Scott Gest Human Development & Family Studies Penn State University Prevention Research Seminar November 17, 2010 A network perspective on peer norms Intervention effects on peer norms in middle/high school (PROSPER Peers) Correlates of classroom-level peer norms Everyday teaching practices and peer norms The promise/hope of setting level program impact ◦ Diffusion, mutual reinforcement, continuation Prevention programs often seek to reorganize social systems ◦ Dividing schools into smaller sub-units Other programs attempt setting-level change via individual-level change ◦ Seek to improve classroom climate by teaching individual social skills In Theory In Research In Prevention/Intervention Practice Public health emphasis on social networks ◦ Prominent in major explanations of deviance ◦ Differential association, social learning, problem behavior theory, peer cluster theory ◦ Friends’ deviance highly reliable predictor ◦ Emphases on refusal skills, friendship choice, parent monitoring about friends Descriptive norms ◦ what most people do (“what is”) ◦ Setting-level measure: average across individuals Injunctive norms ◦ what people are expected to do (“what ought to be”) ◦ Setting-level measure: average across individuals Norm salience ◦ the extent to which a behavior is associated with positive or negative social sanctions ◦ Setting-level measure: correlation between level of behavior and centrality in peer network Youth In-Degree Reach A 5 8 B 5 10 C 3 8 D 3 4 Deviant behavior associated with less central position. Deviant behavior associated with more central position. Contrasts with norm as attribute mean over indivs. Reduced selection of deviant friends ◦ Students encouraged to select prosocial peers who will help them meet positive goals ◦ Parents encouraged to monitor students’ activities and peer affiliations Link to network structure ◦ Altered friendship selection tendencies will place antisocial youth in less central/influential structural positions School based programs targeting substance use, community partnerships with university extension: ◦ Random assignment of communities 26 communities, 2 grade cohorts, 5 waves ◦ Iowa & Pennsylvania, Small towns ◦ 10,000+ Students per wave; 14,000+ total ◦ 368 school/cohort/wave networks Questionnaires assess friendships ◦ Fall of 6th grade & Spring of 6th, 7th, 8th, & 9th ◦ Also assess variety of attitudes and behaviors Lead Investigators ◦ Wayne Osgood, Mark Feinberg, Scott Gest, Jim Moody (Duke Univ.), Karen Bierman Other Investigators ◦ Derek Kreager, Sonja Siennick (Florida State Univ.), Kelly Rullison (UNC Greensboro), Michael Cleveland, Suellen Hopfer Graduate Assistants ◦ Wendy Brynildsen, Robin Gautier, Lauren Molloy, Dan Ragan, Debra Temkin, April Woolnough PROSPER Study Lead Investigators ◦ Dick Spoth, Mark Greenberg, Cleve Redmond, Mark Feinberg Funders ◦ National Institute of Drug Abuse, Division of Epidemiology, Services and Prevention Research, Prevention Research Branch ◦ William T. Grant Foundation Family-focused intervention – 6th Grade ◦ ◦ ◦ ◦ Strengthening Families Program Seven 2-hr sessions Overarching theme: “Love & Limits” Prior results suggest diffusion of effects School-focused intervention – 7th grade ◦ Life Skills Training, Project Alert, or All Stars ◦ 11 to 18 sessions ◦ Relevant focus: Selecting prosocial peers, helping peers make good choices, and resisting negative influence from peers Who are your best and closest friends in your grade? First name Last Name (or if you don’t know their last name, . . . ) How often do you “hang out” with this person outside of school, (without adults around)? 1) Never . . . 5) Almost Every Day YOUR BEST FRIEND or FRIENDS OTHER CLOSE FRIENDS Questionnaire response rate: 87.2% Usable friendship choices, Overall: 81.9% ◦ Of respondents: 93.9% Name matching, Overall: 81.5% ◦ Inter-rater agreement, 98% ◦ Non-matches primarily out of school Reciprocation rate ◦ Overall: 48%, ◦ 1st choice: 76% Regression Coefficient ◦ Within-school association across individuals ◦ Between a measure of deviant behavior (IV) and a network measure of influence potential (DV) ◦ (Mean effect later standardized for effect size) Degree: Number of Friendships Closeness Centrality: Mean distance to reach others Reach: Number of Direct & Indirect Friendships Bonacich Centrality: Links to well connected others Information Central: Harmonic mean dist to others Betweenness Central: Import. in connecting others Composite: Standardized sum of above ◦ All for both incoming and total friendships ◦ Transformed to normalize distributions and make independent of network size Substance Use: ◦ 30 days, 4 substances, IRT scoring Substance Use Attitudes: ◦ Composite of 4 scales, 22 items Delinquency: ◦ Past year, 12 items, IRT scoring Composite antisocial ◦ Sum of other 3, standardized No significant pretest diffs btwn trt & ctrl Waves 2 – 5 as outcome ◦ Pooled test (impact doesn’t vary over time) Multilevel model: ◦ 5 levels: Comm pairs for random assignment, Community, School, Cohort, Wave School & Cohort crossed Level 1 variance heterogeneous by s. e. of b ◦ Controls for wave, state, Network size (Log, quadratic), and pretest ◦ Estimated by Bayesian MCMC in MLwiN ◦ BDIC criterion for variance comps & controls PROSPER Program Effects on Behavioral Trtent Vs. Norms Control Measures Defining Relationship Antisocial Social Status Behavior & (Undirected) Attitudes Composite Composite Composite Substance Use Composite Subst Use Attitudes Composite Delinquency Total Friends Composite Closeness Centrality Composite Direct & Indirect Frnds Composite Bonacich Central. Composite Information Central. Composite Betweenness Central. Composite p < .05, 2 tail p < .10, 2 tail Diff., Std b -0.052 -0.034 -0.048 -0.046 -0.045 -0.056 -0.041 -0.080 -0.063 -0.021 Comparison Std. Error z 0.022 -2.38 0.018 -1.93 0.023 -2.07 0.022 -2.05 0.025 -1.84 0.115 -0.49 0.019 -2.15 0.039 -2.03 0.055 -1.15 0.067 -0.31 N = 253 - 256 networks p 0.017 0.053 0.039 0.040 0.066 0.625 0.032 0.043 0.252 0.760 Std. b for Compos. Social Status with Compos. Antisocial 0.05 0.00 -0.05 -0.10 -0.15 -0.20 Pretest 6th Fall 6th Grade 7th Grade 8th Grade 9th Grade Spring Spring Spring Spring Control Treatment Clear evidence of beneficial intervention impact on behavioral norms ◦ Reduced social status of antisocial relative to prosocial youth ◦ Most consistent for composite measures ◦ Non-signif often equally strong (lower power) Modest effect size ◦ Approximately .05 difference in correlation Implications Solid initial support for social network approach to setting level intervention ◦ Setting level impact distinct from individual effects Next Steps Simulations needed to determine implied reductions in deviance ◦ Networks are complex systems and influence processes are reciprocal and dynamic Assess evidence for mediation ◦ More deviance reductions where bigger network effects? Are positive peer norms associated with better youth experiences & outcomes? How might everyday teaching practices shape peer norms? Standard finding in peer relations literature: aggression is negatively correlated with acceptance (being “liked most”) But this masks considerable variation across classrooms norms against aggression norms supporting aggression TRAILS study (Siegwart Lindenberg, PI) ◦ Dijkstra, Gest, Lindenberg, Sentsa & Veenstra (in prep) N = 3,231 youth in 164 classrooms Mage = 13.60 Peer nominations ◦ acceptance/rejection, behavior Student reports ◦ fun, boredom, excitement at school Teacher reports ◦ student achievement Overall, there were weak norms against academic achievement, but considerable variability. ◦ Mr (164) = -.11, SDr = .28 ◦ 10th %ile r = -.42 90th %ile r = .27 Similar variability in norms for prosocial behavior (weakly supporting) and bullying (moderately against) Norm for achievement, prosocial and bullying were intercorrelated, so overall classroom norms in each classroom were categorized as positive or negative In classrooms categorized as having a positive profile of peer norms: ◦ Students reported more fun & excitement less boredom more emotional and practical support from peers less peer rejection ◦ Teachers reported better academic adjustment ◦ Median d = .15 Peer norms may serve as useful marker of classroom social atmosphere & student experience What teaching practices may be related to these norms? Goal: Identify teaching practices associated with emerging peer network features (including peer norms) and links to student outcomes 1st, 3rd & 5th grade classrooms ◦ Assessed three times within same school year Each assessment includes measures of… ◦ Teaching practices (Observations, Teacher report) ◦ Peer-reported networks ◦ Youth behavior (peer & teacher report) ◦ Youth perceptions of classroom & school Sample ◦ N = 39 classrooms from pilot study year (cross-sectional) ◦ Final sample will include >150 classrooms Investigators: Scott Gest, Phil Rodkin (U of Illinois), Tom Farmer PA Lab ◦ Grad students: Deborah Temkin, Rebecca Madill, Kathleen Zadzora, Rachel Abenavoli ◦ Project Coordinator: Gwen Kreamer ◦ Data manager: Larissa Witmer ◦ Undergraduate assistants: Kristen Granger, Kara McKee Funding ◦ William T. Grant Foundation & Spencer Foundation ◦ Institute of Education Sciences 1930s – Lewin ◦ teachers can and must influence “social atmosphere” of the classroom 1940s-50s – Gronlund (1959) ◦ Teachers should try to prevent “cliques & cleavages” in “social fabric”. Based on clinical wisdom, mostly from descriptive case studies. 1970s – Hallinan ◦ Network analysis to show that reading groups foster friendships 1980s-90s – Cairns & Cairns ◦ “invisible hand” of the teacher in shaping peer networks 2000s – Farmer ◦ Elaboration on how teachers can deliberately influence social related to status, affiliations and aggression Now ◦ potential for network concepts & methods to strengthen and test theories about teacher influence on peer networks • Generally weak associations (little power with N=39) • Emotional support associated with less peer support for aggression • Instructional support associated with less peer support for prosocial behavior and achievement Causality unclear: Peer norms teaching? Teaching peer norms? Longitudinal design (Sep/Nov/May) will help some There is striking diversity in teachers’ use of grouping strategies, even within the same school – what do teachers think their strategies are accomplishing? Sparse empirical literature to guide practice. Might peer norms partially mediate any associations between teaching practices and student experiences of the classroom (e.g., perceptions of support, achievement motivation)? What do we really know about “desirable” and “undesirable” features of peer networks? ◦ How are peer norms related to student experiences and academic learning over time? ◦ Are more tight-knit networks always better? ◦ Are cliques always bad? ◦ Are hierarchies always bad? ◦ Might the desirability of these features depend on which behaviors are valued in the network? What are teachers (and schools) already doing that may have systematic effects on peer networks? ◦ Decades of clinical wisdom but little data ◦ Tremendous variation in very basic practices ◦ Variation in teacher awareness of peer dynamics, goals for network, and planful action ◦ Building an evidence base could clarify processes governing emerging network features and provide a stronger foundation for professional training & development How can we apply this knowledge to advance “prevention science” in schools? ◦ There is value in articulating hypotheses about how school-based interventions may impact peer networks at the setting level Many “universal” interventions target all students in the school setting Implications for peer network are sometimes explicit, but more often implicit ◦ Network concepts can sharpen program theories and indices can permit strong tests ◦ Consideration of network dynamics quickly leads to recognize of potential tradeoffs in intervention effects