On the learnability of saltatory phonological alternations James White Department of Linguistics, UCLA jameswhite@ucla.edu 1. Background and research questions 2. Case study: Saltatory alternations • • Saltatory alternation: An alternation in which an intermediate non-alternating sound is “jumped over.” • E.g. Campidanian Sardinian:[6] How do people learn the phonological alternations of their language? • o Is phonological learning the result of an unbiased search for patterns?[1] o Or is learning guided by biases against certain patterns? o What types of patterns are dispreferred by learners? o What is the nature of these biases? o How can we account for them in learning models? • /pani/ [sːu βãi] ‘the bread’ /binu/ [sːu bĩu] ‘the wine’ 2 feature changes p b β -continuant -continuant +continuant -voice +voice +voice +labial +labial +labial 1 feature 1 feature difference difference Why are they interesting? Acknowledgments o Relatively uncommon. o It has long been argued that alternations between more similar sounds are Much of the recent literature supports a soft bias approach: certain patterns are dispreferred, but may be learned given enough input.[2-5] better than alternations between less similar.[7-8] o Accounting for them is not theoretically straightforward (e.g., classical OT[9] cannot). I would like to thank Bruce Hayes, Megha Sundara, Robert Daland, Kie Zuraw, Sharon Peperkamp, Marc Garellek, Karen Campbell, Adam Albright, Jennifer Cole, audiences at the UCLA phonology seminar and LSA, and anonymous LabPhon reviewers for helpful comments on this project. I would also like to thank undergraduate research assistants who have worked on parts of this project: Kelly Ryan, Kelly Nakawatase, Ariel Quist, Emily Ho, Sarah Yiu, Ben Longwill, and Maria Manukyan. Any remaining errors are my own. This work was funded in part by a UCLA Summer mentorship fellowship. 3. The experiments: Are learners biased against saltatory alternations? Results in generalization phase Method Artificial language learning paradigm --Repeat Exposure/Verification until 80% accuracy-- • Participants: 20 UCLA undergrads per experiment. o Training input summary: • Exposure: Learn alternations by hearing pairs of nonwords with a singular and plural picture. o Exp 1: Ambiguous p f ... [kamap] [kamavi] How can we account for the dispreferred status, but ultimate learnability, of saltatory alternations? • I use Maximum entropy grammar models.[10] • Implemented using the Maxent Grammar Tool (from Bruce Hayes’s webpage). • Provide model: • • • v f b p v f p v b f v Also included: analogous coronals [t, d, θ, ð]. 5. The models I. Traditional faithfulness[9], Unbiased Faithfulness constraints: IDENT(voice), IDENT(cont), IDENT(son) For each input form, a set of candidates and the observed probability of each candidate winning Flat prior: For all constraints, µ = 0, σ2 = 0.5 o A set of constraints and violations Exp 2 60 40 20 0 • Untrained sounds: Ps change intermediate sounds when ambiguously saltatory (Exp 1) much more than they do untrained non-intermediate sounds (Exp 2). = dispreference for saltatory alternations! Faithfulness constraints: More expressive *MAP constraints[11] for every pair of sounds Overall r2 = .51 Exp 1 80 = how tightly a constraint weight is constrained to its µ during learning 60 40 40 40 Training data: Same as experimental participants received. 0 o Constraint weights o For each input form, the predicted probability of each candidate Same markedness constraints for all models: o *V[-voice]V = penalize intervocalic voiceless sounds o *V[-continuant]V = penalizes intervocalic stops Language learners disprefer saltatory alternations, but they are not unlearnable. A MaxEnt grammar model can account for both of these observations, provided that: o o It has a more expressive set of constraints (such as the *MAP variety). It has a similarity bias, derived from confusion matrix data, preferring alternations between similar sounds. 20 20 100 f, θ ✗ Predicts generalization to untrained sounds in Exp 2 Fillers b, d f, θ Exp 2 p, t 100 100 80 80 60 60 40 40 40 20 20 0 0 Exp 3 p, t f, θ 100 80 80 60 60 Problem: the constraint set doesn’t allow saltation 40 20 0 p, t Fillers b, d Fillers p, t f, θ ✓ Able to learn the saltation! ✗ But doesn’t predict that it is harder to learn. Treated just like fillers. 40 f, θ ✓ No overgeneralization in Exp 2! b, d Fillers p, t f, θ ✓ Able to learn the saltation, but still some errors relative to fillers (just like participants)! Exp 3 100 80 60 40 20 20 0 0 f, θ b, d 0 Exp 3 100 Fillers 20 b, d ✗ Totally unable to learn the saltation ✓ Difference in stops and fricatives! Exp 2 60 Fillers Strikingly good fit, with no manipulation of weights by hand! 0 p, t 80 b, d Intermediate frics 20 0 Exp 2 Intermediate stops Participants learn saltatory alternations, but they are harder to learn—they continue to make errors that they don’t make for fillers. Overall r2 = .95 100 Model output: Fillers Exp 1 60 b, d Trained stops Similarity bias prior – similar to Steriade’s P-map[8]: Step 1: Use confusion matrix probabilities[12] as input to a separate Maxent model with *MAP constraints. E.g., *MAP(p, v) = Don’t map underlying /p/ Result: *MAP constraints receive weights based on to surface [v] confusability; Constraints penalizing more confusable (i.e., more similar) sounds get lower weights. E.g., *MAP(b, v) has lower weight than *MAP(p, v). Flat prior: For all constraints, µ = 0, σ2 = 0.5 Step 2: These weights become preferred weights (µ) in the main model. For all constraints, σ2 = 0.5. ✗ No difference in stops vs. fricatives Fillers 20 Faithfulness constraints: *MAP constraints 80 p, t 40 III. *MAP constraints, Similarity bias 100 • Exp 4 60 Untrained stops Untrained frics 60 σ2 Exp 3 80 0 Fillers 80 µ = preferred weight for a constraint 100 Final sound of singular word Overall r2 = .48 Exp 1 Exp 1 100 • • Explicitly saltatory cases: some learning, but a lot more errors than on fillers, despite training (compare faded bars to Fillers). 80 II. *MAP constraints, Unbiased Exp Results Model Predictions Best Possible Fit (Model directly trained on experimental results) Experiments 3 & 4 • Preference for changing untrained stops more than fricatives. 100 Trained stops Equal number of non-alternating fillers (mostly sonorants) in training. E.g. [luman] [lumani] A set of input forms Prior (soft bias), 2 settings for each constraint: b • o 6. Conclusions • p o o • b Exp 4: Explicit fricative saltation Tested in Generalization phase on nonwords ending in [p, b, f] for each experiment. e.g. [kamap] ... [kamavi] -or- [kamapi] • Exp 3: Explicit stop saltation Exp 2: No saltation • o Verification: Tested on subset of exposure pairs. Participants choose between changing and non-changing • auditory plural options (order counterbalanced). 4. Modeling overview • Trained pattern: Good generalization to novel nonwords. % changing plural option chosen Procedure – 3 phases: • Experiments 1 & 2 Generalization: Same test on novel nonwords, including some ending in untrained sounds. % changing plural option chosen • p, t Fillers b, d f, θ p, t Fillers b, d f, θ 7. References {6] Bolognesi, R. (1998). The phonology of Campidanian Sardinian: A unitary account of self-organizing structure. Holland Academics. {1] Blevins, J. (2004). Evolutionary phonology: The emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge Univ. Press. [8] Steriade, D. (2001/2008). The phonology of perceptibility effects: the P-map and its consequences for constraint organization. [2] Hayes, B., et al. (2009). Natural and unnatural constraints in Hungarian vowel harmony. Language. [9] Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1993/2004). Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Blackwell. [3] Hayes, B., & White, J. (in press). Phonological naturalness and phonotactic learning. Linguistic Inquiry. [10] Hayes, B, & Wilson, C. 2008. A maximum entropy model of phonotactics and phonotactic learning. Linguistic Inquiry. [4] Wilson, C. (2006). Learning phonology with substantive bias: An experimental and computational study of velar palatalization. Cog. Sci. [11] Zuraw, K. (2007). The role of phonetic knowledge in phonological patterning: Corpus and survey evidence from Tagalog. Language. [5] Finley, S., & Badecker, W. (2008). Substantive biases for vowel harmony languages. WCCFL 27. [12] Wang, M. D., & Bilger, R. C. (1973). Consonant confusions in noise: a study of perceptual features. JASA. [7] Trubetzkoy, N. S. (1939). Principles of Phonology. . LabPhon 13