TREE 1183 1–2 Update Book Review The fieldworkers’ wild truths The Infanticide Controversy: Primatology and the Art of Field Science by Amanda Rees. University of Chicago Press, 2009. US$40.00 hbk (288 pages) ISBN: 9 780 22670711 2 Volker Sommer OO ED PR interpretation, which has earned me distinguished labels such biological determinist or sexist... According to Rees, it is the fieldworker’s regress that generated the heat between opponents. She explicitly denies that the controversy could have been stirred up by intellectual content alone, such as that of the emerging field of ‘sociobiology’ with its emphasis on competition as a driving force in sociality. Here, she disagrees with other reconstructions of clashes between adaptionists and non-adaptionists (e.g. Refs [3,4]), which relate them to philosophical dichotomies such as nature versus nurture, Rousseau versus Locke, individual versus group selection, and so on. Rees’ reconstruction, similar to former ones, operates in a monolingual Anglo-Saxon horizon. I admit to my own shortcomings in, for example, Mandarin, French or Kiswahili. However, I do know that the complementary debate about conspecific killings in the German-speaking world was not sparked by naturalistic animal studies, but a recent history of militarism and fascism (Refs [5,6] versus Refs [7,8]). Another missing facet is some in-depth engagement with infant killings not committed by the stereotypical big-toothed males, but the allegedly nurturing females, be they gorillas, marmosets, chimpanzees, or humans. Rees’ focus is thus relatively narrow. This enables her to use a stronger magnifying glass, which makes for a fine-grained narration. By contrast, readers can lose the plot, given a multitude of names, institutions, dates, places and species. A timeline might have helped through this maze (and alerted the author to some confusions of locations and time). The book incorporates 20 interviews, most with primate fieldworkers, and many of whom are key players in the controversy. Only three interviewees oppose adaptive explanations (Sussman, Cheverud, Rowell). Sadly, a fresh memorandum from the iconic figure of the resistance movement, Phyllis (Jay) Dolhinow, is missing altogether. Most interviewees, including myself, are hardliners favouring adaptive explanations, such as Sarah Hrdy, who developed the sexual selection hypothesis, or the behavioural ecologists Packer, Pusey or Borries who applied Hrdy’s theoretical framework to their own field data. The skewed ratio, so I guess, reflects the current balance of opinions. Rees’ ‘symmetrical’ outlook thus bestows disproportionate credibility on the marginal party of non-adaptionists. But then, what would I do with my venom, if my opponents were finally silent or silenced? Amanda Rees did well, given her limited aims. The book is full of information, and will serve as a repository of CO RR E CT Exchanging pointed and often nasty arguments (however silly in hindsight) invigorates the participants of academic debates and entertains bystanders. The Infanticide Controversy reconstructs the history of a particularly vicious and drawn-out brawl [1,2]. One faction, the adaptionists, maintains that the killing of immature animals by conspecifics is targeted and follows regular patterns reflecting underlying principles, such as resource competition, cannibalism or intra-sexual selection between males. For the non-adaptionist faction, infant killings are, at best, a pathology or sad mistake, but more probably products of the imagination of sloppy scientists tainted by Zeitgeist demons such individualism or capitalism. I longed for a juicy helping of controversy about the controversy. Instead, The Infanticide Controversy served an overdose of impartiality. This probably indicates the quality in the discipline that spurned the investigation: history and sociology of science. The author vows to have ‘no business making any judgement as to the truth or falsity of any claims about the origin or maintenance of animal behaviour’. This sounds alarmingly like trivial postmodernism, although the variant comes in the more modest disguise of ‘methodological relativism’. Such an approach instructs the faithful sociologist to refrain from ‘judgements on scientific truth or falsity’. Fortunately, for a hot-blooded scientist like myself, who does not aim for ‘symmetrical explanations’ and loves a good spat, Amanda Rees embodies the more pragmatic breed of sociologists who do ‘not dispute the existence of fact’. Prolonged exposure to her soft-spoken storytelling thus put me in touch with my inner sociologist who readily admits to being subjective because ‘facts do not speak for themselves but are spoken for by scientists’. Rees also succeeds in educating me about the notion of the ‘experimenter’s regress’, the conundrum that, although experiments are at the heart of the scientific method, ‘it is never possible to replicate an experiment precisely’. Fieldwork in particular is subjugated to this logic, as a subsequent event such as yet another infant mauling is never identical to the original, requiring a certain ‘interpretive flexibility’. Consequently, findings ‘yield contradictory interpretations where controversy participants held different worldviews’. This, in a nutshell, is the book’s message: a rather muted take on what are stormy scuffles. I know what I am talking about, given that I hold the selfproclaimed world record in witnessing infanticides among wild primates, tied to an undisputed inflexibility about the F Department of Anthropology, UCL, London, WC1E 6BT, UK UN 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 Corresponding author: Sommer, V. (v.sommer@ucl.ac.uk). 1 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 TREE 1183 1–2 Update 0169-5347/$ – see front matter ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2009.08.008 Available online xxxxxx CO RR E CT ED PR 1 Parmigiani, S. and vom Saal, F., eds (1994) Infanticide and Parental Care, Harwood Academic Publishers 2 van Schaik, C. and Janson, C., eds (2000) Infanticide by Males and its Implications, Cambridge University Press F References 3 Haraway, D.J. (1989) Primate Visions: Gender, Race, and Nature in the World of Modern Science, Routledge 4 Segerstråle, U. (2001) Defenders of the Truth. The Sociobiology Debate, Oxford University Press 5 Lorenz, K. (1963) Das sogenannte Böse. Zur Naturgeschichte der Aggression, Borotha-Schoeler 6 Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1997) Die Biologie des menschlichen Verhaltens. Grundriss der Humanethologie, (3rd edn), Seehammer 7 Sommer, V. (1996) Heilige Egoisten. Die Soziobiologie indischer Tempelaffen, C.H. Beck 8 Vogel, C. (1989) Vom Töten zum Mord. Das wirkliche Böse in der Evolutionsgeschichte, Hanser OO knowledge on the controversy for years to come. Her work might also enable fruitful synergies between social and natural sciences. Finally, her book can generate data about the question of how much impartiality a scientist can stomach. Such merciless self-experiment led, in my own case, to the firm resolution to remain as judgemental as possible when it comes to controversies. UN 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 Trends in Ecology and Evolution Vol.xxx No.x 2 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148