Neural synchrony and selective attention The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation Desimone, R. “Neural synchrony and selective attention.” Neural Networks, 2009. IJCNN 2009. International Joint Conference on. 2009. 683-684. © 2009 IEEE As Published http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/IJCNN.2009.5179097 Publisher Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers Version Final published version Accessed Thu May 26 06:32:08 EDT 2016 Citable Link http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/58973 Terms of Use Article is made available in accordance with the publisher's policy and may be subject to US copyright law. Please refer to the publisher's site for terms of use. Detailed Terms Proceedings of International Joint Conference on Neural Networks, Atlanta, Georgia, USA, June 14-19, 2009 Neural Synchrony and Selective Attention Robert Desimone Abstract- A complex visual scene will typically contain many different objects, few of which are currently relevant to behavior. Thus, attentional mechanisms are needed to select the relevant objects from the scene and to reject the irrelevant ones. Neurophysiological studies in monkeys have identified some of the neural mechanisms of attentional selection within the ventral, "object recognition", stream of the cortex, which begins with area VI and continues through areas V2, V4, and IT cortex. At each stage along this stream, attended, or behaviorally relevant, stim uli are processed preferentially compared to irrelevant distracters. The source of the attentional feedback to visual cortex seems to originate in parietal and prefrontal cortex. We proposed some years ago that this attentional feedback biased competitive interactions among neurons in visual cortex, in favor of neuronal responses to the most behaviorally relevant stimulus. More recent work indicates that these competitive interactions are one aspect of a more general visual mechanism for contrast normalization, which is present in most or all visual areas. By providing the appropriate input to this normalization mechanism, feedback from parietal and prefrontal cortex appears to shift the balance of visual cortical responses towards the attended stimulus. I. NEURAL SYNCHRONY AND SELECTIVE ATTENTION A complex visual scene will typically contain many different objects, few of which are currently relevant to behavior. Thus, attentional mechanisms are needed to select the relevant objects from the scene and to reject the irrelevant ones. Neurophysiological studies in monkeys have identified some of the neural mechanisms of attentional selection within the ventral, "object recognition", stream of the cortex, which begins with area V1 and continues through areas V2, V4, and IT cortex (Moran and Desimone 1985; Reynolds, Pasternak et al. 2000; Chelazzi, Miller et al. 2001; Reynolds and Desimone 2003). At each stage along this stream, attended, or behaviorally relevant, stimuli are processed preferentially compared to irrelevant distracters. The source of the attentional feedback to visual cortex seems to originate in parietal and prefrontal cortex. We proposed some years ago that this attentional feedback biased competitive interactions among neurons in visual cortex, in favor of neuronal responses to the most behaviorally relevant stimulus (Desimone and Duncan 1995). More recent work (Reynolds and Heeger 2009) indicates that these competitive interactions are one aspect of a more general visual mechanism for contrast normalization, which is present in most or all visual areas. By providing the appropriate input to this normalization mechanism, feedback Manuscript received March 30, 2009. Robert Desimone Ph.D., is Doris and Don Berkey Professor of Neuroscience, Director, McGovern Institute for Brain Research at MIT, USA. 978-1-4244-3553-1/09/$25.00 ©2009 IEEE from parietal and prefrontal cortex appears to shift the balance of visual cortical responses towards the attended stimulus. In recent years, we have focused on the detailed mechanisms by which the attentional feedback affects cells in visual cortex. Our work has suggested that the attentional bias is mediated, at least in part, in visual cortex through an increase in high-frequency (gamma) synchronization of neurons carrying critical information about the location or features of the behaviorally relevant stimulus (Fries, Reynolds et al. 2001; Liang, Bressler et al. 2003; Fries, Womelsdorf et al. 2008). Increases in gamma synchrony are found during both spatial attention and featural attention engaged during visual search (Bichot, Rossi et al. 2005), and the presence of synchrony in visual cortex predicts faster responses in visual tasks (Womelsdorf, Fries et al. 2006). Recent evidence shows that inputs from the frontal eye fields (FEF) in prefrontal cortex initiates coupled gammafrequency oscillations between FEF and area V4 in the ventral stream during attention, and these oscillations are phase, or time-shifted to allow for conduction and synaptic delays between the two areas, thereby achieving maximally effective communication(Gregoriou, Gotts et al. 2009). Attentional effects in V2 and VI, which are earlier stages in the ventral stream, occur later in V4, suggesting that attentional feedback works in a "backwards", step by step through the ventral stream. Our unpublished human experiments using magnetoencephalograpy (MEG) show results that are remarkably similar to that found in animals. Specifically, with attention, neural activity in visual cortex becomes synchronized with that in the prefrontal and parietal cortex, which a phase shift that seems to allow for the necessary conduction and synaptic delays. Cross-area synchrony may be a general mechanism for regulating information flow through the brain (Buschman and Miller 2007; Saalmann, Pigarev et al. 2007; Lakatos, Karmos et al. 2008) and for regulating spike-timing dependent plasticity. REFERENCES [1] [2] [3] [4] Bichot, N. P., A. F. Rossi, et al. (2005). "Parallel and serial neural mechanisms for visual search in macaque area V4." Science 308(5721): 529-34. Buschman, T. 1. and E. K. Miller (2007). "Top-down versus bottomup control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices." Science 315(5820): 1860-2. Chelazzi, L., E. K. Miller, et al. (2001). "Responses of neurons in macaque area V4 during memory-guided visual search." Cereb Cortex 11(8): 761-72. Desimone, R. and 1. Duncan (1995). "Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention." Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18: 193-222. 683 [5] [6] [7] [8] [9] Fries, P., 1. H. Reynolds, et al. (2001). "Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. " Science 291(5508): 1560-3. Fries, P., T. Womelsdorf, et al. (2008). "The effects of visual stimulation and selective visual attention on rhythmic neuronal synchronization in macaque area V4." J Neurosci 28(18): 4823-35. Gregoriou, G. G., S. 1. Gotts, et al. (2009). High-frequency long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science, in press. Lakatos, P., G. Karmos, et al. (2008). "Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection." Science 320(5872): 110-3. Liang, H., S. Bressler, et al. (2003). "Temporal dynamics of attentionmodulated neuronal synchronization in macaque V4." Neurocomputing 52-54: 481 - 487. [10] Moran, J. and R. Desimone (1985). "Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex." Science 229: 782-784. [11] Reynolds, 1. H. and R. Desimone (2003). "Interacting roles of attention and visual salience in V4." Neuron 37(5): 853-63. [12] Reynolds, 1. H. and D. 1. Heeger (2009). "The normalization model of attention. " Neuron 61(2): 168-85. [13] Reynolds, 1. H., T. Pasternak, et al. (2000). "Attention increases sensitivity ofV4 neurons." Neuron 26(3): 703-14. [14] Saalmann, Y. B., I. N. Pigarev, et al. (2007). "Neural mechanisms of visual attention: how top-down feedback highlights relevant locations." Science 316(5831): 1612-5. [15] Womelsdorf, T., P. Fries, et al. (2006). "Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection." Nature 439(7077): 733-6. 684