International Journal of Primatology, Vol. 21, No. 4, 2000 Socioecological Context of Parturition in Wild Japanese Macaques (Macaca fuscata) on Yakushima Island Ruth Thomsen1 and Joseph Soltis2 Received May 5, 1999; revision December 16, 1999; accepted January 18, 2000 Female primates may adopt special feeding, foraging, and social strategies around the time of giving birth. We observed 8 females during the prepartum period, the day of birth, and the postpartum period in a wild troop of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) on Yakushima Island, Japan. We collected data on their activity budgets, quantitative feeding and foraging behavior, and on female-male interactions. On the day of birth, females spent less time moving and feeding, and more time resting relative to other periods. Overall dietary diversity as well as arthropod foraging also decreased on the day of birth. Females fed mostly on mature leaves, new leaves and shoots during prepartum periods but mostly on fruit during postpartum periods. Decreased feeding on leaves and increased feeding on fruit probably changed as a result of seasonal availability, independently of parturition. Feeding on flowers, fungi and other items remained constant over all periparturitional periods. On the day of birth, new mothers had fewer social interactions with males and spent more time out of proximity with other adults than in other periods. Females rejected grooming presentations from males, groomed less with males, spent less time ⱕ3 m of males, and received less aggression from males on the day of birth. In the postpartum period, interactions with males returned to prepartum values. Grooming with females did not differ across the three periods. These results suggest that interactions with males may be costly for females on the day of birth. KEY WORDS: wild Japanese macaques; birth-giving; feeding and social behavior; male’s role; Yakushima. 1 Department of Zoology, LM-University of Munich, Luisenstrasse 14, 80333 Munich, Germany. E-mail: 320084691601-0001@t-online.de (Ruth Thomsen). 2 Department of Anthropology, University of California, Los Angeles, California 90095, USA. 685 0164-0291/00/0800-0685$18.00/0 2000 Plenum Publishing Corporation 686 Thomsen and Soltis INTRODUCTION Studies of reproductive success in primates and other animals have emphasized the importance of female access to food resources and male access to females (Wrangham, 1980; van Schaik and van Hooff, 1996; Suzuki and Tsukahara, 1996). With the exception of some primate species in which males or older siblings contribute to parental care (Garber, 1997), female primates are solely responsible for rearing their young. Thus, the quality of maternal care, including feeding and social behavior around the time of birth, should also have an important impact on female reproductive success. Studies concerning parturition in primates have mainly focused on patterns of social behavior (Macaca mulatta: Brandt and Mitchell, 1973; Theropithecus gelada: Dunbar and Dunbar, 1974; M. fascicularis: Kemps and Timmerman, 1982; Gorilla gorilla beringei: Stewart, 1984; Papio cynocephalus cynocephalus: Condit and Smith, 1994). Only two reports take into account the feeding behavior of the new mother (Macaca sinica: Ratnayeke and Dittus, 1989; M. fuscata yakui: Thomsen, 1997). This bias stems from the fact that most studies have concerned captive or provisioned animals in which observations of natural feeding behavior is not possible. We describe the feeding and social behavior of mothers around the birth period for wild, unprovisioned Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) on Yakushima Island, Japan. We observed 8 females during their individual periparturitional periods between March and June 1998. We examined the changes in their activity budgets, the quality, quantity and diversity of their diets, and their social behavior. Japanese macaques are female-bonded primates, where a female’s strongest relationships are with her close female kin (Itani, 1972; Pusey and Packer, 1987; Davies, 1992). Studies of the relationship between female and male Japanese macaques have mainly concerned mating strategies (Enomoto, 1978; Huffman, 1992; Soltis et al., 1997). Affiliative relationships between females and males during the birth season have also been observed by Takahata (1982), though, infanticide was suspected in this population (personal observations). Therefore, we focused on social interactions between parturient females and adult males. METHODS Study Site and Subjects Yakushima Island (30⬚20⬘N; 130⬚30⬘E) constitutes the southern limit of the distribution of Japanese macaques and its population is an endemic Socioecological Context of Parturition in Wild Japanese Macaques 687 subspecies, Macaca fuscata yakui (Yakushima macaque). The mating period is from September to February, and the birth period from March to July. The monkeys have no natural predator. The diversity of plants is very high for a temperate forest, with evergreen as well as deciduous tree species. The vegetation types include primary forest, mixed primary and secondary forest, secondary roadside forest and coastal vegetation (Maruhashi, 1980; Hill, 1997). We studied the well-habituated NINA-A troop, whose home range extended from sea level to about 220 m asl and covered an area of about 1.4 km2. A single road transected the range. Records of troop composition and male transfers have been kept since 1994 (T. Tanaka; Yakushima Research Group). We studied the NINA-A troop continuously from September 1997 to December 1998. At the beginning of the study, the troop consisted of 52 monkeys, including 15 adult males, and 15 adult females. All adults and nearly all subadults could be distinguished individually by each observer. At the beginning of the 1997 mating season, 4 females were lactating, 3 were considered nulliparous (estimated 4–6 years with no living offspring), and 8 females were parous, each with at least one living offspring in the troop. All nonlactating and ⱖ2 lactating females copulated in the 1997 mating period. Data Collection In order to examine behavior around the time of parturition, we restricted systematic data collection to 3 periparturitional periods as follows. (1) The prepartum period, which began 25 days before the estimated day of birth and ended two days before, (2) the day of birth, (3) the postpartum period, which began the second day after birth and ended 25 days thereafter. We excluded two additional periods—the day preceding and the day after the day of birth—due to lack of data. We estimated birth days so that prepartum behaviors could be recorded for pregnant females. Although female Japanese macaques often experience more than one estrus during the mating period, most conceive from their first ovulation (Mitsunaga et al., 1992). We calculated the probable birth range by counting forward 173 days from the first day and last day of each female’s first mating period (Nigi, 1976; Mitsunaga et al., 1992). For prepartum periods, we followed the 11 nonlactating females and 2 control—lactating—females from midMarch to beginning of June 1998. Five parous females, 2 primiparous females and 1 control female delivered during the study period (N ⫽ 8 births). We excluded females that did not give birth from data analysis. When a 688 Thomsen and Soltis newborn was detected, we recorded the date, time of detection, sex of the baby and the absence of the placenta or placentophagia. We recorded data using focal-animal sampling with 1-min point-sampling (Altmann, 1974) for 6 hrs per day for ⱖ2 days in the pre- and postpartum periods for each female. On the day of birth, we conducted focalanimal sampling for as long as possible, given the steep terrain, but always for ⱖ3 hrs without interruption. For statistical analysis, we used 331 observation hrs (19860 points; range: 2250–2700; mean ⫾ SD: 2467.5 ⫾ 134.9 points per female) on the 8 females (range: 37.7–45.0, mean: 41.12 ⫾ 2.25 hrs) over the 3 periods (prepartum period: range: 12–18; mean: 17.25 ⫾ 2.12 hrs; day of birth: range: 3.0–9.0; mean: 6.15 ⫾ 1.85 hrs; postpartum: mean 18.0 ⫾ 0.0 hrs). RT conducted 316 hrs 30 min over all periods. Soltis conducted an additional 14 hrs 30 min when newborns were observed in the absence of Thomsen (N ⫽ 3). Activity Budget At each minute point-sample, we recorded one of the following activities: feeding (processing and eating food items), moving (all locomotor activities on the ground and in trees, except for those related to aggression), grooming (of and by others, excluding self-grooming), resting (sitting or lying down without grooming). For each period, we calculated the percentage of time a female spent in each activity or in proximity with other individuals by summing the points for each activity and dividing by the sum of all points. Feeding Behavior We know most of the major plant food species when we began systematic data collection. When a focal female was feeding, we recorded the species and the part consumed at each point sample. We distinguish 6 broad categories: (1) fruit and seeds, (2) mature leaves, (3) young leaves, buds and shoots, (4) flowers (5) fungi and (6) other: pith, bark, lichen, leaves and stem of vines, fern, moss, grass and soil. We calculated percentage of feeding time on each food category as we did for activity budgets. Foraging and Feeding on Arthropods We analyzed foraging and feeding on arthropods separately from the other feeding records. To measure arthropod foraging and feeding activity, Socioecological Context of Parturition in Wild Japanese Macaques 689 we used a hand-held counter to record each searching bout in a potential arthropod patch (foraging) and each grip on an arthropod followed by hand-to-mouth motion by a focal female (feeding). We recognized the following arthropod patches: bunches of dried hanging leaves, dried ferns, beneath bark of rotten or live trees, within rotten trees, within small branches (for ants), and under mature leaves, stones or the crash barrier near the road. We calculated each female’s arthropod foraging rate for each period per hour of active behavior: feeding and moving. Dietary Diversity We made a daily food list for each female. For each period, we calculated the number of plant species consumed per 6 hours of observation. The complete food list (38 spp. of 26 families) is available upon request. Social Contact and Female–Male Relationships We calculated the percent of time females remained alone: 3 m away from other adults. We also recorded (1) female rejection of male grooming presentations, (2) male-to-female aggressive interactions per hour, (3) the percentage of time females were ⱕ3 m of a male, (4) the percentage of time grooming with males, and (5) the percentage of time grooming with females. Because aggressive behaviors are rare and short-lived, we recorded all occurrences of aggression during focal samples. Aggressive behaviors included noncontact (lunge, open-face threat, chase) and contact aggression (bite, cuff). Statistical Analysis We used Friedman’s test for differences across the 3 periparturitional periods and Dunn’s procedure to compare specific periods (Conover, 1980) via in InStat 2.01 for Macintosh. All p-values presented are two-tailed, with significance set at p ⬍ 0.05. RESULTS Activity Budgets The time females spent in each of the 4 activities differed across the 3 periparturient periods. Compared to the prepartum and/or the postpartum 690 Thomsen and Soltis periods, the day of birth was characterized by a decrease in the time spent feeding, moving and grooming, and an increase in the time spent resting (Table I). Quantitative Feeding Behavior Feeding on arthropods, fruit, mature leaves, new leaves and shoots, and overall dietary diversity differed across the 3 periods. Time spent feeding on flowers, fungi and other items, however, remained at a constant, low level of around 5% (Table II). On the day of birth the arthropod foraging rate as well as the overall dietary diversity decreased. The percent of feeding time on fruit showed an increase across the three periods. By contrast, feeding on leaves and shoots was high in the prepartum period, but lower on the day of birth and in the postpartum period (Table II). Additionally, placentophagia occurred in 2 of the 8 females in the early morning hours. We saw no placenta when we first detected the other 6 babies. Social Interactions and Female-Male Relationships Most measures of social interaction varied across the three periparturitient periods (Table III). On the day of birth, females spent more time Table I. Activity budget of periparturitional females (N ⫽ 8)a Activity 1. Feeding time (%) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 2. Moving time (%) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 3. Resting time (%) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 4. Grooming time (%) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 Friedman’s test (df ⫽ 2) A prepartum B day of birth C postpartum Fr ⫽ 12.250 p ⫽ 0.0009 34.3 ⫺ 45.1 38.4 ⫾ 3.3 B 7.4 ⫺ 13.5 10.6 ⫾ 1.9 A and C 32.3 ⫺ 40.3 36.4 ⫾ 2.55 B Fr ⫽ 14.000 p ⫽ 0.0001 16.1 ⫺ 29.3 22.01 ⫾ 4.2 B 10.0 ⫺ 17.2 13.44 ⫾ 2.34 A 13.3 ⫺ 21.10 16.9 ⫾ 2.8 Fr ⫽ 13.00 p ⫽ 0.0003 10.8 ⫾ 23.5 16.7 ⫾ 3.86 B 51.0 ⫺ 78.0 60.56 ⫾ 9.7 A and C 12.0 ⫺ 28.9 18.12 ⫾ 5.24 B Fr ⫽ 9.750 p ⫽ 0.0048 18.0 ⫺ 29.0 22.62 ⫾ 4.34 0.0 ⫺ 26.6 15.8 ⫾ 9.8 C 20.0 ⫺ 37.0 28.12 ⫾ 49 B Range of percentages, mean ⫾ SD. Significant differences between the 3 periparturitional periods are indicated by A, B, or C in the rows labeled Dunn’s post test. a Fr ⫽ 7.750 p ⫽ 0.0179 Fr ⫽ 9.250 p ⫽ 0.008 Fr ⫽ 6.250 p ⫽ 0.0469 all NS 6.0 ⫺ 12.0 7.75 ⫾ 2.43 B Fl. 0.0 ⫺ 8.2; 2.4 ⫾ 2.7 Fu. 4.8 ⫺ 14.8; 7.4 ⫾ 2.9 Oth. 1.4 ⫺ 23.3; 6.7 ⫾ 7.4 12.2 ⫺ 43.2 26.27 ⫾ 11.35 C 18.1 ⫺ 51.0 36.91 ⫾ 10.72 B and C 10.0 ⫺ 40.6 20.35 ⫾ 10.16 B 6.2 ⫺ 12.1 9.1 ⫾ 2.0 Fr ⫽ 14.250 p ⫽ 0.001 Fr ⫽ 8.600 p ⫽ 0.0099 A prepartum Friedman’s test (df ⫽ 2) 0.0 ⫺ 42.0 18.5 ⫺ 15.75 A 0.0 ⫺ 26.2 9.4 ⫾ 16.08 A 0.0 ⫺ 4.6 2.05 ⫾ 1.6 C 1.0 ⫺ 6.0 3.38 ⫾ 1.41 A 0.0 ⫺ 2.0; 0.66 ⫾ 0.93 0.0 ⫺ 15.6; 5.0 ⫾ 5.4 0.0 ⫺ 33.6; 7.8 ⫾ 11.39 0.0 ⫺ 77.3 33.86 ⫾ 28.12 B day of birth 0.0 ⫺ 7.0; 3.6 ⫾ 3.2 0.0 ⫺ 13.0; 6.6 ⫾ 4.4 3.2 ⫺ 19.1; 10.3 ⫾ 6.3 26.1 ⫺ 72.2 53.14 ⫾ 17.99 A 7.5 ⫺ 32.4 16.03 ⫾ 8.7 A 2.3 ⫺ 13.3 10.25 ⫾ 7.4 3.8 ⫺ 30.1 16.7 ⫾ 8.9 B 5.0 ⫺ 10.0 7.5 ⫾ 1.93 C postpartum a Range of percentages, mean ⫾ SD. Significant differences between the 3 peripartuitent periods are indicated by A, B, or C in the rows labeled Dunn’s post test. 4. Fruit (% of feeding time) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 5. Mature leaves (% of feeding time) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 6. New leaves and shoots (% of feeding time) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 1. Arthropod foraging rate (grips/hour) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 2. Dietary diversity (within 6 hr) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 3. Flower, fungi, and other (% of feeding time) Ecological context Table II. Ecological context of parturition (N ⫽ 8 females)a Socioecological Context of Parturition in Wild Japanese Macaques 691 0.0 ⫺ 20.0 6.36 ⫾ 7.87 B 0.000 ⫺ 0.333 0.135 ⫾ 0.106 B 9.1 ⫺ 21.1 15.1 ⫾ 4.2 B 0.0 ⫺ 14.1 5.52 ⫾ 4.96 B 10.0 ⫺ 25.4 17.61 ⫾ 5.31 32.3 ⫺ 48.4 38.6 ⫾ 5.2 Fr ⫽ 13.067 p ⫽ 0.0001 Fr ⫽ 10.750 p ⫽ 0.0024 NS Fr ⫽ 10.867 p ⫽ 0.0011 Fr ⫽ 9.2500 p ⫽ 0.008 Fr ⫽ 11.385 p ⫽ 0.0011 A prepartum Friedman’s test (df ⫽ 2) 66.6 ⫺ 100 95.82 ⫾ 11.81 A and C 0.0 ⫺ 0.0 0.0 ⫾ 0.0 A and C 0.0 ⫺ 16.1 5.4 ⫾ 5.1 A and C 0.0 ⫺ 0.0 0.0 ⫾ 0.0 A 0.0 ⫺ 26.4 15.81 ⫾ 9.8 32.5 ⫺ 86.9 49.6 ⫾ 18.4 C B day of birth 13.4 ⫺ 36.5 24.6 ⫾ 7.6 27.9 ⫺ 60.5 36.3 ⫾ 10.7 B 0.0 ⫾ 12.5 3.49 ⫾ 5.05 B 0.0 ⫺ 0.222 0.111 ⫾ 0.066 B 6.2 ⫺ 23.3 15.4 ⫾ 5.2 B 0.0 ⫺ 7.1 3.89 ⫾ 2.87 C postpartum a Range of percentages, mean ⫾ SD. Significant differences between the 3 periparturient periods are indicated by A, B, or C in the rows labeled Dunn’s post test. Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 4. Groom with males (%) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 5. Groom with females (%) 6. Females being alone (%) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 1. Groom rejection of males (%) Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 2. Aggression with males/hour Dunn’s post test, p ⬍ 0.05 3. Males ⬍3 m (%) Social context Table III. Social context of parturition (N ⫽ 8 females).a 692 Thomsen and Soltis Socioecological Context of Parturition in Wild Japanese Macaques 693 ⱕ 3 m of other adults than in the postpartum period. Also, rejection of grooming presentations from males increased dramatically on the day of birth. Further, grooming with males, aggression received from males, and time spent ⱕ3 m of males all decreased on the day of birth compared to the perpartum and/or postpartum periods. Conversely, time spent grooming with females did not differ across the three periods (Table III). DISCUSSION Activity Budget and Ecological Context The prepartum period was characterized by particularly long times spent feeding on mature and young leaves and high dietary diversity. This finding reflects the general pattern observed among Yakushima macaques of the warm temperate forest, in which time spent feeding is positively correlated with leaf feeding, and the more Yakushima macaques feed on leaves the higher dietary diversity becomes (Hill, 1997). In the postpartum period, lactating females consumed more fruit and seed than in the prepartum period; thus, dietary diversity was lower. Quantitative food selection is dependent on availability (Agetsuma, 1995), and in general Yakushima macaques prefer to feed on ripe fruit, which mainly is available in autumn and early winter (Hill, 1997). They change to leaves during late winter and spring if fruit become scarce. The female feeding pattern prepartum as well as postpartum reflects this rule for omnivorous primates (Harrison, 1984) and may not be closely related with parturition. On the day of birth, females spent more time resting and less time moving and feeding than before and after birth. Such extreme reduction in feeding time on the day of birth, from around 40 to 10%, was previously reported for periparturitional Yakushima macaques by Thomsen (1997) and may be similar for other primate species. Females that give birth may require increased resting time, which may be achieved by reducing feeding to minimal levels and by reducing moving time to the minimum required to keep up with the troop. Arthropod foraging may have decreased because it is incompatible with carrying a newborn infant. In the two cases where in the first detection of the newborn occurred early in the morning, placentophagia occurred. Placentophagia occurs in other wild primates (Dunbar and Dunbar, 1974; Ratnayeke and Dittus, 1989) and may help to compensate for the lack of nutritional intake on the day of birth (Brandt and Mitchell, 1971). 694 Thomsen and Soltis Social Strategies On the day of birth, females reduced the time spent ⱕ3 m of other adult troop members. Overall interaction with males was greatly reduced. Time spent with males, grooming with males, and aggression received from males, all decreased on the day of birth. Moreover, females dramatically increased rejection of male grooming presentations from about 5 to 95%. Grooming received from other females, however, did not differ across the periparturient periods. This suggests that interactions with males are costly in parturient contexts. There is potential for both costly and beneficial interaction with males in Japanese macaques. Aggression from males towards infants occurs in primates, e.g., Macaca mulatta (Brandt and Mitchell 1973; Rawlins 1979), and infanticide is often part of male reproductive strategy (Hrdy et al., 1995). Furthermore, Soltis (in press) observed male attacks on infants and one case of infanticide in the study population. Conversely, affiliative male-female relationships in the birth season also occur in provisioned Japanese macaques. Females may benefit from cofeeding with males when food is clumped (Grewal, 1980; Takahata, 1982). Since females reduced feeding time on the day of birth and clumped food is rare in this habitat, females may have little demand for cofeeding benefits immediately after parturition. Our findings suggest that at least on the day of birth the costs of being close to males outweighs any presumed benefits. In the postpartum period interactions with males returned to prepartum values. It is possible that benefits of male interaction, e.g., protection from infanticide or cofeeding opportunities, outweigh costs at these times with vulnerable infants. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We would like to thank Naohiko Noma for teaching plant identifications and Toshiaki Tanaka from Yakushima Research Group for providing data on the study troop. We also thank Sachiko Hayakawa, Miki Matsubara and Hideki Sugiura for their cooperation in the field on Yakushima Island and at the PRI. We received very helpful comments and critical advise on an earlier draft of the manuscript from Mike Huffman, David Hill, Volker Sommer, and Juichi Yamagiwa. RT furthermore wants to thank Juichi Yamagiwa and Hideyuki Ohsawa and the PRI for continual support. JS thanks Yukimaru Sugiyama and Juichi Yamagiwa for their continual support. RT was financed by a grant (HSP III) from the German Academic Socioecological Context of Parturition in Wild Japanese Macaques 695 Exchange Service (DAAD) and JS by the USA National Science Foundation and the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science. REFERENCES Agetsuma, N. (1995). Dietary selection by Yakushima macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui): The Influence of Food Availability and Temperature. Int. J. Primatol. 16 (4): 611–627. Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–265. Brandt, E. M., and Mitchell, G. (1971). Parturition in primates: Behavior related to birth. In: Rosenblum, L. A. (ed.): Primate Behavior: Developments in Field and Laboratory Research. New York. Academic Press, Vol. 2. pp. 178–223. Brandt, E. M., and Mitchell, G. (1973). Labor and delivery behavior in rhesus monkeys (Macacamulatta). Amer. J. Phys. Anthrop. 38: 519–522. Condit, V. K., and Smith, E. O. (1994). Yellow baboon labor and parturition at the Tana River National Primate Reserve, Kenya. Am. J. Primatol. 33: 51–55. Conover, W. J. (1980). Practical Nonparametric Statistics. John Wiley & Sons, New York. Davies, N. B. (1992). Mating systems. In: Krebs, J. R., and Davies, N. B. (eds.): Behavioural Ecology: an Evolutionary Approach. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications, pp. 263–294. Dunbar, R. I. M., and Dunbar, P. (1974). Behaviour related to birth in wild Gelada baboons (Theropithecus gelada). Behaviour 50: 185–181. Enomoto, T. (1978). On the social preference in sexual behavior of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). J. Hum. Evol. 7: 283–293. Garber, P. A. (1997). One for all and breeding for one: Cooperation and competition as a Tamarin reproductive strategy. Evol. Anthrop. 5: 187–199. Grewal, B. S. (1980). Social relationships between adult central males and kinship groups of Japanese monkeys at Arashiyama with some aspects of troop organization. Primates 21(2): 161–180. Harrison, M. J. S. (1984). Optimal Foraging Strategies in the Diet of the Green Monkey, Cercopithecus sabaeus, at Mt. Assirik, Senegal. Int. J. Primatol. 5: 435–471. Hill, D. A. (1997). Seasonal variation in feeding behavior and diet of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) in lowland forest of Yakushima. Am. J. Primatol. 43: 305–322. Hrdy, S. B., Janson, C., and van Schaik, C. (1995). Infanticide: Let’s not throw the baby with the bathwater. Evol. Anthropol. 3: 151–154. Huffman, M. A. (1992). Influences of female partner preference on potential reproductive outcome in Japanese macaques. Folia Primatol. 59: 77–88. Itani, J. (1972). A preliminary essay on the relationship between incest avoidance in primates. In: Poirer, F. E. (ed.): Primate Socialization. New York, Random Press, pp. 165–171. Kemps, A., and Timmerman, P. (1982). Parturition behaviour in pluriparous Java-macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Primates 23: 75–88. Maruhashi, T. (1980): Feeding behavior and diet of the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata yakui) on Yakushima Island, Japan. Primates 21: 141–160. Mitsunaga, F., Nozaki, M., Inoue, M., Takenaka, A., Takenaka, O., Sakura, O., Sugiyama, Y., and Ohsawa. H. (1992): Steroid hormones and sexual behavior of female Japanese monkeys in an enclosed group. In: Itoigawa, N., Sugiyama, Y., Sackett, G. P., and Thompson, R. K. R. (eds.): Topics in Primatology, Vol. 2. Tokyo, Japan: University of Tokyo Press, pp. 23–34. Nigi, H. (1976). Some aspects related to conception of the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata). Primates 17(1): 81–87. Pusey, A. E., and Packer, C. (1987). Dispersal and philopatry. In: Primate Societies, B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham, and T. T. Struhsaker (eds.). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, pp. 250–266. 696 Thomsen and Soltis Ratnayeke, A. P., and Dittus, W. (1989). Observation of a birth among wild toque macaques (Macaca sinica). Int. J. Primatol. 10: 235–242. Rawlins, R. (1979). Parturient and postpartum behaviour of a free-ranging Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). J. Mammal. 60(2): 432–433. van Schaik, C. P., and van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1996). Toward an understanding of the orangutan’s social system. In: Great Ape Societies, W. C. McGrew, L. F. Marchant, and T. Nishida (eds.). Cambridge University Press, pp. 3–15. Soltis, J., Mitsunaga, F., Shimizu, K., Yanagihara, Y., and Nozaki, M. (1997). Sexual selection in Japanese macaques. I: Female mate choice or male sexual coercion? Anim. Behav. 54: 725–736. Soltis, J., Thomsen, R., Matsubayashi, K., and Takenaka, O. (1999). Infanticide by resident males and female counter-strategies in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Stewart, K. (1984). Parturition in wild gorillas: Behaviour of mothers, neonates, and others. Folia Primatol. 42: 62–69. Suzuki, S., and Tsukahara, T. (1996). Variation in social mechanisms by which males attained the alpha rank among Japanese macaques. In: Evolution and Ecology of Macaque Societies, Fa J. E., and Lindburg D. G. (eds.), pp. 444–458. Takahata, Y. (1982). Social relations between adult male and females of Japanese monkeys in the Arashiyama B troop. Primates 23: 1–23. Thomsen, R. (1997). Observation of periparturitional behaviour in wild Yakushima macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui). Folia Primatol. 68: 338–341. Wrangham, R. W. (1980). An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour 75: 262–300.