The role of adversity in shaping children’s self-regulation: Drawing from experimental and nonexperimental evidence C. Cybele Raver Professor Dept. of Applied Psychology Director, Institute of Human Development and Social Change My Programs of Research • • • Measurement and models of poverty – Raver, Gershoff, & Aber (2007). Testing equivalence of mediating models of income, parenting, and school readiness for White, Black, and Hispanic children in a national sample. Child Development Children’s emotional and behavioral regulation as a predictor of school readiness – Raver (2004). Placing emotional self- regulation in sociocultural and socioeconomic contexts. Child Development. – Ursache, Blair, & Raver, (in press). The promotion of self-regulation as a means of enhancing school readiness and early achievement in children at risk for school failure. Child Development Perspectives. Adults’ mental health and caregiving – Raver (2003). Does work pay?: The effects of employment on depressive symptoms and parenting among low-income families. Child Development. – Zhai, Raver, & Li-Grining, (2011). Classroom-based interventions and teachers’ perceived job stressors and confidence: Evidence from a randomized trial in Head Start settings. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. Placing developmental models in context Theory Employment Psychological wellbeing Poverty Caregiving, instruction Child selfregulation Welfare Reform Emotional climate Policy & Prevention “bottom line” Social & academic competence HS School Overview • Self-regulation in the context of poverty • “Whirlwind tour” of the Chicago School Readiness Project (CSRP) • RCTs versus the ‘real world’ – lessons from longitudinal follow-up of CSRP • Implications for developmental science, prevention science, and for policy Child and Family Poverty in the U.S. In 2010, 18.8% of Pennsylvania children lived <= poverty line, up from 15.9% in 2007. Today, national child poverty rate = 21.2%. DeNavas-Walt, C., Proctor, B. D., & Smith, J. C. (2010). Income, Poverty, and Health Insurance Coverage in the United States: 2009. U.S. Census Bureau, Current Population Reports, P60-238,.Washington, DC.: U.S. Government Printing Office. Available at: http://www.census.gov/prod/2010pubs/p60-238.pdf Educational outcomes associated with child poverty • Poverty associated with children’s lower academic achievement, higher grade retention, and higher risk for dropping out. – Example: In Chicago, 86.50% of children eligible for free/reduced-priced lunch. – More than 25% of Chicago’s 24,000 third graders were not able to pass their school district’s gated proficiency standards, and subsequently faced high levels of long-term educational risk (Roderick & Nagaoka, 2005). • Low-income children face higher risk of behavioral problems, lower levels of competence in getting along with teachers and peers (Bolger et al, 1995) • Gap at school entry: Reading scores for kindergartners from lowest SES group are 60% lower than those of students at the highest end (Burkham & Lee, 2002). Is the solution to ask teachers to “teach more?” • Cumulative poverty-related stressors (e.g. instability, family/community violence) -> children’s higher risk of withdrawal, disruptive behavior. • Under-resourced classrooms in low-income communities are ill-equipped to cope with the emotional and behavioral needs of children facing high levels of risk (Dodge et al, 1999) – 16% of children in Head Start classrooms exhibited disruptive/unsafe behaviors, e.g. kicking, hitting and threatening at least once a day (Kuperschmidt, et al 2000). – Children’s disruptive behavior may limit not only their own but others’ opportunities for learning “Lenses” of developmental research Definitions • Emotional and behavioral self-regulation – temperament research – emotional “reactivity and regulation” + effortful/inhibitory control (Thompson, 1994; Eisenberg & Fabes, 1992) • Executive function – neurocognitive research – children’s working memory, attention deployment, and ability to inhibit prepotent impulses in order to meet external demands (Diamond & Taylor, 1996; Blair, 2002). Preschool– Children expected to modulate distress in context of highly arousing and potentially stressful situations (e.g. conflict over toys, teacher expectation of compliance). If interventions can be designed to support children’s self-regulation, will that improve their chances of school success? Evidence that children’s self-regulation is influenced by environment, malleable? EF and poverty as example: • Previous findings of SES gradient and EF (Hackman & Farah, 2010). • Strong support for model of experiential canalization, where poverty-rel. stress-> greater allostatic load ->alterations in HPA axis ->alterations in PFC functioning (Blair, 2010; Blair, Raver et al., 2011) • Recent work suggests may be particularly important to take into account chronicity of poverty, over time (Duncan & Magnuson, 2009; Raver, Blair, Willoughby & FLP Key investigators) Raver, Blair, Willoughby, & FLP Key Investigators (to be revised and resubmitted) Asking two questions: 1) Is self-regulation “malleable?” and 2) Does self-regulation “matter” for academic achievement? – Problems with inferring that low-resource environments shape children’s difficulties with self-regulation – Problems with inferring children’s emotional and behavioral problems as cause of academic difficulty: Perhaps children act out because they’re frustrated they can’t read. SR Inc Ach X3 – Solution: Random assignment of some preschool sites to receipt of “treatment” targeting children’s emotional and behavioral regulation and other sites to “control group” • Allows for empirical tests of causal claims. Integrative theoretical model of the role of children’s emotional and behavioral adjustment in predicting school readiness (Raver & Jones, 2003) Fall/ Sc hool entry PRE-K S CHOOL Y EA R Spring/Trans ition to Kindergarten Teac her instruction (language/ literacy) c hild language/ pre-literacy T2 child language/ pre-literacy T1 child emotiona l/ behav ioral adjustment T1 School Readines s T3 (e.g. Fall Kindergarten) Teac her classroom manage ment (provision of regulatory support) Cu mulative f amily/ neighborhood risk c hild emotiona l/ behav ioral adjustment T2 Acknowledgements PI: C. Cybele Raver Co-PI. Stephanie M. Jones, Co-Is: Christine Li-Grining, Fuhua Zhai Research Staff: Amy Lowenstein Julie Neuspeil Rachel Pess Nicole Tirado-Strayer Emily Pressler Kathleen Zadzora Genevieve Okada Dana Charles Allison Friedman Breeze Luetke-Stahlman, Ta-Tanisha Young, Tiffany Hayes, Latriese Sardin Adjei, Molly Metzger, Bonnie Solomon Chevon Townsend Radiah Smith-Donald Kina Smallwood + 20 grad student assessors Support: – W.T. Grant Foundation – Interagency Consortium on School Readiness, (NICHD) – Spencer Foundation – McCormick Tribune Foundation Intervention staff: Sybil Madison-Boyd Darlene Jones-Lewis, Kimya Barden Elysia Aufmuth Greg O’Donnell (Metro Family Svcs) Partnering Delegate Agencies: Department of Children Youth Services Chicago Public Schools Ounce of Prevention/Casa Central Hull House Humbolt Park Boys & Girls Club Carole Robertson Early Learning Centers CYC Rebecca Crown and Ida Mae Fletcher Centers South Shore United Methodist Center YMCA Hypotheses • The principal aim of CSRP is to improve low-income preschool-aged children’s school readiness. – Short-run: Test whether children in treatment-assigned classrooms show • significantly improved behavioral self-regulation, and • significantly greater gains in pre-academic skills than do children in control-assigned classrooms, in spring of Head Start year. Long-run: Does additional investment in preschool make substantive difference in young children’s chances of school success? • If accrued across treatment year, are gains sustained as children make transition into Chicago Public Schools? Overview of CSRP services Fall Winter Spring Teacher training + coaching Stress reduction 1-on-1 for children with highest EBPs Train teachers to respond in proactive ways to support children’s regulation and to limit disruptive behavior.(Webser-Stratton et al, 2001; Brotman et al, 2005, Kellam, et al, 1998) Paired teacher-training with “coaching” by MHC in light of a) concern for dosage b) need for stress reduction Direct “1-on-1” services by MHC. Children’s behavior problems - beyond the scope of teachers’ responsibility. Selecting 18 sites (35 classrooms): Balancing generalizability with feasibility • • Exclusion criteria Neighborhoods < 400 HS-eligible children • Demolition of CHA housing & gentrification – • Communities experiencing < median levels of crime. • 18 Sites, 90 teachers, 602 children enrolled in two cohorts • Rates of parental consent for child participation ranged from 66.6% to 100% across all sites, Mean = 91%, SD = 6% . Sample • 66% of families identified as African American/Black and the remaining families identified as Hispanic/Latina/o • Families enrolled in CSRP reported monthly incomes of $1,168 on average (average income-to-needs = .67), with 67% supporting their families as single heads of household. • Most parents were working, and many faced a range of serious poverty-related stressors. • Teachers were also supporting families on low incomes and 1/3rd of teachers reported feelings of high levels of stress and low confidence in managing children’s behaviors in the classroom. • Classroom quality at baseline was “adequate,” on average (Mean ECERS = 4.97), with substantial variability (SD = 0.77). Design Each Head Start site matched with another “sister” site on range of demographic characteristics of families, site characteristics indicating program capacity, etc. and then randomized to tx vs. control status. Treatment – “Package A” Control – “Package B” Services provided 30 hours of training offered. MHCs with a MSW spent 1 day a week in classroom, from Sept to May of school year. Teacher’s assistant (1 day1/wk) Sites 9 9 Classrm 18 18-1 NB: Originally funded for 10 sites– “hopelessly underpowered” (Cook, 2005). Boosted to 18 (expecting attrition of 1 pair) and included covariates. Best guess that ICCs were <.05 Data Collection • • • Site level – 14 characteristics (e.g. % of teachers with BA) from PIR Classroom level – CLASS (La Paro & Pianta, 2004) (Sept, Jan, Mar, May) Classroom climate, teacher sensitivity, etc. – Teacher psychosocial characteristics (K-6, job stressors) – Classroom-level Covariates • Classroom quality, ECERS, structure Child level – Family interview (Fall baseline, Fall 1-year-followup) – Teacher report of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems (Sept, Jan, March, May) – Direct assessment (Sept, May) – Kindergarten follow up (teacher report, school record). CSRP: Significant reductions in children’s externalizing and internalizing behavior problems Overall Gender Race ≥2 risks One risk No risk Other Hispanic Black Girl Boy CSRP Treatment Effects Figure 2. Effects of CSRP Treatment on Child BPI Externalizing Scores 1.0 0.0 -1.0 -2.0 -3.0 -4.0 -5.0 -6.0 -7.0 -8.0 -9.0 -10.0 -11.0 -12.0 -13.0 Family Risks Raver, Jones, Li-Grining, Zhai, Metzger & Solomon (2010). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology The Preschool Self-Regulation Assessment (PSRA) Smith-Donald, Raver, Hayes & Richardson, 2007; Li-Grining & Raver (revise & resubmit) ; Raver et al (in press) We embedded direct assessment of children’s self-regulation within a standardized battery that included vocabulary, letter-naming, & math a) Executive functioning (pencil task, balance beam) b) Compliance (“cool toys” clean up) c) Effortful control (giftwrap/no peek + snack delay) Plus– Yields ASSESSOR REPORT of attention/impulse control Figure 1.3 CSRP Treatment Effects on Self-regulation and Pre-academic Outcomes 1.0 0.8 Standard Deviation ** 0.6 ** * 0.4 * * 0.2 0.0 -0.2 Executive Function Effortful Control Attention/ Impulsivity -0.4 Raver, Jones, Li-Grining, Zhai, Bub, & Pressler (2011). Child Development PPVT Letter Naming Early Math Skills ** p<0.01, * p<0.05 Are these findings trustworthy? – Replicate…. Foundations of Learning, Newark & Chicago • Pamela Morris, PI with MDRC as lead CSRP FoL Pilot Newark 2004-2006 2006-2007 2007-2008 2008-2009 Total # of Sites 18 17 51 20 Program/ Control 9/9 9/8 26/25 10/10 Total # of Classrooms 36 17 51 40 Timeline FoL Full-Scale FoL Full-Scale Newark Chicago FOL improved children’s levels of engagement, behavioral control Morris, Raver, Millenky, Jones, & Lloyd (2010). Making preschool more productive: How classroom management training can help teachers. New York, NY: MDRC. 28 Take-home messages of those intervention findings • Support for model of experiential canalization of self-reg (Blair, 2010; Blair & Raver, revise & resubmit). • When children’s self-regulation is targeted, at the level of targeted classroom context, children demonstrate substantial academic gains – – Vindication of Zigler’s emphasis on “whole child” • Caution: No “silver bullet” to solve the problems of poverty – Optimistic hypothesis: children will be better able to capitalize on future opportunities for learning in early elementary years. – A less optimistic hypothesis: gains will be sustained only as long as children continue to have access to high quality classroom practices. SR and multiple forms of adversity • RCT great for causal inference, but problem of limited generalizability (utos) • Expectations of long-term impact? • What are children facing, besides ∆ in preschool qual? Family poverty & Instbility SR and experimental ∆ in preschool Family poverty & Instbility SR in K school quality SR in 3rd school quality Children are exposed to “2nd treatment” of higher vs. lower school quality SR in the context of K school quality Treatment Child Covariates at Baseline Boy Age Non-Hispanic black Pretreatment score Low-quality Schools C-TRF C-TRF Internalizing Externalizing 0.91 2.66 (2.26) (3.39) High-quality Schools C-TRF C-TRF Internalizing Externalizing -2.99* -5.36** (1.27) (1.66) 1.71 (2.28) -0.03 (0.21) 1.45 (1.26) 0.52+ (0.29) 1.34 (2.38) 0.05 (0.27) -3.50 (3.36) 0.80** (0.17) Mother Covariates at Baseline Mother was married -2.88 -3.77 (2.64) (3.37) Less than high school education -4.09* -0.80 (2.01) (6.41) Working 10 hours or less per week 3.09+ 4.55+ (1.65) (2.71) Income below 50% poverty line -1.35 -0.30 (1.59) (3.01) Classroom Covariates at Baseline not included in table for ease of presentation -1.62 (2.39) 0.10 (0.12) 0.09 (2.08) 0.48+ (0.26) 3.45 (3.20) 0.15 (0.24) -1.04 (4.89) 0.66** (0.27) 1.79 (2.68) 1.75 (2.17) -1.50 (1.61) 1.03 (1.39) -0.84 (3.33) 1.20 (2.60) -4.54 (3.49) -1.08 (3.27) Overall, the effects of CSRP on self-reg and school readiness in K are null Zhai, Raver & Jones (submitted) Children & Youth Services Review using principal score matching and stratification (n=361) to test for treatment effects in k. SR and experimental ∆ in preschool School quality K Where “school quality” = the % of students meeting or exceeding state standards on the ISAT within that school. What about the emotional climate of the school? – Children exposed to threatening home environments have been found to demonstrate serious disruptions in emotional regulation and EF – Do these findings extend to CSRP children’s exposure to unsafe schools? Capitalizing on Chicago Student Connection Surveys: Family poverty & Instbility SR and experimental ∆ in preschool Family poverty & Instbility School quality K School quality 3rd In 2009, 17% of CPS middle school students reported that they did not feel physically safe (problems with fights, theft, or vandalism) and/or did not feel emotionally safe (often teased, bullied, harassed). Items included wanting to stay home, wanting to change schools due to not feeling safe at school. Analytic approach • Level-1 equation: Yij = β0j + β1j(Child’s attention/impulse control in Head Start) + β2j(Child’s gender) + β3j(Child’s race/ethnicity) + β4j(Child’s grade) + β5j(Chronicity of family poverty) + β6j(Maternal marital status) + β7j(Maternal education) + β8j(CSRP site pairs) + β9j(CSRP Tx status) + rij • Level-2 equation: β0j = γ00 + γ01(Unsafe Climate) + γ02(High Adult Support) + γ03(% Low-income students) + u0j • Level-2 equation with attention/impulse control*school climate interactions: β0j = γ00 + γ01(Unsafe Climate) + γ02(High Adult Support) + γ03(% Low-income students) + u0j β1j = γ10 + γ11(Unsafe Climate) + γ12(High Adult Support) • Level-2 equation with gender*school climate interactions: β0j = γ00 + γ01(Unsafe Climate) + γ02(High Adult Support) + γ03(% Low-income students) + u0j β2j = γ20 + γ21(Unsafe Climate) + γ22(High Adult Support) HLM analyses examining child- and school-level predictors of STRS conflict with teacher in K Model 1 1.50 (3.03) Model 2 1.31 (2.95) Conflict with the teacher in Head Start 0.46*** (0.08) 0.45*** (0.08) Family income-to-needs ratio at kindergarten -1.77* (0.77) -1.66* (0.78) School size (total enrollment) 0.00*** (0.00) 0.00*** (0.00) % low-income students in the school -0.11† (0.06) -0.08† (0.05) Safe and Positive Climate 0.09 (0.78) 1.06 (0.83) Climate of Respect and Responsibility -1.27† (0.72) -1.82 (1.15) High Adult Support 2.75 (2.10) 4.44 (3.39) Safe and Positive Climate*Child is male Climate of Respect and Responsibility*Child male -2.56*** 1.24 (0.77) (1.60) High Adult Support*Child male 9 -2.78 (4.65) Conflict with the Teacher Variable Child is African American (vs. all else) 8 7 6 5 Girls 4 Boys 3 2 1 0 Low on Safe and Positive Climate Lowenstein, Raver, Jones, Friedman & Pess (submitted) High on Safe and Positive Climate Role of school climate predicting cognitive regulation, 3rd grade 1.2 Cognitive Dysregulation 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 Low Att/Imp Cont 0 High Att/Imp Cont -0.2 -0.4 -0.6 -0.8 Low Preliminary analyses suggest: 1) Attention/impulse control in Head Start 2) Chronic poverty (trend) 3) Unsafe School X Attention/Impulse control in HS Med High Safe & Positive Climate (Corrected/excludes ISAT) Expanding perspectives on adversity to include neighborhood violence Family poverty & Instbility 2500 Feet 2000 Feet 1500 Feet 1000 Feet 1.2 0.8 0.4 Effect size SR and experimental ∆ in preschool 0.0 -0.4 -0.8 Neighbrhood violence -1.2 -1.6 Executive Functioning Effortful Control Attention Impulse Control Dependent Variable Figure 2. Estimates of homicide effects on four dimensions of self-regulation using homicide zone fixed effects specifications. Estimates represent effects of homicides occurring within distances of 2,500 feet, 2,000 feet, 1,500 feet, and 1,000 feet of child’s residential address. Sharkey, Tirado-Strayer, Papachristos, & Raver (to be submitted) CSRP children exposed to high levels of violence near home and school Our current work -- analyses of 5th grade ER and EF predicted by: Chronic poverty, negative school climate, and household & neighborhood violence Family poverty Family poverty Family poverty EF and ER T BIS/BRIEF 3rd EF and ER 5th RCT preschool School School Neigh violence Neigh violence Neigh violence School Neighborhood Crime: Geocoding procedures were employed to link children’s school address to the Chicago Police Department crime index Implications for prevention • Capitalizing on “booster” SEL treatment that occurs nonrandomly in Chicago Public Schools – – Zhai & Raver, APPAM, 2011 Tradeoffs re: higher external validity, but lower internal validity • Capitalizing on out-of-school time: “Adventure Camp” RCT pilot study – 2010 • N =24, age 7-11, high SES (µ = $99,000) – 2011 • N =37 , age 7-12, middle to high SES • CSRP data collection underway (Cohort 1 collected, Cohort 2 in January!) with opportunity to pilot, adapt new measures for large-scale survey & prevention research. • How do low-income, ethnic minority 5th graders understand emotion? Deploy attention when faced with emotionally negative stimuli? Respond to opportunity to “take gamble?” • Testing concurrent and longitudinal relations between ER and EF Concluding comments • RCTs as “gold standard” for efficacy of intervention/prevention • Expanding frameworks to consider the roles of “exogenous shocks” – housing vouchers, community policing, income loss • Capitalize on RCTs for opportunity to draw causal inferences re: Central role of self-regulation for learning • Expand basic science models to better understand the role of adversity in shaping the ER and EF of low-income, ethnic minority children. Thanks!