This file was created by scanning the printed publication. Errors identified by the software have been corrected; however, some errors may remain. SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 1978. The ecology and behaviorof the PrairieWarblerDendroicadiscolor.Ornithol.Monogr. 26. PRESTON, F. W. 1948. The cowbird (M. ater) and the cuckoo (C. canorus).Ecology 29:115-116. ROTHSTEIN, S. I. 1974. Mechanismsof avian egg recognition:possiblelearnedand innatefactors.Auk 91: 796-807. ROTHSTEIN, S. I. 1975. Mechanismsof avian egg recognition:do birdsknowtheirown eggs?Anim. Behav. 23:268-278. ROTHSTEIN, S. I. 1978. Mechanismsof avian egg-recNOLAN, V., JR. 111 ognition: additional evidence for learned components. Anim. Behav. 26:671-677. VICTORIA, J. K. 1972. Clutchcharacteristics andeggdis- criminationabilityof the AfricanVillageWeaverbird Ploceuscucullatus.Ibis 114:367-376. L. H. 1949. Twenty-fiveeggs apparently WALKINSHAW, laid by a cowbird.Wilson Bull. 61:82-85. RockefellerUniversityField ResearchCenter,Millbrook, New York12545. Received 31 August 1981. Final acceptance 7 May 1982. Condor85:111-113 ? The Cooper Ornithological Society 1983 SEASONALVARIATION IN NEST PLACEMENTOF ABERT'S TOWHEES DEBORAHM. FINCH Several meteorologicalvariablesare known to influence the heat budgetsof nestingbirds(e.g., Walsbergand King 1978a, b). Such factorsinclude air temperature,incident radiation,wind, and humidity.If the microclimateof the nest is unfavorable,parentbirdsmay become inattentive, exposingeggs or nestlingsto excessive heat or cold. They may then desertthe nest and their offspringmay die. To improvethe probabilityof nestingsuccessfully,many Old Worlddesertbirdsplacetheirnests so as to receivemorning sunlight (Maclean 1976). Several investigatorshave demonstratednonrandomorientationof hole entrancesof New Worldcavity-nestingbirds(Ricklefsand Hainsworth 1968,Inouye 1976,Inouyeet al. 1981), butfew have tested for the significanceof nonrandomplacementor variation in placementbetweenseasonsof opennests.Maclean(1976) indicatedthat larksin the KalahariDesert orientedtheir nests to receiveshadein summerand sun in winter.Balda and Bateman (1970) predictedthat Pinyon Jays (Gymnorhinuscyanocephalus)in Arizona may place firstnests of the seasonon the sunnysides of ponderosapines (Pinus ponderosa),but that they may orientlaternests more randomly. They showed that these trees in Marchwere 30C warmeron the south side than on the north side, and that the jays selectednest sites to gain more heat:all jay nests, regardlessof season, had a significantsouthwardorientation. I reportherea secondtest of Baldaand Bateman's (1970) hypothesis,usingdata for a birdof hotterclimates, the Abert'sTowhee(Pipiloaberti).This sedentaryspecies inhabits only desert riparianregionsof the southwestern United States,whereasthe mobile Pinyon Jay occupiesa greaterelevationaland latitudinalrange,thoughbreeding chiefly in foothills and lower mountain ranges.Because nestsof the Abert'sTowheearetypicallyexposedto higher springand summertemperaturesthanthose of the Pinyon Jay, a secondtest of Baldaand Bateman'spredictionmay demonstrategreaterseasonalvariationin nest placement. The study was conductedin honey mesquite(Prosopis glandulosa)habitaton the ColoradoRiver Indian Reservation 10 km northof Ehrenberg,Arizona.FromJanuary to July 1980, I spent 15 h each week (84 5-h searches) looking for nests on, or near a griddedstudy plot of 20 ha. Field workterminatedin Julywhen no new nestswere started. The Abert'sTowheesin my studyareabuilt their open nests in mesquite,willow (Salix goodingii),exotic salt cedar(Tamarixchinesis),saltbush(Atriplexlentiformis),in- kweed (Suaeda torreyana),arrowweed(Tesseriasericia), and mistletoe (Phoradendroncalifornicum),a parasiteof mesquite(Finch 198la). Nests built in Marchwere constructed of mesquite bark, saltbush, salt cedar and arrowweedleaves, grassesand even newspaper.Laternests were made predominantlyof mesquiteleaves, which became availablein April (Finch 1981a). Compassdirectionof the nest with respectto the main trunksof mesquites,willowsand salt cedarswas recorded for 52 nests. I did not include nests built in small shrubs because compass directions were difficult to determine accurately.Nests were allocatedto early and late seasons by dividing in half the number of days of the observed breedingseason(118 days fromfirstto last nest initiated). The RayleighZ test (Batschelet1965)was used to test for randomnessof nest concentrationof early and late nests, and Batschelet's(1965:33)parametrictwo-sampletest applying the F-statistic was used to test for a differencein mean directionbetweenearlyand late nests. Information on solarpathsand prevailingwind directionsin the lower ColoradoRiver valley was providedby the Laboratoryof Climatology,ArizonaState University, Tempe. The resultsindicate that the placementsof early nests were more concentratedthan late nests (Table 1). Their distributionwas significantlyunimodal with a preferred southeastdirection,whereasthe distributionof late nests hada nonsignificantnorthwestwardorientation.The mean directionsof earlyand latenestsweresignificantlydifferent (F5(so,= 14.82,P < 0.001). Becausethe distributionof late nest orientationswas nonsignificant,the value of this twosample test may be lessened. The Rayleigh tests nevertheless confirmthe differencein the distributionsof nest orientations.The circulardistributionsof early and late nests are shown in Figure 1. These resultssuggestthat Abert'sTowhees place their nests in response to environmentalfactors that are importantonly duringthe first half of the breedingseason. In the lower ColoradoRiver valley, the sun rose at 890 (almostdue East)in March1980 and traversedthe south- TABLE 1. Orientationof Abert'sTowhee nests with respect to the main trunkof the tree. Season Number of nests Nest concentrationa Rayleigh's Z testb Mean directionc Early Late 24 28 0.367 0.205 3.23 1.18 1300 ? 690 3210 ?+ 720 a A value of I indicates all nests were placed in precisely the same direction, and a value of 0 indicates a uniform distribution. bRayleigh's Z test (Batschelet 1965) shows that the distribution of early nests was significantly unimodal (P < 0.05), whereas the distribution of late nests was random (P > 0.05). c Standard deviation is included. 112 SHORTCOMMUNICATIONS Early 00 n=24 2700 0*- 9 o Lote ?? /, 1800*180? i N 0" 0 "0 Late 270 - II 0 0? n = 28 0o n 0=28 0 180? 0 90 ?I 0 0 0 270 -900 04 0 0 ? 180 FIGURE I. Compassorientationof earlyand late nests in the lower ColoradoRiver valley. ern hemisphereof the sky; the elevation of the sun from the horizon was only 58*at solar noon. In June, the sun rose at 6 l*NEand was almostdirectlyoverhead(8 1 from closest horizon)at solar noon. Earlynests of Abert'sTowhees that were placed on the southeast sides of trees receivedmaximumincidentradiationin the morning,when the air was cool. As the breedingperiod progressedto rosefrom23.80C August,meanmaximumairtemperatures to 42.90C(Finch 198la), and nest placementthat allowed rapidheat gain presumablybecameless adaptive.Late in the season, nests tended to be placed northwestwardly although the distributionwas not significant.Warmer, southeasternnests may reduceadult and nestlingenergy expensesearly in the season, whereaslater, northwestern nests may protectembryosfrom potentiallylethalradiant heat (Bennett and Dawson 1979) and/or prevent high evaporativewaterlosses from parentsand offspring. Wind direction may also influencenest placement of Abert'sTowhees. A nest on the lee side of a tree may be betterprotectedfrom cooling or destructivewinds. From MarchthroughMay of 1980, the prevailingwind directions were from west-northwest,but from June through Septemberthe prevailingwinds were from south-south- east. Southeastwardly placednests in springwereshielded from the greatestproportionof winds (in contrast, see Balda and Bateman's 1970 study). Summer nests were apparentlyplaced randomlywith respectto wind direction. For a dark-plumagedbird like an Abert'sTowhee, convectiveheatloss via feathererectionin wind may even be a principalmeansof coolingduringthe desertsummer (Walsberget al. 1978). Earlynests of Abert's Towhees were thereforeplaced advantageouslyrelative to both solar radiationand prevailing winds. The relativeimportanceof either of these factorswas not determined.Becauseconcentrationof late nestswas not significantlydifferentfromrandom,it seems likely that either (1) air temperatureand wind did not influencenest site selectionand nestingsuccesslate in the season (e.g., air temperaturemay have been identicalon all sides of the tree as indicated by Balda and Bateman 1970), (2) the probabilityof reproductivesuccesswas independentof nest site placement,or (3) towheesused other, less obvious, methodsof selectingnest sites (e.g.,Walsberg 1981). Nesting success of the towhees descreasedduringthe season(Finch 198la), associatedwith an influxof BrownheadedCowbirds(Molothrusater).The pressuresof cowbird parasitismor predation(Finch 1981 lb, 1982a) may have overriddendecisions to place nests primarilywith regardto microclimaticconditions (see Best and Stauffer 1980:153).As the season progressed,towhees spent less time locating and constructingnests (Finch 1981 la) and even built some nests that fell apart (pers. observ.). In addition, they confiscatedabandonednests of their own or other species (Finch 1982b). This apparent inclination to "save time" at the end of the breedingperiod suggeststhat they took less care in selecting nest sites. When compared with an overwhelmingprobability of nestingfailurecausedby predationand brood parasitism (approximately 95%; Finch 1981a), a less-than-satisfac- tory microclimateat a late nest may have negligibleeffect on the productivityof Abert'sTowheesin the lower Colorado River valley. I thank C. J. Henny, F. L. Knopf, and H. Kruegerfor reviewingthis manuscript.I am gratefulto the Colorado River Indiantribesfor permissionto work on their land. LITERATURECITED R. P., ANDG. C. BATEMAN. 1970. The breeding BALDA, biology of the Pifion Jay. Living Bird 11:5-42. E. 1965. Statisticalmethodsforthe analysis BATSCHELET, of problems in animal orientationand certain biologicalrhythms.AmericanInstituteof BiologicalSciences, Washington,DC. A. F., ANDW. R. DAWSON. 1979. Physiological BENNETT, responsesof embryonicHeerman'sGulls to temperature. Physiol.Zool. 52:413-421. 1980. Factorsaffecting BEST,L. B., ANDD. F. STAUFFER. nestingsuccessin riparianbirdcommunities.Condor 82:149-158. FINCH, D. M. 198la. Variationin the reproductiveecology of the AbertTowhee.M.Sc.Thesis,ArizonaState Univ., Tempe. D. M. 1981b. Nest predationof Abert'sTowhees FINCH, by coachwhipsand Roadrunners.Condor83:389. D. M. 1982a. Rejectionofcowbirdeggsby Crissal FINCH, Thrashers.Auk 99:719-724. D. M. 1982b. Interspecificnest use by arid-land FINCH, birds. Wilson Bull 94:582-584. INOUYE,D. W. 1976. Nonrandom orientation of entranceholes to woodpeckernests in aspentrees. Condor 78:101-102. ANDD. W. INOUYE.1981. R. S., N. J. HUNTLY, INOUYE, Non-random orientationof Gila Woodpeckernest entrancesin SaguaroCacti. Condor83:88-89. G. L. 1976. Arid-zoneornithologyin Africa MACLEAN, SHORT COMMUNICATIONS and South America.Proc. XVI Int. Ornithol.Congr. (1974):468-480. RICKLEFS,R. E., AND F. R. HAINSWORTH.1968. Temperaturedependent behavior of the Cactus Wren. Ecology49:227-233. WALSBERG, G. E. 1981. Nest-site selection and the radiative environmentof the WarblingVireo. Condor 83:86-88. WALSBERG,G. E., E. S. CAMPBELL,AND J. R. KING. 1978. Animal coat color and radiativeheat gain:a re-evaluation. J. Comp. Physiol. 126:211-222. WALSBERG,G. E., AND J. R. KING. 1978a. The heatbud- 113 get of incubatingMountainWhite-crownedSparrows (Zonotrichialeucophrys)in Oregon.Physiol.Zool. 51: 92-103. G. E., ANDJ. R. KING. 1978b. The energetic WALSBERG, consequencesof incubationfor two passerinespecies. Auk 95:644-655. U.S. Departmentof Agriculture,Rocky MountainForest and Range ExperimentStation, 222 South 22nd Street, Laramie, Wyoming82070. Received 27 February1982. Final acceptance24 July 1982. Condor85:113 ? The Cooper Ornithological Society 1983 FLIGHT SONG AND SONG FLIGHT IN THE ORANGE-CROWNED WARBLER WILLIAMM. GILBERT Birds of many species commonly sing while flying. The behavior is characteristicof many open-countryspecies, such as larks, pipits, and Bobolinks (Dolichonyx oryzivorus)and also occurs among some woodland species, including several wood warblers(Parulidae;Ficken and Ficken 1962). In some of these warblerspecies, both the songgiven duringflight,and the flightitself, are distinctly differentfromperchsongand "normal"directflight.Ficken and Fickendescribedsuch songs as "usuallya variable warbling[accompaniedby a] rising flight on slowly flapping or quiveringwings [and]typicallya directand silent descent." Workershave reported"flightsongs"and "songflights" occurringin several species of Vermivora,includingthe Tennessee Warbler(V. peregrina;Bowdish and Philipp BowlesandBowles (V ruficapilla; 1916),NashvilleWarbler( 1906, and Thayer, reported in Chapman 1907), Bachman's Warbler(V. bachmanii;Howell 1924, reportedin Bent 1953), and Blue-wingedWarbler(V. pinus; Pitelka 1939). The songs and flightsdescribedfor these species, however,appearto be less modifiedthan those described by the Fickensfor non-Vermivorawarblers. I observedflightsongs and song flightsoccurringin the Orange-crownedWarbler(V. celata) during behavioral studiesof the speciesin its typicalnestinghabitatin Contra Costa County, central coastal California.I followed the behaviorof unmatedterritorialmales priorto 12:00,during March,April, and May of 1980, 1981, and 1982. Although I may have overlookQdthe behaviors, I did not observe flight songs and song flights in unmated birds during late February,June, and early July, nor among matedbirds duringany month. Populationsof these warblerswererelativelydensein my studyareas,with all males having at least two territorialneighbors.Territorialdisputes were common. Most flightsongs I heardwere begunin mid-flight,and completed on a perch. Occasionallythe entire song was renderedin the air. I could detect, by ear, no difference betweensongs sung duringflightand those sung entirely while perched.Whenin song flight,the wingsof a warbler "fluttered"or "quivered"in a way I nevcharacteristically er saw in normaldirect flight.I observednothingresembling the rising flightand direct descent describedby the Fickens. I noted nine instancesof birds singingwhile in flight during260 min of observationin which the individualOrange-crowned Warblerswerein sight.Sincethese birds usually sang four to six songs per minute, it is apparentthatflightsongsoccurredmuchless frequentlythan perch songs. Flight songs and song flightsapparentlyoccurredat random,and I noticed no associatedspecialcircumstances,such as the presenceof a female or a competing male. The flightsongs and song flightsI observedin Orangecrowned Warblersdifferedfrom those describedby the Fickens.Theygenerallyresembledthose describedforother species of Vermivora,although the flutteringin song flight has hitherto been reportedonly for the Nashville Warbler(Bowles and Bowles 1906). I am aware of only two reportsof song and flightbehaviorresemblingthose I havereportedhereoccurringin parulidsotherthanspecies of Vermivora.The AmericanRedstart(Setophagaruticilla) apparentlyhas an "unmodified"flightsong and song flight often associated with encounterswith females or rival males (Ficken 1962). The Kirtland'sWarbler(Dendroicakirtlandii)also has such songs and flights,but they do not seem to be associatedwith any special environmental context (Mayfield1960:133). Relatively unmodifiedflight song and song flight now are reportedfor the majorityof North American Vermivora,suggestingthat these forms of behavior are characteristicof the genus. LITERATURECITED A. C. 1953. Lifehistoriesof NorthAmericanwood BENT, warblers.U.S. Natl. Mus. Bull. 203. R. S., ANDP. B. PHILIPP. 1916. The Tennessee BOWDISH, Warblerin New Brunswick.Auk 33:1-8. C. W., ANDJ. H. BOWLES. 1906. The Calaveras BOWLES, Warblerin westernWashington.Condor8:68-69. F. M. 1907. The warblersof North America. CHAPMAN, Appleton& Co., NY. M. S. 1962. Agonisticbehaviorand territoryin FICKEN, the AmericanRedstart.Auk 79:607-632. M. S., ANDR. W. FICKEN.1962. The comparFICKEN, ative ethologyof the wood warblers:a review.Living Bird 1:103-122. H. 1960. The Kirtland'sWarbler.Cranbrook MAYFIELD, Inst., BloomfieldHills, MI. F. A. 1939. Flight song of the Blue-winged PITELKA, Warbler.Auk 56:340-341. 4630 DriftwoodCourt,El Sobrante,California94803. Received 22 February1982.Finalacceptance7 August1982.