Does providing descriptive and/or explanatory information affect peer attitudes and behaviour intentions towards children with High Functioning Autism? Dr Ciara Close Rationale The social benefits of inclusive educational practices have been emphasized; however, there are still many unresolved issues, such as the degree of acceptance shown by peers towards a child with autism (Burack, Root, & Zigler, 1997). Pupils with ASD were found to be less accepted by their peers and had significantly higher social rejection ratings than comparison students when measured by peer sociometric ratings (Jones & Frederickson, 2010). Introducing the concept of autism to peers is an important component to successful inclusive education. However, there is a lack of research indicating how best this concept is delivered (Volkmar, 2001). Descriptive info– a written message describing common hobbies/interests of children ages 9-11years based on cognitive consistency theory, which posits that individuals are motivated to experience consistent attitudes (Heider, 1958). Explanatory info- a written message providing an explanation for HFA based on Weiner’s (1980) social attribution theory, which proposes an individual’s perceived control of his or her behaviour influences others’ affective responses (e.g. anger or sympathy), which, in turn, influence others’ behavioural intentions (e.g. willingness to help). Method 330 peers aged 9-11 years from 8 mainstream school (low levels of SEN) watched a 4 min video of a hypothetical child displaying a range of behaviour characteristics associated with HFA. Peers rated their attitudes, behaviour intentions (BI), perceptions of similarity and responsibility about the child using questionnaires. Peers were allocated to 1 of 4 groups and received different info: 1. No info 2. Descriptive info (des) 3. Explanatory info (exp) 4. Des + Exp info Results Peers did not report more positive attitudes or behaviour intentions when they were given combination of both des and exp information. Girls reported significantly more positive attitudes than boys, irrespective of information received and reported more willingness to engage in an academic related tasks with an unfamiliar peer than males. Boys who received explanatory information only, were significantly less willing to share in activities alongside an unfamiliar peer with HFA compared to boys in other groups. Girls reported themselves to be more similar than males to the child with HFA. Providing an explanation did not alter peer perceptions of responsibility or elicit positive attitudes towards the hypothetical peer with HFA. In fact, males attributed the child with HFA to be more responsible of his behaviour in all groups. Implications Providing an explanation of autism to children aged 9-11 years does not appear to elicit positive attitudes or BI from peers to help the social inclusion of a child with HFA. An explanation of autism did alter peers perception of responsibility e.g. peers still felt the child with HFA was in control/responsible for his behaviour even after a causal explanation was provided. Different approaches to intervention might need to be considered for girls and boys e.g. girls were willing to engage in academic related task with a child with HFA. Approaches to inclusion interventions might need to consider school population factors e.g. such as peers prior experiences of autism, diversity and inclusion. References Burack, J. A., Root, R., & Zigler, E. (1997). Inclusive education for students with autism: Reviewing ideological, empirical, and community considerations. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. New York, NY: Wiley & Sons. /Heider, F. (1958). The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. / Humphrey, N. & Parkinson, G. M. (2006). Research on interventions for children and young people on the autistic spectrum: a critical perspective. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 6, 76–86 ./ Jones, A.P. & Frederickson, N. (2010). Multi-Informant Predictors of Social Inclusion for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders Attending Mainstream School, Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 40, 1094–1103./ Volkmar, F. (2001). Editorial preface. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 31, 117–118. / Weiner, B. (1980). A cognitive attribution-emotion-action model of motivation behaviour: An analysis of judgments of helpgiving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 186–200.