice and feeding by male and female Common Treecrepers (Certhia familiaris) during the nestling period. Ornis Fennica 68:17-25. Suorsa, P., H. Helle, V. Koivunen, E. Huhta, A. Nikula, and H. Hakkarainen. 2004. Effects of forest patch size on physiological stress and immunocompetence in an area-sensitive passerine, the Eurasian Treecreeper (Certhia familiaris): an experiment. Proceedings of the Royal Society London 271:435-440. Suorsa, P., E. Huhta, A. Jäntti, Aa. Nikula, H. Helle, M. Kuitunen, V. Koivunen, and H. Hakkarainen. 2005. Thresholds in selection of breeding habitat by the Eurasian Treecreeper (Certhia familiaris). Biological Conservation 121:443-452. Steger, C., and C.L. Hitchcock. 1998. Influence of forest structure and diseases on nestsite selection by Red-breasted Nuthatches. Journal of Wildlife Management 62(4):1349-1358. Stephens, D. W., and J. R. Krebs. 1986. Foraging theory: Monographs in behavior and ecology. Princeton University Press. Princeton, New Jersey. 51 Tewksbury, J.J., S.J. Hejl, and T.E. Martin. 1998. Breeding productivity does not decline with increasing fragmentation in a western landscape. Ecology Washington DC 79(8):2890-2903. Thiollay, J.M. 1988. Comparative foraging success of insectivorous birds in tropical and temperate forests - ecological implications. Oikos 53(1):17-30. Tittler, R., S.J. Hannon, and M.R. Norton. 2001. Residual tree retention ameliorates short-term effects of clear-cutting on some boreal songbirds. Ecological Applications 11(6):1656-1666. Van Horne, B., and A. Bader. 1990. Diet of nestling winter wrens in relationship to food availability. Condor 92:413-420. Verts, B.J., and L.N. Carraway. 1998. Land mammals of Oregon. University of California Press, Berkely. 270 pp. Vickery, P.D., M.L. Hunter jr., and J.V. Ills. 1992. Use of a new reproductive index to evaluate relationship between habitat quality and breeding success. The Auk 109(4):697-705. Virkkala, R. 2004. Bird species dynamics in a managed southern boreal forest in Finland. Forest Ecology and Management 195:151-163. Virkkala, R., A. Rajasärkkä, R.A. Väisänen, M. Vickholm, and e. Virolainen. 1994. Conservation value of nature reserves: do hole-nesting birds prefer protected forests in southern Finland? Ann. Zool. Fennici 31:173-186. Walters, E.L., and E.H. Miller. 2001. Predation on nesting woodpeckers in British Columbia. Canadian Field Naturalist 115(3):413-419. Weikel, J. M., and J. P. Hayes. 1999. The foraging ecology of cavity-nesting birds in young forests of the northern coast range of Oregon. Condor 101:58-66. West, S.D., R.A. Gitzen, C.C. Maguiere, and T.E. Manning. In preparation. Response of terrestrial small mammals to varying levels and patterns of green-tree retention. Wiens, J.A. 1989. Spatial scaling in ecology. Functional Ecology 3:385-397. Wilcove, D.S. 1985. Nest predation in forest tracts and the decline of migratory songbirds. Ecology Washington DC 66(4):1211-1214. Wolda, H. 1990. Food availability for an insectivore and how to measure it. Studies in Avian Biology 13:38-43. 52 Zar, J. H. 1999. Biostatistical analysis. 4th edition. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, New Jersey. 663 pp. Zarnowitz, J. and D. Manuwal. 1985. The effects of forest management on cavitynesting birds in Northwestern Washington. Journal of Wildlife Management 49: 55-263. 53 Appendix A: Sample Code for RUF Computation in Program R This sample code will fit a Geyer saturation process function to the relocation points of a Brown Creeper located at the DEMO study area for the creation of a RUF, with the covariates for landcover “vergrd8m.csv”, contrast-weighted edge “area_cwed.csv”, and distance to nest “nestloc”. This code was developed by Mark S. Handcock. For the code package please visit the following web site: http://csde.washington.edu/~handcock/ruf/ 54 Appendix B: Fitting the Spatial Point Process Model Section 1: Relocations of the Brown Creeper “wo03” (small circles) and land cover covariates at the 40% aggregated treatment unit. The white area represents the surrounding forest, the dark grey circles the 40% aggregated patches, and the light grey area the clearcut area. Section 2: Use of the land covers by the bird “wo03”. 55 Section 3: Contrast-weighted edge covariate and the bird “wo03”. Shades of dark grey to lighter grey represent a decrease in the edge contrast from high to low, respectively. Section 4: Use of the contrast-weighted edge resource by the bird “wo03”. 56 Section 5: Distance to nest covariate and the bird “wo03”. Shades of light grey to darker grey represent the increasing distance away from the nest. Section 6: Use of resources in relation to distance to nest by the bird “wo03”. 57 Section 7: Fitted Geyer process intensity function for the bird “wo03”. The intensity of resource use is characterized by the shades from dark grey (highest use) to white (low or no use). 58 Section 8: Simulation of fitted Geyer process intensity function for the bird “wo03”. Section 9: Another simulation of the fitted intensity function for the bird “wo03”. 59 Appendix C: Bark Arthropods as Food Abundance I collected fecal samples during the banding process of Brown Creepers that set up territories at my study sites (n=9 in 2003 and n=4 in 2004). Fecal analysis can assist in the understanding of diet preferences of my focal bird species (Calver and Wooller 1982, Ralph et al. 1985, Van Horne and Bader 1990, Burger et al. 1999). Although this technique has its limitations (Rosenberg and Cooper 1990), it can be used successfully as a complementary method to observations of bird foraging behavior and prey availability assessment to gain a better understanding of avian foraging ecology (Halaj, in preparation). There was a 92% similarity between bark arthropod samples and the food for nestlings of the Eurasian Treecreeper (Certhia familiaris; Kuitunen 1989). The fecal sample analysis was performed by Cascadian, Inc. and Pacific Analytics, L.L.C (for more details see Brenner 2005, and Halaj, in preparation). The samples contained mainly fragments of small beetles (Coleoptera), of which some were identified to be in the family of Staphylinidae, Elateridae, and Curculionidae (Section 1 and 2). There were also some fragments of spiders (Arachnidae), one fly and fly egg (Diptera), and parts of Lepidoptera larvae (probably Tortricidae). All beetles were assumed to be less than 10 mm in size. The main size class of diet items by biomass for nestling Treecreepers in southern Finland was about 5-10mm (Kuitunen and Törmälä 1983). Coleoptera also comprised the largest portion of Brown Creeper stomach content in California (Otvos and Stark 1985). Spiders contributed most in biomass and energy to the diet of adult and nestling Treecreepers in Finland (Kuitunen and Törmälä 1983, Suhonen and Kuitunen 1991), and also were most abundant in the diets of Brown Creepers in North America, Washington (Mariani and Manuwal 1990). Fecal analysis might not reflect adequately the amount of soft bodied prey such as arachnids or lepidoptera species, due to the destructive effects of digestion. 60 Section 1: Estimated number of prey items in each fecal sample. Fecal Sample 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 Coleoptera 3 2 1 3 5 2 2 4 2 1 4 1 0 Lepidoptera Larvae 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 0 Diptera Fly Diptera Egg Spider 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 0 1 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 Section 2: Proportions of arthropod and arachnid prey items in the fecal samples of captured Brown Creepers at my study sites (n=13). Spider 17% Diptera Egg 2% Diptera Fly 2% Lepidoptera 5% Coleoptera Lepidoptera Coleoptera 74% Diptera Fly Diptera Egg Spider