Articles The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer’s disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: a longitudinal cohort study David A Bennett, Julie A Schneider, Yuxiao Tang, Steven E Arnold, Robert S Wilson Lancet Neurol 2006; 5: 406–12 Published Online April 4, 2006 DOI:10.1016/S1474-4422(06) 70417-3 Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center (D A Bennett MD, J A Schneider MD, R S Wilson PhD), Department of Neurological Sciences (D A Bennett, J A Schneider, R S Wilson), Department of Pathology (J A Schneider), Department of Behavioral Science (R S Wilson), and Rush Institute for Healthy Aging and Department of Internal Medicine (Y Tang PhD), Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL, USA; and Center for Neurobiology and Behavior, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA (S E Arnold MD) Correspondence to: Dr David A Bennett, Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, 600 S Paulina, Suite 1028, Chicago, IL 60612, USA dbennett@rush.edu 406 Summary Background Few data are available about how social networks reduce the risk of cognitive impairment in old age. We aimed to measure this effect using data from a large, longitudinal, epidemiological clinicopathological study. Methods 89 elderly people without known dementia participating in the Rush Memory and Aging Project underwent annual clinical evaluation. Brain autopsy was done at the time of death. Social network data were obtained by structured interview. Cognitive function tests were Z scored and averaged to yield a global and specific measure of cognitive function. Alzheimer’s disease pathology was quantified as a global measure based on modified Bielschowsky silver stain. Amyloid load and the density of paired helical filament tau tangles were also quantified with antibodyspecific immunostains. We used linear regression to examine the relation of disease pathology scores and social networks to level of cognitive function. Findings Cognitive function was inversely related to all measures of disease pathology, indicating lower function at more severe levels of pathology. Social network size modified the association between pathology and cognitive function (parameter estimate 0·097, SE 0·039, p=0·016, R²=0·295). Even at more severe levels of global disease pathology, cognitive function remained higher for participants with larger network sizes. A similar modifying association was observed with tangles (parameter estimate 0·011, SE 0·003, p=0·001, R²=0·454). These modifying effects were most pronounced for semantic memory and working memory. Amyloid load did not modify the relation between pathology and network size. The results were unchanged after controlling for cognitive, physical, and social activities, depressive symptoms, or number of chronic diseases. Interpretation These findings suggest that social networks modify the relation of some measures of Alzheimer’s disease pathology to level of cognitive function. Introduction Several clinicopathological studies over the past two decades have shown that many elderly people with extensive pathology of Alzheimer’s disease do not clinically manifest cognitive impairment.1–4 This ability to tolerate the pathology of this disease without obvious clinical consequences is increasingly referred to as cognitive or neural reserve.1,5 Identification of factors associated with neural reserve has important implications for disease prevention. For example, one such factor is education. Clinicopathological studies suggest that the relation between quantitative measures of Alzheimer’s disease pathology and level of cognition differ by duration of formal education.6 Another potential factor that could modify this relation is social networks. Social networks have been related to a reduced risk of death and a reduction in a wide variety of adverse health outcomes in old people.7 Several studies have also examined the relation between the extent of social ties and cognitive function and dementia. Most,8–10 but not all,11 showed that people with more extensive social networks were at reduced risk of cognitive impairment. Little is known about the cellular, molecular, and neuropathology of social networks and potential neurobiological mechanisms underlying this association. Although social networks could be directly related to the accumulation of Alzheimer’s disease pathology, it seems more likely that social network size is related to reserve capacity capable of reducing the likelihood that the disease pathology will be clinically expressed as cognitive impairment. We aimed to test this hypothesis using data from the Rush Memory and Aging Project—a large, longitudinal, epidemiological, clinicopathological study of ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Methods Participants and procedures Participants were elderly people without known dementia in the Rush Memory and Aging Project12 (see acknowledgments). Each participant gave written informed consent and an anatomical gift act for brain donation. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Rush University Medical Center. More than 1100 people have agreed to participate and have completed their baseline clinical assessment. The overall annual follow-up rate of survivors exceeds 90%, and the autopsy rate exceeds 75%. Post-mortem data were available for analysis from the first 89 people. All participants underwent a uniform structured clinical assessment that included a medical history, neurological examination, and neuropsychological performance http://neurology.thelancet.com Vol 5 May 2006 Articles testing. Neuropsychological test results were reviewed by a board-certified neuropsychologist who gave an opinion about the presence and severity of cognitive impairment. Each participant was assessed in person by a physician. On the basis of this evaluation, and review of the cognitive testing and the neuropsychologist’s opinion, participants were classified with respect to Alzheimer’s disease and other common conditions with the potential to affect cognitive function, according to the recommendations of the joint working group of the National Institute of Neurologic and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS/ADRDA)13 as previously described.12 Annual follow-up evaluations were identical in all essential details and were done by examiners unaware of previously obtained data. At the time of death, all available clinical data were reviewed and a summary diagnostic opinion was given as to the most likely clinical diagnosis at death. Summary diagnoses were made by reviewers unaware of all post-mortem data. 21 cognitive performance tests were administered each year. Details of the cognitive function tests have been previously reported.12,14 Briefly, one test, the mini-mental state examination (MMSE), was used to describe the cohort but was not used in analyses. A second test, complex ideational material, was used for diagnostic classification but was not used in the composite measure of cognition. The remaining 19 tests were used to assess five domains of cognitive function. There were seven tests of episodic memory including immediate and delayed recall of story A from logical memory and of the east Boston story, and word list memory, recall, and recognition. Three measures assessed semantic memory including a 15-item version of the Boston naming test, verbal fluency, and a 15-item reading test. There were three tests of working memory including digit span forward and backward and digit ordering. There were four tests of perceptual speed, including symbol digit modalities test, number comparison, and two indices from a modified version of the Stroop neuropsychological screening test. Finally, there were two tests of visuospatial ability, including a 15-item version of judgment of line orientation, and a 16-item version of the Raven’s standard progressive matrices. The primary outcome measure in the study was a global measure of cognitive function. We focused on the continuous measure of cognition, rather than a dichotomous variable of dementia or Alzheimer’s disease, because doing so allowed us to fully examine the spectrum of both pathology and cognition, and their association with social networks, in the most direct way and with the greatest statistical power. Furthermore, because factors affecting neural reserve are likely to affect some cognitive systems more than others, we did a series of secondary analyses to explore five different cognitive abilities. This approach is identical to that we have taken in similar analyses with data from another study.6,15 The raw scores http://neurology.thelancet.com Vol 5 May 2006 from the 19 tests were converted to Z scores and averaged to yield a global cognitive summary, and measures of five different cognitive abilities as previously described.12,14 We quantified social network size with three sets of standard questions about the number of children, family, and friends of each participant and how often they interacted with them.10 Because we wanted to measure the influence of premorbid social networks on the relation between pathology and cognition, we restricted the analyses to social network data from the baseline assessment. Participants were asked about the number of children they have and see monthly. They were asked about the number of relatives (besides spouse and children) and other close friends to whom they feel close and with whom they felt at ease and could talk to about private matters and could call upon for help, and how many of these people they see monthly. Social network size was the number of these individuals seen at least once per month.10 We also assessed five potential mediators and covariates that could account for or confound the association of social networks with cognition, as previously reported. We only used data from the baseline evaluation to be concurrent with the assessment of social networks. We assessed current participation in nine cognitively stimulating activities,14 five physical activities,12 and six social activities.10 Depressive symptoms were assessed with a ten-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies depression scale.12 We measured seven chronic diseases—diabetes, hypertension, heart disease, cancer, thyroid disease, head injury, and stroke.12 Brains of deceased participants were removed, weighed, cut into 1-cm-thick coronal slabs, and immersion fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 72 h. Tissue blocks from the mid-frontal gyrus, the superior temporal gyrus, the inferior parietal gyrus, the entorhinal cortex proper, and the hippocampus (CA1/subiculum) were embedded in paraffin, sectioned at 6 µm and stained with a modified Bielschowsky silver stain. Neuritic plaques, diffuse plaques, and neurofibrillary tangles were counted in the region that appeared to have the maximum density of each pathological index as previously described, resulting in 15 measures.16 A composite measure of global Alzheimer’s disease pathology was created as previously described by dividing each raw count by the standard deviation of the mean for the same neuropathological index in that region and averaging the scaled scores to yield the composite measures.16 Multiple tissue blocks from entorhinal cortex proper, hippocampus (CA1/subiculum), superior frontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, inferior temporal cortex, angular gyrus cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and calcarine cortex were embedded in paraffin and cut into 20-µm sections. Up to 24 sections were available for each case for each post-mortem index. Amyloid-β was labelled with an N-terminus directed monoclonal antibody (10D5, courtesy Elan Pharmaceuticals; 1:1000). Immunohistochemistry was done as previously described17 407 Articles Baseline Proximate to death Age (years) 84·3 (5·6) 87·2 (5·9) Female 55·1% .. White non-Hispanic 94·4% .. Education (years) 14·4 (3·3) .. 6·9 (4·9) .. with diaminobenzidine as the reporter with 2·5% nickel sulphate to enhance immunoreaction product contrast. All sections were run with identical incubation times on an automated immunohistochemical stainer (Biogenex, San Ramon, CA, USA) in precisely timed runs. Paired helical filament (PHF) tau was labelled with an antibody specific for phosphorylated tau, AT8 (Innogenetics, San Ramon, CA, 1:1000). Positive and negative (no primary antibody) control sections were included in all runs. A systematic random sampling scheme was used to capture video images of amyloid-β stained sections for quantitative analysis of amyloid deposition.17 The region of interest was outlined at low power with StereoInvestigator software version 6 (MicroBrightfield, Colchester, VT, USA) and an Olympus BX-51 microscope with an attached motorised stage. A grid of predetermined size was randomly placed over the outlined area to sample about 50% of the region of interest. After camera and illumination calibration, the magnification was raised to ×200 and 24-bit colour images obtained at each sampling site with a motorised stage employed to automatically position the tissue before each capture. We quantified amyloid-β load by image processing in an automated, multistage computational image analysis protocol algorithm, as previously described,18 with the addition of a defective pixel removal procedure to exclude faulty pixels. Mean fraction (% area) per region and per person were computed. Quality of segmentation was controlled by visual inspection on a randomly selected subset of images. Values for all regions were averaged to yield a composite measure of amyloid-β deposition. Quantification of tangle density per mm² was done with the stereological mapping station described above. Briefly, after the region was delimited at low power, a grid Demographic Social networks (number) Mediators and covariates Cognitive activity 3·4 (1·0) .. Physical activity 2·1 (2·6) .. Social activity 2·3 (0·7) .. Depressive symptoms 1·5 (1·8) .. Chronic diseases 1·7 (1·2) .. Cognitive function MMSE 25·8 (4·7) 24·0 (7·6) Global cognition –0·28 (0·82) –0·48 (0·98) Episodic memory –0·42 (1·01) –0·57 (1·10) Semantic memory –0·12 (0·77) –0·44 (1·12) Working memory –0·04 (0·93) –0·31 (1·24) Perceptual speed –0·32 (1·11) –0·75 (1·21) Visuospatial ability –0·08 (0·94) –0·24 (1·07) Pathological Global AD pathology .. Amyloid load .. 0·70 (0·63) 3·60 (4·08) Neurofibrillary tangles .. 5·58 (7·02) Data are mean (SD) unless otherwise indicated. MMSE=mini-mental state examination. AD=Alzheimer’s disease. Table 1: Selected clinical characteristics at baseline, and clinical and pathological characteristics at the last assessment before death for participants in the Rush Memory and Aging Project Main effects Estimate (SE) With interaction term p Estimate (SE) p Model 1 (disease pathology) Intercept Global disease pathology Social networks 0·034 (0·203) 0·868 0·256 (0·229) 0·267 –0·581 (0·142) 0·0001 –1·242 (0·301) 0·0001 0·012 (0·018) 0·519 –0·026 (0·023) 0·273 0·097 (0·039) 0·016 Social networks × global disease pathology Model 2 (amyloid) Intercept –0·290 (0·202) 0·154 –0·133 (0·221) 0·550 Amyloid load –0·052 (0·025) 0·039 –0·119 (0·047) 0·015 0·015 (0·019) 0·439 –0·005 (0·023) 0·815 0·010 (0·006) 0·104 Social networks Social networks × amyloid load Model 3 (tangles) Intercept Neurofibrillary tangles Social networks Social networks × neurofibrillary tangles 0·014 (0·177) 0·936 0·289 (0·186) 0·124 –0·070 (0·011) <0·0001 –0·140 (0·023) <0·0001 0·008 (0·016) 0·642 –0·032 (0·020) 0·104 0·011 (0·003) 0·001 All models controlled for age, sex, and education. Table 2: Global cognition as a function of social networks, three different pathological indices, and interaction between each pathological index and social networks 408 http://neurology.thelancet.com Vol 5 May 2006 Articles Statistical analysis Multiple linear regression was used to examine the extent to which social network size was related to each of the three measures of Alzheimer’s disease pathology. We then constructed a linear regression model that examined global cognition as a function of the global measure of disease pathology and social networks. We then made a second model which included the main effects for disease pathology and social networks and also included an interaction term. The interaction term directly tests the hypothesis that the number of people in the social network modifies the effect of a unit of pathology on level of cognitive function. To see if the effects were present for one type of pathology but not the other, we repeated these models for amyloid load and PHFtau tangles. To determine whether the association was due to cognitive, physical, or social activities, depression, or chronic diseases, we repeated all three sets of models controlling for these covariates. To determine whether social networks modified the relation of pathology to some cognitive abilities but not others, we subsequently examined the relation of all three post-mortem indices to separate summary measures of five different cognitive abilities. All models were adjusted for age, sex, and education, and were validated graphically and analytically. Analyses were done with SAS/STAT software version 8 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA) on a SunUltraSparc workstation.19 Role of the funding source The sponsors of the study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report. The corresponding author had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication. Results Participants were about 81 years of age, had about 14 years of education, and were predominantly white, nonHispanic (table 1). Mean MMSE was nearly 26 at baseline. Global cognitive function and other cognitive scores at baseline ranged from close to the mean for the entire cohort at baseline for working memory to nearly half a http://neurology.thelancet.com Vol 5 May 2006 2 High social networks Low social networks 1 0 –1 –2 –3 –4 Global cognition of predetermined size was randomly placed over the entire region by the software program. Total magnification was raised to ×400 and the program was engaged to direct the motorised stage on the microscope to stop at each intersection point of the grid for sampling. The operator focused on each field visualised on the video monitor within the superimposed counting frame. All objects within the 150×150 µm counting frame that did not touch the exclusion lines of the box (bottom and left sides) were counted. Neurofibrillary tangles labeled with AT8 have a characteristic appearance and location in the neuronal cell body or as ghost tangles. The density of tangles (per mm²) was averaged within region and subsequently across regions. –5 0 0·5 1·0 1·5 Global pathology 2·0 2·5 2 High social networks Low social networks 1 0 –1 –2 –3 –4 –5 0 10 20 30 40 Neurofibrillary tangles (PHFtau) Figure 1: Predicted association between pathology and global cognitive function score proximate to death Upper=global Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Lower=PHFtau tangles. Red line=90th percentile of social network size (13 participants). Blue line=10th percentile of social network size (two participants). Dotted lines indicate 95% CIs. Both models controlled for age, sex, education, and main effects for social networks and each pathological index. standard unit below the mean for episodic memory. At the last assessment before death, age was just over 87 years, mean MMSE score was 24, and cognitive scores ranged from about a third to three quarters of a standard unit below the mean for the entire cohort at baseline. Overall, these data suggest that participants had very good cognitive scores at baseline and, on average, experienced substantial cognitive decline over the study period. The average social network size of seven is similar to that reported in other community-based studies of old people.10 Social networks were related to social activity (r=0·36, p<0·0001) and cognitive activity (0·23, p=0·031), but not to physical activity (–0·09, p=0·43), depression (0·01, p=0·90), chronic disease (0·12, p=0·273), or income (0·11, p=0·53). Social networks were not related to the global Alzheimer’s disease pathology score (parameter estimate=–0·073, p=0·50), or to measures of amyloid-β load (–0·062, p=0·57) or the density of PHFtau 409 Articles Episodic memory Estimate (SE) p Semantic memory Working memory Perceptual speed Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE) p p Visuospatial ability p Estimate (SE) p Model 1 Intercept 0·201 (0·266) 0·452 0·272 (0·284) 0·340 0·343 (0·323) 0·292 –0·260 (0·327) 0·429 0·241 (0·305) 0·432 Social network –0·006 (0·027) 0·836 –0·035 (0·028) 0·206 –0·018 (0·033) 0·582 –0·015 (0·033) 0·646 –0·043 (0·029) 0·142 Global disease pathology –1·240 (0·349) 0·0007 –1·236 (0·366) 0·001 –0·894 (0·425) 0·039 –1·156 (0·429) 0·009 –0·628 (0·386) 0·109 Social network × disease pathology 0·065 (0·046) 0·156 0·116 (0·047) 0·016 0·054 (0·056) 0·333 0·087 (0·056) 0·121 0·061 (0·049) 0·215 –0·189 (0·257) 0·466 –0·090 (0·257) 0·728 –0·037 (0·294) 0·899 –0·695 (0·313) 0·029 –0·059 (0·264) 0·824 0·007 (0·027) 0·800 –0·012 (0·026) 0·637 0·009 (0·030) 0·780 0·006 (0·031) 0·846 –0·026 (0·026) 0·329 –0·136 (0·055) 0·016 –0·124 (0·054) 0·024 –0·051 (0·063) 0·422 –0·077 (0·065) 0·239 –0·024 (0·055) 0·663 0·009 (0·007) 0·242 0·013 (0·007) 0·081 –0·002 (0·008) 0·842 0·008 (0·009) 0·378 0·005 (0·007) 0·495 Model 2 Intercept Social network Amyloid Social network × amyloid Model 3 Intercept 0·271 (0·212) 0·204 0·372 (0·242) 0·128 0·526 (0·282) 0·065 –0·211 (0·282) 0·458 0·232 (0·282) 0·413 Social network –0·017 (0·022) 0·438 –0·049 (0·025) 0·050 –0·053 (0·030) 0·079 –0·021 (0·029) 0·481 –0·047 (0·028) 0·096 Tangles –0·148 (0·027) <0·0001 –0·154 (0·030) <0·0001 –0·146 (0·036) 0·0001 –0·132 (0·035) 0·0003 –0·070 (0·036) 0·057 0·009 (0·004) 0·023 0·015 (0·004) 0·0005 0·015 (0·005) 0·005 0·010 (0·005) 0·056 0·008 (0·005) 0·114 Social network × tangles All models controlled for age, sex, and education. Table 3: Global cognition before death in different domains as a function of interaction of social networks tangles (–0·088, p=0·42) in linear regression models controlling for age, sex, and education. In an analysis of global cognition as a function of Alzheimer’s disease pathology and social networks, the average global cognitive score was about 0·6 (p=0·0001) of a unit lower for each unit of global disease pathology, but social networks were not related to cognition; the adjusted R² for the model was 0·250 (table 2, main effect in model 1). We then repeated the analysis with a term added for the interaction between pathology and social networks. There was an interaction, and the adjusted R² for the model increased to 0·295. To illustrate this effect, we plotted the predicted relation between the global measure of disease pathology and the global cognitive score for participants with two different social network sizes: the 90th percentile (13 members) and the 10th percentile (two members; figure 1). The change in cognitive function across disease pathologies was more pronounced for people with smaller social network sizes than for those with larger ones. We did a similar series of analyses with amyloid-β load and PHFtau tangles. Corresponding to each one percent increase in amyloid, the average cognitive function score was 0·05 units lower (p=0·039; table 2). The interaction between amyloid and social networks was not significant. In separate analyses, the average cognitive function score was about 0·07 (p<0·0001) standard unit lower for each neurofibrillary tangle (per mm²) with an adjusted R² of 0·385 (table 2). The interaction between tangles and social networks was significant and R² increased to 0·454. To illustrate this effect, we plotted the predicted relation between tangle density and the global cognitive score for participants with two different social network sizes (figure 1). Again the divergence of the lines suggests 410 a protective effect of social network size on the association of tangles with cognition. We next repeated all three sets of models five times adding terms for cognitive, physical, and social activities, depressive symptoms, and chronic medical conditions to ensure that these variables did not confound or mediate the association of social networks with cognition. The results were essentially unchanged when examined separately or all five together. We also constructed a model that included a term for the interaction of education with measures of disease pathology and the modifying effects of social networks were unchanged. Memory and other forms of cognition are not unitary processes but are composed of dissociable systems that mediate different types of information processing. To see if social networks modified the relation between measures of disease pathology to some forms of cognition but not others, we undertook three sets of analyses, one for each pathological measure, separately for five different cognitive domains assessed proximate to death, namely episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, perceptual speed, and visuopsatial ability (table 3). Social networks modified the association of neurofibrillary tangles with episodic memory, semantic memory, and working memory. The effect for semantic memory was especially striking, with the adjusted R² for the model increasing from 0·210 to 0·318 with the addition of the interaction term. Social networks also modified the relation of global Alzheimer’s disease pathology with semantic memory. Discussion We found that the extent of social networks modified the relation between some measures of Alzheimer’s disease http://neurology.thelancet.com Vol 5 May 2006 Articles pathology and level of cognitive function assessed proximate to death. The effect was evident with several measures of pathology acquired with different methodologies, but was strongest for neurofibrillary tangles. The effect persisted after controlling for various potentially confounding variables. It was evident across multiple domains of cognition, but was most evident for semantic memory, which is the repository of knowledge about the world and is fundamentally involved in unique human cognitive processes such as language. These data provide evidence that the extent of social networks, or something related to social networks, provides some type of reserve which reduces the deleterious effect of Alzheimer’s disease pathology on cognitive abilities in old age. In most physiological systems, considerable tissue destruction must take place for function to be compromised and signs and symptoms of disease to become evident. Although structurally and functionally complex, the nervous system can also tolerate injury and pathology without expressing it clinically as functional impairment. Some type of reserve has likewise been hypothesised to protect the human brain from expressing the pathology of Alzheimer’s disease as impaired cognitive function.1 Clinical pathological studies report that education1,6 or variables related to education20 can protect the brain from the pathology of Alzheimer’s disease. To modify the relation between pathology and cognition, social networks must reflect either the strength of the neural systems underlying neurocognition or tap into systems that support other cognitive processes to reduce the deleterious effect of disease pathology. We are unaware of any study that has yet examined the relation between social networks and Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Data about the relation of social networks to cognition come from epidemiological studies limited to clinical measures.8–11 Although the basis for this association is unknown, most research has emphasised the potential benefits for an individual of having a large social network.21 Even though people with larger social networks are more likely to engage in cognitive, physical, and social activities, all of which are associated with decreased risk of cognitive impairment and dementia,10,22–24 our findings were unchanged after controlling for participation in these activities. Although those with larger social networks might be less depressed, which is also associated with cognitive impairment and dementia,25 controlling for depression had only a marginal effect on our findings. Finally, controlling for comorbid conditions did not alter our results. The importance of social connectedness has been recognised since antiquity. However, not all individuals are equally capable of developing and maintaining friendships and social ties. For example, several neurodevelopmental disorders are characterised, in part, by an impaired ability to develop social ties, including the autism spectrum disorders, fragile X syndrome, and http://neurology.thelancet.com Vol 5 May 2006 schizophrenia.26 People with these conditions have deficits in social cognition. Social cognition is supported by an extensive system of limbic and associational cortical and subcortical brain regions.26,27 Many of these regions also support episodic memory, semantic memory, and other cognitive functions. These systems allow us to make symbolic representations of the characteristics of self and non-self, thoughts and feelings, and other aspects of the social environment, and endow us with the capability of viewing ourselves from the perspective of another person (so-called theory of mind). Focal brain lesions, including strokes, can impair aspects of social behaviour while leaving other cognitive abilities relatively intact.28 Neurodegenerative diseases, including Parkinson’s disease, frontotemporal dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease are known to be associated with impaired aspects of social behaviour.29,30 Thus, it is possible that aspects of cognitive processing that allow people to develop and maintain large social networks might also provide a reserve against the development of cognitive impairment despite the accumulation of Alzheimer’s disease pathology, or otherwise compensate for the effects of degeneration of non-social cognitive systems. Recruitment of alternative brain regions in response to injury due to ageing and neurodegenerative diseases has been well documented in neuroimaging studies. For example, when undertaking a cognitive task, ageing is associated with increased activation in regions not activated by younger people.31 This pattern is thought to reflect compensation for age-related damage by activating alternative neural networks.32 Old people with mild Alzheimer’s disease also activate additional brain regions to do a cognitive task at a similar level compared with those without the disease.33 The observations that alternative networks are activated in response to brain damage rather than being on-line and in use at all times would explain the absence of a main effect of social networks on cognitive function but with the robust modifying effect becoming evident as Alzheimer’s disease pathology accumulates. Our study has several strengths. The ability to link social networks to several measures of disease pathology and to multiple cognitive domains assessed proximate to death in a single study offers a unique integrative approach to hypothesis testing. All analyses were on people from a single cohort with high rates of follow-up and brain autopsy. Uniform structured procedures were followed with blinding to previously collected data and blinding of personnel collecting post-mortem data to clinical data. The study also has limitations. Although the associations are correlative and causation cannot be proven with complete confidence, some hypotheses are not amenable to clinical trials. Although we controlled for many potential confounders, we did not examine the quality of the networks nor did we have information about networks early in life.34,35 411 Articles Contributors DAB obtained funding, and was involved in study design, data collection, statistical analysis and interpretation, and writing and editing of manuscript. JAS, SEA, and RSW were involved in design of data collection, interpretation of analyses, and critical editing of manuscript. YT was involved in statistical analysis and interpretation and critical editing of manuscript. All authors had full access to all study data and take full responsibility for the integrity of the data. Conflicts of interest We have no conflicts of interest. Acknowledgments We are indebted to the residents from the following groups participating in the Rush Memory and Aging Project: Fairview Village, Wyndemere, Luther Village, The Holmstad, Windsor Park Manor, Covenant Village, Bethlehem Woods, King-Bruwaert House, Friendship Village, Mayslake Village, The Moorings, Washington Jane Smith, Victory Lakes, Village Woods, Franciscan Village, Victorian Village, The Breakers of Edgewater, The Oaks, St. Paul Home, The Imperial, Frances Manor, Peace Village, Alden Waterford, Marian Village, The Birches, Elgin Housing Authority, Renaissance, Holland Home, Trinity United Church Of Christ, St. Andrews-Phoenix, Green Castle, Kingston Manor, Lawrence Manor, Community Renewal-Senior Ministry, Garden House, and the residents of the Chicago metropolitan area. We thank the staff of the Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center and Institute for Healthy Aging. This research was supported by National Institute on Aging Grant R01AG17917. References 1 Katzman R, Terry R, DeTeresa R, et al. Clinical, pathological, and neurochemical changes in dementia: a subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques. Ann Neurol 1988; 23: 138–441. 2 Crystal HA, Dickson DW, Sliwinski MJ, et al. Pathological markers associated with normal aging and dementia in the elderly. Ann Neurol 1993; 34: 566–73. 3 Riley KP, Snowdon DA, Markesbery WR. Alzheimer’s neurofibrillary pathology and the spectrum of cognitive function: findings from the Nun Study. Ann Neurol 2002; 51: 567–77. 4 Knopman DS, Parisi JE, Salviati A, et al. Neuropathology of cognitively normal elderly. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2003; 62: 1087–95. 5 Scarmeas N, Stern Y. Cognitive reserve: implications for diagnosis and prevention of Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2004; 4: 374–80. 6 Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to cognitive function in older persons. Neurology 2003; 60: 1909–15. 7 Berkman LF, Syme SL. Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: a nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. Am J Epidemiol 1979; 109: 186–204. 8 Bassuk SS, Glass TA, Berkman LF. Social disengagement and incident cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly persons. Ann Intern Med 1999; 131: 165–73. 9 Fratiglioni L, Wang HX, Ericsson K, Maytan M, Winblad B. Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study. Lancet 2000; 355: 1315–19. 10 Barnes LL, Mendes de Leon CF, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Evans DA. Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology 2004; 63: 2322–26. 11 Seeman TE, Lusignolo TM, Albert M, Berkman L. Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Health Psychol 2001; 20: 243–55. 12 Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, Mendes de Leon CF, Bienias JL, Wilson RS. The Rush Memory and Aging Project: study design and baseline characteristics of the study cohort. Neuroepidemiology 2005; 25: 163–75. 13 McKhann G, Drachmann D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan E. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984; 34: 939. 412 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Kreuger KR, Hoganson G, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Early and late life cognitive activity and cognitive systems in old age. JINS 2005; 11: 400–07. Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Berry-Kravis E, Arnold SE. Amyloid mediates the association of apolipoprotein E ε4 to level of cognitive function in older persons. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005; 76: 1194–99. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, et al. Apolipoprotein E ε4 allele, Alzheimer’s disease pathology, and the clinical expression of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2003; 60: 246–52. Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Arnold SE. Neurofibrillary tangles mediate the association of amyloid load and with clinical Alzheimer’s disease and level of cognitive function. Arch Neurol 2004; 61: 378–84. Mitchell TW, Nissanov J, Han LY, et al. Novel method to quantify neuropil threads in brains from elders with or without cognitive impairment. J Histochem Cytochem 2000; 48: 1627–38. SAS Institute. SAS/STAT users guide, version 8. Cary, NC: SAS Institute. 2000. Mortimer JA, Borenstein AR, Gosche KM, Snowdon DA. Very early detection of Alzheimer neuropathology and the role of brain reserve in modifying its clinical expression. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2005; 18: 218–23. Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol 2004; 3: 343–53. Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Barnes LL, et al. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA 2002; 287: 742–48. Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA 2004; 12: 1454–61. Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med 2003; 348: 2508–16. Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Gore R, Sands L, Reus V, Browner WS. Depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in nondemented elderly women: a prospective study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56: 425–30. Grady CL, Keightley ML. Studies of altered social cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders using functional neuroimaging. Can J Psychiatry 2002; 47: 327–36. Adolphs R. The neurobiology of social cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2001; 11: 231–39. Bird CM, Castelli F, Malik O, Frith U, Husain M. The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on ‘Theory of Mind’ and cognition. Brain 2004; 127: 914–28. Saltzman J, Strauss E, Hunter M, Archibald S. Theory of mind and executive functions in normal human aging and Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2000; 6: 781–88. Gregory C, Lough S, Stone V, et al. Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: theoretical and practical implications. Brain 2002; 125: 752–64. Stern Y, Habeck C, Moeller J, et al. Brain networks associated with cognitive reserve in healthy young and old adults. Cereb Cortex 2005; 15: 394–402. Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002; 8: 448–60. Grady CL, Furey ML, Pietrini P, Horwitz B, Rapoport SI. Altered brain functional connectivity and impaired short-term memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2001; 124: 739–56. Berkman LF, Glass T, Brissette I, Seeman TE. From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. Soc Sci Med 2000; 51: 843–57. Seeman TE, Crimmins E. Social environment effects on health and aging: integrating epidemiologic and demographic approaches and perspectives. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2001; 954: 88–117. http://neurology.thelancet.com Vol 5 May 2006