ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2002, 63, 637–648 doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1897, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on ARTICLES Postconflict behaviour of wild Indian langur monkeys: avoidance of opponents but rarely affinity VOLKER SOMMER, ALISON DENHAM & KATHERINE LITTLE Department of Anthropology, University College London (Received 24 November 1999; initial acceptance 11 February 2000; final acceptance 29 August 2001; MS. number: 6414R) Friendly postconflict (PC) interactions between former opponents have been described for a large number of primate species held in captivity. We investigated conflict management behaviour for wild Hanuman langurs, Semnopithecus (Presbytis) entellus entellus, in India, using a large sample of over 6000 agonistic and affinitive interactions recorded in one one-male/multifemale troop and three all-male bands. We compared dyadic PC affinity with the dyadic baseline affinity during the overall observation time to minimize biases of traditional matched-control samples. PC affinity was recorded during only 15% of all dyads (immature male–immature male 0%, adult male–adult male 0%, adult male–immature male 5%, adult male–adult female 0%, adult female–adult female 42%). PC affinity is probably absent amongst males because their dominance relationships are strongly asymmetrical, leaving little room for emotional insecurity. Dyads among females, on the other hand, reflect frequent rank changes and close kinship which probably corresponds to higher levels of emotional uncertainty and greater need for PC affinity. Overall, PC affinity reached only 26% of the randomly expected value. Thus, the vast majority of langur monkey opponents avoided each other after conflicts. Avoidance as a low-cost option for group-living animals to cope with conflicts is often not possible in captivity. This suggests that reports of high rates of reconciliation may be at least partly artefacts of captivity. 2002 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. Members of many primate species approach former opponents shortly after a conflict and initiate sociopositive behaviours such as embracing, grooming or huddling (see reviews in Walters & Seyfarth 1987; Kappeler & van Schaik 1992; Silk 1998; Aureli & de Waal 2000). Such responses are believed to be part of conflict management brought about by selective pressures that favour cooperation. Friendly reunions may reduce tension and anxiety, increase tolerance and decrease the likelihood of future conflict, thus effectively serving as ‘reconciliation’ (sensu de Waal & van Roosmalen 1979). However, a reconciliatory function cannot easily be demonstrated and not all sociopositive postconflict interactions might reflect direct efforts to repair disharmonized relationships. Some authors therefore prefer more descriptive terms such as ‘postconflict friendly reunion’ or ‘peaceful postconflict contact’ (see Cords 1993 and definitions in Aureli & de Waal 2000, page 387f). Correspondence: V. Sommer, Department of Anthropology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, U.K. (email V.Sommer@ucl.ac.uk). 0003–3472/02/$35.00/0 Various hypotheses try to explain the occurrence of friendly reunion. The minimum cognitive capacity hypothesis (Gallup 1982) assumes that only certain species are capable of displaying such behaviour but does not address its potential adaptive value. The social evolution hypothesis assumes that conflict management is a condition for group living and thus predicts a roughly equal rate of reconciled conflicts for all dyads (de Waal 1986b). The dominance style hypothesis expects more egalitarian species to reconcile more often than those with despotic hierarchies (de Waal 1989). Other explanations expect friendly postconflict contact only for particular dyads. The reconciled hierarchy hypothesis predicts that dominant animals grant reconciliation to opponents in species with formalized submissive signals (de Waal 1986b). The good relationship hypothesis predicts that certain group members reconcile to maintain valuable interindividual relationships (de Waal 1986a). Finally, the dominance asymmetry hypothesis states that postconflict reunions are more likely if the resource-holding potential of opponents is similar, rendering the relationship more symmetrical and thus 637 2002 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. 638 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 63, 4 generating insecurity about who is dominant and who subordinate (Thierry 1990). Friendly reunion after conflict (i.e. reconciliation) has been reported for about 30 primate species (Aureli & de Waal 2000, page 383) and is particularly frequent for the genera Macaca (see Matsumura 1996, and references therein) and Pan (e.g. de Waal & van Roosmalen 1979; de Waal 1987). However, current research suffers from various shortcomings: for example (1) the variability between groups of the same species (cf. de Waal & Luttrell 1989; Call et al. 1996; Castles et al. 1996) is rarely addressed; (2) the majority of studies investigate only a small fraction of the dyads that exist in a given group; (3) all but a handful of reports (see Discussion) are restricted to captivity; and (4) few studies refer to colobines (McKenna 1977; Ren et al. 1991; Björnsdotter et al. 2000). The lack of field studies is especially regrettable since postconflict behaviour in the wild may differ from that in captivity where animals cannot easily maintain their distance from a former opponent. In addition, interspecies variability can be understood only if a wider range of social systems is investigated, and colobines are certainly underrepresented in this respect. In this paper we attempt to fill this gap by presenting the first data on wild colobines. The data were recorded in Jodhpur, northwest India, for two types of groups of Indian langur monkeys: a one-male–multifemale troop and three all-male bands. The data set is one of the largest ever used in a study on postconflict interactions and includes several dyadic categories (male–female, female– female, male–male). We expected postconflict contacts to differ between these types of dyads since they reflect varying balances of cooperation and competition. For example, females are philopatric and closely interrelated (facilitating high levels of cooperation) whereas males change groups frequently and are hardly ever in the company of related adult conspecifics (translating into high levels of competition). Such differences may allow us to assess the validity of some of the functional hypotheses about friendly postconflict contacts. METHODS Study Site and Population Jodhpur (240 m above sea level) is in northwest India in the state of Rajasthan at the eastern edge of the Great Indian Desert. The climate is dry, with maximum temperatures of up to 50C during May and June and minimum temperatures around OC during December and January. Jodhpur receives 90% of its scanty rainfall (annual average 360 mm) during the monsoon (July– September). The town stands on a hilly sandstone plateau which covers ca. 85 km2. The plateau is inhabited by a geographically isolated population of about 1200–1300 Hanuman langurs, Semnopithecus (Presbytis) entellus entellus (Rajpurohit & Sommer 1991), which has been monitored since 1967 (Mohnot 1974). All langur groups forage on the natural vegetation, which is xerophytic open scrub, and some groups raid crops. In addition, the majority are fed by local people for religious reasons. Proportions of natural, provisioned and raided food items vary considerably between groups (Sommer & Mendoza-Granados 1994). The reproductive units are 27–29 multifemale/one-male troops (average size 38.5 members, range 7–93). Each troop occupies a home range of ca. 0.5–1.3 km2. Males emigrate from their natal troops and join one of the 12–14 all-male bands (average size 11.8 members, range 2–47). Band ranges comprise 1.5–3.5 km2 but are not well defined, because individual males can move over areas of more than 20 km2. Bands invade troops in an unpredictable pattern. Changes of the adult resident male can be rapid takeovers (occurring in days) or gradual processes (up to several months) with temporary multimale stages and successive short adult male tenures (interim residencies; Sommer 1988). Residencies of single adult males in bisexual troops range from just a few days to at least 74.0 months, with an average of 26.5 months. Stable onemale situations are typical for 94.5% of a given troop’s history. Therefore, troops are termed ‘harems’ (Sommer & Rajpurohit 1989; Sommer et al. 1992). Data Collection The present study comprises about 2000 h of observation by V.S. on one troop and three bands living under varying ecological conditions (Table 1). All group members were known individually. Study periods lasted between 11 and 17 months. Observations were approximately equally distributed throughout the day (0600– 1800 hours) and year. Data are presented on all group members with the exception of infant and juvenile members of the harem troop. These individuals were still dependent on their mothers and not yet integrated into the dominance hierarchy. The analysis is based on a distinction between agonistic and affinitive interactions briefly categorized below (for details, see e.g. Hrdy 1977; Dolhinow 1978; Sommer 1996). We collected data ad libitum (Altmann 1974) while focal-animal sampling on other topics was carried out simultaneously. The data sets were pooled. The observer changed his position within the group every 30 min with rotation of the randomly selected focal animal, thus minimizing the potential bias that particular individuals were overrepresented in the ad libitum sample. In a forested area, ad libitum records underreported langur interactions by 22% compared to pure focal-animal samples (Podzuweit 1994). The bias is probably smaller in the open Jodhpur habitat. For the same reason, it is also unlikely that affinitive acts will be recorded less than agonistic acts which are typically more conspicuous. While this sampling procedure does not easily allow a context analysis it greatly increases the data density necessary to calculate baseline rates for individual dyads (cf. Cords 1993). The data set includes 37 monkeys. All of them were focal animals except two adult males who succeeded each other as harem residents of the bisexual troop. Interactions of these two males were recorded ad libitum and SOMMER ET AL.: POSTCONFLICT BEHAVIOUR OF LANGURS Table 1. Details of the study groups of langur monkeys at Jodhpur and methodology Male band Harem troop Kailana-I Machiya Canal Chopasani Nov 1981–Sep 1982 288 Sep 1986–Dec 1987 72 Sep 1986–Mar 1988 56 Oct 1986–Feb 1988 59 6F 2F, 1M 3F 1 2 2 3 4F, 3M 3F, 4M 26 20.4 2 1 6 1 1 1 4 10 8.8 4 3.6 10 7.0 Focal observations Hours‡ Animals Dyads Mean h/dyad Minimum h/dyad Maximum h/dyad 660 All adult females 77 573 234 660 515 All males 45 384 79 515 397 All males 6 316 234 397 420 All males 42 207 10 420 Habitat§ Range size (ha) Open scrub Gardens, fields Human habitations Water access 75 +++ — +, — +++ 349 +++ — +, — +, — 165 ++ ++ + ++ 142 + +++ ++ +++ Diet (annual feeding time) Provisioned food (%) Natural food (%) Crop raiding (%) 22.2 77.8 0.0 2.4 97.7 0.0 3.0 93.8 3.2 15.6 64.6 19.8 Observation period Total days* Group composition† Adult (old) Adult (middle-aged) Adult (young) Immature (subadult) Immature (juvenile) Immature (infant) Total members Mean members/month *In bands mostly dawn-to-dusk follows. †For age classification, see Rajpurohit et al. 1995. F=female; M=male. The harem troop’s adult male (given as 1M) was replaced once during the study period (June 1982). Bands contained only males; not all were present throughout the study because of group transfers. Some individuals changed age classes during the study. ‡Continuous focal-animal sampling with almost equal diurnal and annual distribution. Additional scan and ad libitum sampling was carried out during most study periods. Focal hours=subsample of present study. §+ + + =much/many; + + =moderate; + =little/few, —=none. in conjunction with focal samples of their 11 adult females. Agonistic acts (roughly in order of increasing intensity; cf. Borries et al. 1991) were as follows. (1) Being avoided: A moves towards B; B anticipates A’s approach and moves away. (2) Displace (‘A causes B to leave’): A and B are close to each other or A approaches B. The individuals will sometimes have aggressive acoustic, visual or tactile contact (see categories below). B moves away. A occupies position or resource previously controlled by B. (3) Vocalize at: A directs grunts, guttural vocalization or teeth grinding (canines or molars) towards B. (4) Threat: A bares canines towards B, often while vocalizing (face threat); A raises hand(s) towards B (hand threat); A jumps towards B (lunge threat). (5) Fence: A prevents B from approaching further or acquiring a resource, typically through briefly raising both arms and threatening A. (6) Hit or kick: A hits or kicks B, typically while A is stationary. (7) Pull or push ear or fur: A pulls/pushes ear(s)/fur of B with one or both hands. (8) Jump at: A jumps into B’s flank or on to B’s back, often quadrupedally, typically while B moves quickly. (9) Chase: A runs after B who flees. (10) Bite: in females, virtually always ‘mock biting’; males sometimes draw blood. Affinitive acts (roughly in order of increasing intensity; cf. Borries et al. 1994) were as follows. (1) Grunt-and-grin: Mild vocalization accompanied by bared-teeth display. (2) Pat jaw: A strokes B’s jaw or pulls hair around B’s face. (3) Pull head: A pulls B’s head towards A. (4) Kiss: A muzzles B’s head, often the jaw and lip region. (5) Embrace: A puts arms around neck or back of B who will often reciprocate. One or several of the preceding categories could initiate an embrace or be associated with it. (6) Move in circle: two animals describe a circle while embracing each other. 639 640 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 63, 4 (7) Invite allogrooming: A presents body parts to B, typically flank or back or belly while raising arm(s). (8) Allogrooming: coordinated movement through a groupmate’s fur with one or both hands or mouth. Particles may be removed in the process. Agonistic female–female encounters are short events of usually not more than a few seconds. Male–male conflicts can be prolonged and may result in wounding (e.g. Dolhinow 1972; Hrdy 1977). Affinitive embrace interactions do not exceed a few seconds but most allogrooming is considerably longer (among adult troop females: 5.9–68 min, N=1194; among male band members: 1.9– 23 min, N=2005; Sommer 1996). For the purpose of this study, we considered all agonistic and affinitive acts to be events and not states. The analysis is thus based on frequencies of behaviours. Combinations of consecutive agonistic acts (e.g. pull fur+ hit; bite+chase) and affinitive acts (e.g. pat jaw+embrace; invite allogrooming+allogrooming) within the same minute were counted as only a single event. Continuous interactions, mostly allogrooming, that span several minutes were counted as a single event; an interruption had to last for at least 30 s before an interaction qualified as a new event. Transforming grooming states into grooming events is justified because grooming duration and grooming frequency are highly correlated in Jodhpur langurs (rs =0.964, Borries et al. 1994). Polyadic conflicts are rare in langurs and almost absent amongst females (cf. Borries et al. 1991; Borries 1994); mutual grooming (A grooms B while B grooms A; rare) and polyadic grooming (e.g. A and B groom C; not uncommon) were broken down into dyads. Some related types of interaction were excluded. (1) Sexual harassment: copulations of the harem holder were frequently disturbed by one or several females (Sommer 1989). The male responded with threats or chases. Since these resembled ‘mock attacks’ and were often polyadic, they were excluded from the analysis. (2) Infanticide: one of the two harem holders was infanticidal, resulting in various aggressive attacks and joint defence by females (Sommer 1987). Infanticidal attacks in conjunction with the replacement of harem residents can be expected only every 2.2 years and thus represent a particular type of conflict outside the focus of the present study. (3) Mounting: same-sex dorsoventral contacts resembling a heterosexual copulation were excluded because they represent an ambiguous mixture of agonistic and affinitive patterns (Srivastava et al. 1991). (4) Huddling: this behaviour, although common amongst many Old World primates, is very rarely seen in the Jodhpur langurs. It was not observed during the study period (apart from females who held immatures and huddled over them). (5) Proximity: close spatial contact is commonly seen but much of it seemed to be caused by the need to share shady or comfortable spots rather than being a reflection of affiliation. Several other studies explicitly excluded decreased interindividual distances because of such an ambiguous motivational basis (cf. Kappeler & van Schaik 1992). The exclusion of proximity is also the reason that we avoid the broader term ‘affiliation’ in favour of the more specific ‘affinity’. Data Analysis The ways in which postconflict contact is measured alter the results of an analysis (see review in Kappeler & van Schaik 1992). Many previous studies compared behaviour during postconflict observations (PC) with that during matched-control observations (MC). Suitable MCs should correspond closely with characteristics of the PC. MCs are often carried out the following day at the same time and for the same length of time as the corresponding PC. The PC/MC method has various disadvantages. (1) It is basically designed for captivity. (2) It is questionable if MCs can ever reproduce all conditions of the corresponding PC, for example, with respect to proximity of opponents after a conflict has occurred. In the field, it is close to impossible to define MCs that sufficiently match the environmental and social context of PCs, such as subgroup composition, spatial distribution of group members, visibility, food availability and intake, temperature and external disturbances including predation risk. These variations affect how likely it is that an affinitive interaction between certain individuals will occur during the MC, devaluating PC–MC comparisons considerably. (2) The concentration on specific PC/MC pairs will typically result in a rather sketchy documentation of the overall dyadic pattern of interaction and often yields dyadic samples of less than 10. (3) The period following a conflict is likely to be influenced by its outcome, for example whether the opponents reconcile, and is thus not an appropriate ‘neutral’ comparison (Silk 1997). Therefore, we did not analyse specific MC samples. Instead, we calculated interaction rates for individual dyads, and compared the (observed) rates of dyadic PC affinity during a given time window after the conflict to the (expected) rates of dyadic baseline affinity during the overall observation time, a variation of the ‘rate method’ (de Waal 1987; Judge 1997) and the ‘time rule method’ (Aureli et al. 1989). We recorded PC affinity immediately after an aggressive interaction. Consecutive affinitive events (e.g. pat jaw plus embrace) within the same PC minute were counted only once. Continuous affinity that spanned several minutes was counted as a single event unless the interaction was interrupted for more than 30 s. In reality, interruptions of PC affinity were almost nonexistent. The PC period was defined as being 3 min long since other studies have mostly found that friendly reunions occur immediately after the conflict (Veneema et al. 1987). The likelihood of recording affinitive PC interactions in standard PC–MC studies decreases with longer PC periods because of increased background noise caused by affiliation during the baseline situations (Veneema et al. 1994). Our own method is not prone to such bias since all recorded affinitive events are analysed anyway. As an additional control, we calculated affinity during a 15-min PC window to minimize the likelihood of false negatives. We tested whether observed PC affinity was above expected baseline levels of affinity. Baseline levels were defined as affinitive acts/h of observation time while two animals had the opportunity for dyadic interaction, that SOMMER ET AL.: POSTCONFLICT BEHAVIOUR OF LANGURS is, were members of the same group. This procedure controls for monkeys that died, disappeared, immigrated, emigrated or were temporarily absent from the group. The latter refers to males only, as bands regularly split in a fusion–fission pattern. Such subunits may lose visual and acoustic contact with each other. The subunits will often merge again during the day, but males may also leave particular bands for several days or weeks (for histories of study groups, see Sommer et al. 1992; Rajpurohit et al. 1995). Male bands as well as harem troops were otherwise very cohesive. The following is an example of the calculation of rates of dyadic PC affinity and dyadic baseline affinity. Adult females 12 and 11 were permanent members of troop Kailana-I during the 660 h of observation in that group (cf. Table 1). During this period, 24 agonistic and 61 affinitive interactions were recorded for this dyad which translates into an expected baseline rate of PC affinity of 0.11/h (61 affinitive interactions1.2 h/660 h). Two affinitive interactions occurred within 3 min of an agonistic interaction, thus counting as PC affinity. PC observation time was 1.2 h (24 events3 min=72 min). This translates into an observed rate of PC affinity of 1.67/h which is well above the baseline. There is no statistical method to test whether observed and expected PC affinity differ significantly since pairs had at best a few affinitive PC interactions (see Results). Other authors also refrain from tests of significance even if they analyse dyadic interactions via the PC–MC method. For example, these count as ‘attracted pairs’ (i.e. reconciled dyads) where PC affinity ‘occurred earlier in the PC than in the MC observations’ (Call et al. 1999: 166). ‘Earlier’ is as descriptive as our use of ‘above or below expectation’. Both approaches reflect difficulties with small sample sizes which primatological research will often face. We refrain from calling affinitive PC interactions above baseline levels ‘reconciliations’ because we do not know enough about the motivational state of the former opponents. Nevertheless, one could argue that, from a heuristic point of view, PC affinity above baseline levels could serve as an operational definition (cf. Cords 1993) of reconciliation. Our baseline method requires two additional steps. (1) Affinitive acts during the first 3 min at the beginning of each observation block were discarded because they might represent a PC affinity that followed an agonistic act that took place just before observations began. Agonistic acts during the last 2 min of a given observation block were also discarded since they might be followed by affinitive PC interactions just after the observation ended. (2) Baseline affinity might be inflated against PC affinity if aggression occurs at times with lower overall affiliation. Theoretically, aggression could be more common during travel or feeding whereas affinity could be more common during resting. However, this was not the case since the hourly distribution of agonistic and affinitive events covaried and did not differ significantly (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test: D=0.065, N=13, NS). Nevertheless, our method is not without biases. Unlike ‘traditional’ MC samples, we did not try to select situ- ations similar to the PC, for example with respect to close spatial distance of two animals. The procedure will therefore bias against rates of affinitive baseline interactions (perhaps even more than an MC sample). However, a reverse bias is introduced by the ad libitum sampling since sometimes either the agonistic or the affinitive interaction in the context of a conflict will go unrecorded. This lack of records will bias against rates of PC affinity, thus reducing the first error. On the other hand, our method enables us to sample many more PC situations than traditional studies, whether or not one wants to conduct additional baseline calculations. The combination of ad libitum with focalanimal sampling ensures a data density that allows the reconstruction of solid sociometric matrices which provide background information about dominance ranks, grooming networks and long-term dynamics of social relationships (e.g. Borries et al. 1991, 1994). Furthermore, the method can be used a posteriori and might facilitate analysis of field data for other species, which are urgently needed. RESULTS We identified 1365 agonistic events and 4862 affinitive events in the four monkey groups (Fig. 1). However, a mere 43 affinitive postconflict (PC) interactions were recorded within 3 min of conflicts (Fig. 2) while a single other affinitive interaction occurred up to 15 min PC. PC affinity was restricted to 25 of 170 dyads (14.7%), representing a small fraction (26.1%) of the randomly expected interactions (calculation: 3 min following each of 1365 agonistic events amount to 68.3 h, which equal 3.4% of total observations; in this time, 3.4% of all 4862 affinitive events should have occurred, i.e. 165). No PC affinity was recorded in band Canal and only single episodes in bands Machiya (1.7% of potential dyads) and Chopasani (1.2% of potential dyads). In the harem troop, 41 interactions were recorded in 23 of 76 dyads (30.3% of potential dyads), with 6 of 23 dyads being reciprocal. The number of affinitive PC events was either one (12 dyads), two (10 dyads), three (one dyad) or four (two dyads). PC events exceeded the expected dyadic rates in all recorded instances. Thus, 120 of 145 dyads (82.8%) had agonistic interactions but no PC affinity. The vast majority of former opponents not only failed to establish peaceful PC contacts but also avoided affinity with each other after conflicts. Rates of interactions broken down for sex and age (AF=adult female, AM=adult male, IM=immature male) differed markedly (Fig. 3a). AM–AM dyads had the most frequent agonistic and affinitive contact. All other age-sex classes had much lower overall rates but affinity was 4–9 times higher than agonism amongst AM–AF, AF–AF and IM–IM dyads. Percentages of dyads with agonistic/ affinitive interactions were as follows: AM–AF (22 dyads) 81.8/95.4%; AF–AF (55 dyads) 100.0/100.0%; AM–AM (17 dyads) 88.2/100.0%; AM–IM (50 dyads) 76.0/86.0%; IM–IM (26 dyads) 73.0/100.0%. 641 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 63, 4 (a) Harem troop Kailana-1 Recipient Agonism Actor Age Rank Female F12 F11 F4 F7 F1 F13 F2 F8 F3 F6 F9 M20 M43 Y 1.3 F12 0.2 0.6 0.3 0.5 0.2 Y 2.3 F11 3.5 0.8 0.5 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.2 0.3 0.3 0.6 1.3 0.4 1.3 O 4.0 F4 0.6 2.0 0.9 0.2 0.6 0.2 0.3 0.2 0.2 0.4 M 4.0 F7 1.1 1.4 0.8 O 5.0 F1 1.2 2.4 0.2 0.3 0.2 0.2 Y 5.3 F13 0.5 2.1 0.5 0.5 0.6 O 7.0 F2 3.3 2.4 2.1 0.8 2.1 1.1 0.8 0.2 0.6 0.2 0.2 0.6 0.4 0.5 0.4 0.4 O 7.0 F8 0.8 0.8 0.9 0.6 0.6 1.5 0.3 0.2 0.6 0.8 0.2 0.2 0.4 0.4 0.2 0.9 O 97 F3 0.9 0.8 0.9 1.5 1.8 1.2 1.2 1.4 0.3 0.6 0.2 0.4 O 10.0 F6 1.2 2.9 1.4 2.0 1.5 1.5 2.6 2.6 2.1 0.5 0.6 M A 10.3 F9 M20 0.5 2.7 0.8 0.2 1.7 0.4 0.8 0.6 0.9 1.1 0.2 2.4 1.9 1.1 1.7 A M43 2.0 1.0 10.0 1.0 0.6 Affinity 644 Events Female F12 F11 F4 F7 F1 F13 F2 F8 F3 F6 F9 M20 M43 F12 Female F12 F11 F4 F7 F1 F13 F2 F8 F3 F6 F9 M20 M43 F12 3.6 9.1 5.6 4.2 6.5 1.8 2.9 5.0 4.8 2.7 F11 5.6 7.9 6.7 8.5 5.0 1.7 2.6 2.9 1.1 1.5 F4 10.5 1.8 3.5 2.4 3.6 1.1 1.7 1.4 1.2 2.1 F7 7.9 5.8 6.2 3.9 3.6 3.3 3.5 3.3 2.0 1.4 F1 6.5 4.7 2.4 4.7 5.2 0.6 2.0 1.5 1.1 0.5 F13 12.9 3.6 6.1 3.2 3.3 F2 3.8 1.1 3.0 3.5 0.9 5.6 0.9 2.6 2.1 3.6 5.3 7.6 2.6 0.6 4.8 0.2 F8 3.3 2.0 2.4 3.2 0.8 3.0 3.3 1.2 1.8 2.6 0.2 0.4 F3 8.9 2.0 2.4 3.5 2.0 3.2 2.3 2.4 0.6 1.7 F6 7.1 0.6 2.4 2.4 1.1 4.7 1.1 2.3 2.0 0.6 0.2 F9 M20 9.7 1.0 1.2 0.2 4.8 1.7 3.2 0.2 0.6 0.8 10.0 3.6 2.4 0.8 4.2 2.1 2.3 0.8 0.2 3.1 1.5 M43 0.4 3.4 4.3 6.4 14.1 3.4 9.8 6.4 13.3 0.9 1.3 PC affinity 2747 Events 0.3 0.3 F11 0.3 0.2 F4 0.3 F7 F1 F13 F2 0.2 0.3 0.3 F8 0.2 F3 F6 0.2 M43 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.3 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.5 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.3 41 Events (b) Male band Machiya Actor Agonism F9 M20 0.3 0.2 0.3 Age A Y/A S/Y Rank 1.0 1.1 2.3 Male M86 M87 M89 M86 6.4 M87 4.4 0.0 M89 12.7 M88 3.8 1.0 M4.6 0.4 M90 0.2 M91 0.2 M93 M92 0.2 M94 0.0 Male band Chopasani Recipient Y J2/S 2.6 3.9 M88 M4.6 2.5 5.7 2.6 0.8 7.6 3.9 0.2 J2/S 5.1 M90 1.3 0.7 2.7 0.7 0.5 J1/2 J1/2 5.6 6.6 M91 M93 1.3 1.3 3.5 3.5 1.6 0.8 1.4 1.6 1.4 0.5 1.2 0.0 0.8 0.8 1.4 0.4 0.2 Male band Canal Recipient 0.2 J1/2 J1/2 7.6 9.1 M92 M94 2.0 3.9 1.9 2.5 2.7 1.2 0.6 1.5 1.5 0.5 1.5 0.7 0.5 0.2 Affinity 388 Events Male M86 M87 M89 M88 M4.6 M90 M91 M93 M92 M94 3.8 M86 3.8 2.5 1.3 0.0 M87 20.3 8.3 5.5 1.1 0.2 0.4 19.2 1.0 0.2 0.2 2.3 0.5 M89 10.2 12.3 M88 19.1 8.1 20.2 0.4 0.2 0.2 5.1 M4.6 1.3 2.3 1.2 0.5 1.0 1.2 0.8 2.4 M90 0.2 0.7 0.5 0.2 0.2 1.0 0.2 M91 1.3 0.4 0.6 1.0 0.8 4.9 2.5 4.4 M93 3.8 1.8 3.9 0.6 0.8 1.5 7.4 1.7 31.1 6.4 M92 0.7 0.4 0.8 0.6 1.2 2.3 1.2 2.4 M94 7.6 0.7 1.5 0.2 3.2 1.7 6.6 31.8 2.7 PC affinity 642 Male M86 M87 M89 M88 M4.6 M90 M91 M93 M92 M94 M86 M87 0.2 M89 M88 M4.6 M90 M91 M93 M92 M94 1051 Events 1 Event Age A O O A/O A/O J2/S Rank 1.0 1.3 1.8 3.0 3.1 3.8 Male M76 M49 M10 M125 M204 M205 M76 19.4 1.7 M49 10.7 113.0 1.5 0.5 8.1 M10 2.5 0.2 M125 102.7 41.1 M204 1.0 M205 1.0 2.0 0.5 M208 0.5 M209 1.0 0.8 0.3 0.8 M207 M206 5.6 1.0 Recipient J2/S S J2/S J2 4.4 5.4 6.5 6.8 M208 M209 M207 M206 1.9 5.3 2.1 1.4 5.6 1.2 1.2 1.4 0.5 Age Y/A Y/A S/Y J2 Rank 1.4 1.6 3.0 4.0 Male M240 M120 M121 M250 9.6 5.5 1.3 M240 M120 6.3 1.5 3.8 M121 0.3 3.4 1.3 M250 1.3 125 Events 2.6 0.9 0.6 208 Events Male M76 M49 M10 M125 M204 M205 M208 M209 M207 M206 M76 0.5 16.6 0.5 0.5 0.5 1.0 M49 1.0 1.5 2.5 1.5 1.0 1.4 3.6 6.0 1.7 M10 M125 13.3 10.3 10.3 5.7 M204 5.6 4.6 0.5 0.5 M205 3.9 13.1 1.4 2.0 5.7 4.6 9.7 6.9 M208 2.4 0.2 1.4 0.5 2.9 6.6 2.0 2.6 M209 1.5 0.3 3.7 5.8 2.4 3.2 M207 5.8 2.5 8.6 2.0 3.2 4.6 M206 1.5 2.0 6.0 10.0 3.1 8.6 3.8 5.8 4.6 Male M240 M120 M121 M250 21.4 12.1 1.3 M240 M120 19.9 5.5 M121 29.0 15.9 5.5 3.0 23.0 M250 5.5 502 Events 562 Events Male M76 M49 M10 M125 M204 M205 M208 M209 M207 M206 M76 M49 M10 M125 M204 M205 M208 M209 M207 M206 0.5 1 Event Male M240 M120 M121 M250 M240 M120 M121 M250 0 Events Nonexisting dyad Figure 1. Agonism, affinity and postconflict affinity in (a) a harem troop and (b) three male bands. Female age classes: Y=young, M=middle aged, O=old; male age classes: J1=Juvenile-1, J2=Juvenile-2, S=subadult, Y=young adult, A=prime adult, O=old (see Rajpurohit et al. 1995 for definitions of age classes). Individuals are arranged in descending dominance rank from left to right and from top to bottom (cf. Borries et al. 1991). Cells are standardized as dyadic events/100 h. SOMMER ET AL.: POSTCONFLICT BEHAVIOUR OF LANGURS Table 2. Behavioural categories and contexts of agonistic and affinitive baseline and postconflict interactions PC affinity/100 h 5 4 Harem troop Kailana-I (41 events) 3 Male band Canal (0 events) 2 Male band Machiya (1 event) 1 Male band Chopasani (1 event) 0 2 1 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Minute postconflict (a) SD 0.71 0.4 Agonism Affinity 877 0.3 279 885 Mean dyadic events/h 0.2 2518 0.1 229 63 0 0.5 0.4 0.3 315 353 581 127 AM-AF AF-AF AM-AM AM-IM IM-IM (22 dyads) (55 dyads) (17 dyads) (50 dyads) (26 dyads) (b) 1.31 SD 3.35 Expected PC affinity Observed PC affinity 41 0.2 0.1 0 Conflict context (baseline=561 events)* Natural food Provisioned food Position, shade Allogroom Other/unknown 25.3 24.4 23.3 6.2 20.7 19.0 28.5 16.6 16.6 19.0 Agonism (baseline=1365 events) Displace Hit/kick Pull/push Chase Teeth grind Other 69.5 13.3 5.2 5.6 4.3 2.3 62.8 20.9 11.6 2.3 0.0 2.3 Affinity (baseline=4862 events) Allogroom Embrace Other 94.2 5.6 0.2 48.8 51.1 0.0 Category Figure 2. Postconflict affinity within a 15-min window in four langur study groups. 0.5 Baseline (%) Postconflict sample (% of 43 cases) 2 AM-AF AF-AF AM-AM AM-IM IM-IM (18 dyads) (55 dyads) (15 dyads) (38 dyads) (19 dyads) Figure 3. Dyadic events/h among four langur age–sex class permutations. Means are given +SD. AF=adult female; AM=adult male; IM=immature male. Numbers above bars indicate total events. (a) Agonism and affinity. (b) Expected and observed affinitive events within 3 min postconflict (PC). We used the frequency of dyadic conflict to calculate expected and observed rates of PC affinity (Fig. 3b). There was no PC affinity in AM–AF, AM–AM and IM–IM dyads despite some of the highest expected rates. Observed rates for AM–IM dyads were 6.0 times higher than expected and those for AF–AF dyads 39.2 times. We compared the pattern of PC interactions with baseline expectations (Table 2). (1) The contexts of baseline conflicts and conflicts followed by PC affinity were similar and included disputes over food, position and grooming partners. (2) PC affinity was more easily triggered by conflicts that involved physical contact (hit/kick; push/pull) than by nonphysical displacements. (3) Opponents expressed PC affinity more often than expected via embracing, whereas grooming was underrepresented. PC affinity was more often initiated by the loser of a conflict (58.5%) than by the winner (41.5%). All categories were observed in all study groups. *Harem troop Kailana-I. Subordinates who directed PC affinity to the winner used embracing more often than allogrooming (58.3 versus 42.7%; N=24). Dominants who directed PC affinity to the loser used embracing less often than grooming (33.3 versus 66.6%; N=18). Conflicts related to sexual harassment of copulations and infanticidal attacks were excluded from the quantitative sample. In any case, PC affinity was not observed between opponents in such instances. We tested the possibility that specific agonistic interactions were not followed by PC affinity but that dyads maintained some general reconciliatory balance over a longer period. Therefore, we compared dyadic agonistic and affinitive frequencies to see if pairs with more conflict also had more affinity (Fig. 4). However, no such correlation was found in any study group (harem troop Kailana-I, AF–AF dyads only: rS = 0.30, N=55, P=0.025; male band Machiya: rS = +0.08, N=45, P=0.587; male band Canal: rS = +0.43, N=6, P=0.399; male band Chopasani: rS = 0.03, N=42, P=0.861). Finally, we tried to identify predictors for dyads that showed PC affinity. Dominance rank could be relevant because animals that are closer in rank may have stronger relationships than those further apart. The two incidences of PC affinity amongst males do not lend themselves to such analysis but for females, no correlation existed between PC affinity and dominance rank distance (rS = 0.011, N=55, NS; data on rank from Borries et al. 1991). However, female relationships could overall be characterized as strong, as indicated by the positive ratio of baseline affinity to agonism (cf. Fig. 3a). On average, seven affinitive dyadic interactions occurred before one 643 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 63, 4 10 0.30 (a) 0.20 0.10 0.00 0.40 0.30 0.02 0.04 0.06 0.08 0.63 0.20 0.19 0.10 0.02 0.04 0.06 0.16 (c) 0.20 0.10 0.00 0.02 0.04 0.06 0.08 0.40 0.30 (d) 0.20 2.10 0.10 0.40 0.20 0.00 6 4 2 0.02 0.04 0.06 Dyadic agonism/h 5 10 15 20 25 Ratio affinity/agonism per dyad 30 Figure 5. Postconflict affinity among adult langur females as a function of the baseline ratio of affinity and agonism. x/C: Dyad with/without postconflict affinity. 0.08 0.40 0.30 8 0 (b) 0.00 Observed – expected PC affinity/h 0.40 Dyadic affinity/h 644 0.08 Figure 4. Affinity as a function of agonistic interactions in four langur study groups. (a) Harem troop Kailana-I; (b) male band Machiya; (c) male band Canal; (d) male band Chopasani. ◆, x/ , e, C: Dyad with/without postconflict affinity; : female–male dyad; ◆, e: female–female dyad; x, C: male–male dyad. agonistic act was recorded and not a single AF–AF dyad had more agonistic than affinitive events. The ratio was higher for dyads without PC affinity (XSD=8.68.0, N=32) than for dyads with PC affinity (4.23.5, N=23; Mann–Whitney U test: Z=1.80, P<0.05, one-tailed; Fig. 5). This indicates that females with relatively weaker sociopositive relationships were more likely to initiate PC affinity. DISCUSSION In this first analysis of postconflict (PC) interactions in wild colobines, we found rates of PC affinity well below baseline in all four study groups of Hanuman langurs. Interindividual rates were also low since 82.8% of all dyads had no PC affinity at all. What might be responsible for the relative lack of friendly reunions of opponents in wild langurs? Hypotheses About Reconciliation The minimum cognitive capacity hypothesis (Gallup 1982) would assume that colobines lack individual recog- nition and memory to reconcile. This is unlikely given that (1) langur social life is complex (e.g. Hrdy 1977; Sommer 1996), (2) captive colobines show some degree of friendly PC reunion (McKenna 1977; Ren et al. 1991; Björnsdotter et al. 2000) and (3) the study langurs were able to avoid affinity with each other. Lack of PC affinity and a considerable dyadic variation in overall affinity also does not support the social evolution hypothesis which views reconciliation as a prerequisite of group living (de Waal 1986b). PC affinity in Hanuman langurs did not correlate with rank. Therefore, support is also lacking for the reconciled hierarchy hypothesis which suggests that dominants grant friendly PC contacts to those who acknowledge their superior rank (de Waal 1986b). The dominance style hypothesis predicts that a more relaxed style in which dominant individuals are relatively tolerant of low-ranking conspecifics correlates with high levels of reconciliation ( de Waal 1989; de Waal & Luttrell 1989). All langur females were affinitive with all other adult troop females and the same is true for 95.5% of female–male and 92.5% of male–male dyads (cf. Fig. 3). In this sense, the langurs were very tolerant of other group members. Hanuman langurs often live sympatrically with rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta (Hrdy 1977; Sommer 1996), who are certainly more despotic in their dominance style; yet (captive) rhesus cannot be said to show lower levels of PC affinity (de Waal & Luttrell 1989). Low rates of friendly reunions should also be associated with low levels of victim initiation (de Waal 1989) but more than half of all langur PC affinity was initiated by the victims. In this respect it should be kept in mind that categories of ‘dominance style’, which were developed largely for cercopithecine monkeys and chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes (de Waal 1989), may have to be redefined to accommodate colobines. The good relationship hypothesis maintains that animals reconcile particularly valuable dyads (de Waal 1986a). A ‘good relationship’ could be defined as having high affinity and low agonism compared to other dyads (Watts 1995). However, the langur data do not support this hypothesis. It is true that AM–AM dyads could be characterized as ‘bad relationships’ since they showed less baseline affinity than agonism (mean ratio 0.9) and no PC affinity. On the other hand, AF–AM dyads (ratio SOMMER ET AL.: POSTCONFLICT BEHAVIOUR OF LANGURS affinity/agonism=3.5) and IM–IM (ratio 9.0) should count as ‘good relationships’ but neither showed PC affinity. Only AM–IM dyads (ratio 1.5) and AF–AF dyads (ratio 4.3) showed PC affinity. Nevertheless, those AF–AF dyads with PC affinity did not have higher ratios of baseline affinity/agonism (4.2) than those without (8.6; see Fig. 5). Olive baboons, Papio anubis, reconcile more often when they are closer in rank (Castles & Whiten 1998) and thus perhaps have a relatively strong relationship worth ‘repairing’. Again, this finding could not be replicated for langurs. Further general support for the hypothesis would have been generated if dyads with higher agonistic baseline rates also had higher baseline affinity. However, a reverse correlation was found (cf. Fig. 4). The dominance asymmetry hypothesis states that frequent reconciliation corresponds with high levels of bidirectional or undecided conflicts, that is, weak dominance asymmetries (Thierry 1990). A simple measure of dominance asymmetry can be obtained by calculating the proportion of dyads within a given age-sex class permutation whose agonistic interactions did not include reversals or rank changes (cf. Fig. 1). This produces the following results: AM–IM 71.1%, IM–IM 63.2%, AF–AM 55.6%, AM–AM 46.7%, AF–AF 34.5%. Thus, dominance relationships amongst females were the least asymmetrical, and did indeed display the vast majority (95.3%) of observed PC affinity. In addition, more PC affinity (32.5%) than expected (18.5%) occurred after contact aggression. PC affinity after contact aggression was restricted to female langurs, suggesting that females dispute each other’s dominance position more often than males. This should correspond with higher levels of emotional uncertainty (Aureli & van Schaik 1991) and a relatively greater need for PC affinity (cf. Cords & Aureli 1993). Our data thus lend general support to this hypothesis although the lack of a correlation between PC affinity and close dominance rank remains somewhat contradictory. Context Analysis Embracing was greatly overrepresented as a means to initiate PC affinity whereas allogrooming was underrepresented (cf. Table 2). The preference for brief exploratory touches instead of extended body contact probably reflects the tension that exists between opponents shortly after a conflict. Nevertheless, subordinates seemed to be more insecure after a conflict than dominants. This is reflected in the finding that ‘appeasements’ from losers to winners included a greater proportion of embracings than ‘reassurances’ directed from winners to losers. Our study could not replicate the finding for some wild primates that PC affinity was greatly reduced after conflicts over food (Aureli 1992; Matsumara 1996; Castles & Whiten 1998) which is considered to be a heavily contested resource. Competition over food should be less intense in colobines than in cercopithecines or chimpanzees since colobine diets contain more scattered leaves of low calorific value. In the study langurs, displacements over natural food were twice as frequent as expected and displacements over provisioned food four times (Borries et al. 1991). This indicates that langurs, too, compete for food, and even more so if a desired resource is clumped, such as fruits, vegetables or wheat cakes which local people distribute. Nevertheless, PC affinity after conflicts over food was similar to expected levels (cf. Table 2). Some studies restrict PC analysis to aggression that includes physical contact considering that only severe conflicts might trigger the need to reconcile. However, exclusion of PC affinity after less severe aggression would hardly alter the results for wild langurs. The proportion of conflicts after which affinity was observed would merely increase from 43/1365 (3.2%) to 15/385 (3.9%). On the other hand, PC affinity did occur after displacements and was only slightly less common (62.8%) than expected (69.5%). This draws attention to the fact that observers may sometimes be mistaken in their judgement of what constitutes a ‘severe’ conflict. Being displaced may well have greater emotional impact than being hit. We are far from being able to apply such considerations to the study of nonhuman primates. Our study does not address third party involvement (cf. contributions in Aureli & de Waal 2000). Nevertheless, a preliminary analysis reveals the occurrence of PC interactions that could be labelled as ‘consolation’ (affinity towards the loser), ‘congratulation’ (affinity towards the winner) and ‘protectionism’ (affinity of mothers directed towards individuals that were aggressive towards their offspring). Sex Differences In PC Affinity Wild langur females showed PC affinity more often (41.8% of dyads) than dyads involving males (1.9%). Female relationships were also less individualistic and asymmetrical than those of males (compare the cluster of female dyads in Fig. 4a with the dispersed male dyads in Fig. 4b–d). These findings could be due to sex differences (less intense intrasexual competition amongst females) but may also be linked to the higher degree of genetic relatedness in troops than bands. Paternity certainty at Jodhpur is assumed to be very high, so that infants sired during the tenure of given harem holders are at least paternal half-siblings (Sommer & Rajpurohit 1989; for genetic data on one-male troops in a different population, see Borries et al. 1999). Consequently, close kinship networks develop within troops, particularly because female transfer has not been observed. The majority of female dyads should therefore be resilient against upheavals conflicts could cause. Pairs with lower ratios of baseline affinity to agonism compensated for their relatively weaker sociopositive relationships in that they were more likely to initiate PC affinity (see Fig. 5). Females may need to minimize the damage that strained relationships could cause since they depend heavily on their troopmates’ cooperation to meet challenges such as recruitment of babysitters, home range disputes with neighbours, defence against infanticidal males or protection against predators (Hrdy 1977; Sommer 1996). Males, on the other hand, showed hardly any PC affinity. This may be due to different life history 645 646 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 63, 4 trajectories. Immature males leave their natal troops and cohorts of emigrants are often comprised of close relatives. Indeed, agonism was rare amongst immature males while levels of affinity were high (Fig. 3a), as was the case among (related) females. Serious aggression was absent among immature members of male bands and with it the need for PC affinity. However, nontroop males suffer high mortality and will frequently transfer between groups again. As a result, (half-)brothers have little likelihood of growing up together. Therefore, at least the highest ranks in bands are composed of nonrelated ‘individualistic’ males. They compete heavily among each other for access to females while alliance formation and polyadic support are rare (Sommer 1988; Rajpurohit & Sommer 1993). Adult males had the highest average levels of affinity of any age-sex class combination (owing to half a dozen strong grooming relationships; see Figs 1b, 4b–d, 5) but also by far the most frequent conflicts (Fig. 1a). This agonism establishes clear-cut asymmetrical dominance relationships which again carry little insecurity. Relationships are often short lived because males tend to leave bands frequently (Rajpurohit et al. 1995). Thus, there is little need to mend relationships, as in unrelated mountain gorilla, Gorilla gorilla, females (Watts 1995). A comparison with gorillas, where females tend to seek peaceful PC contacts with males (Watts 1995), may also be useful to understand the absence of PC affinity in AF–AM dyads in wild langurs. The relationship of harem holders and females differs greatly between Hanuman langurs and gorillas. Unlike gorilla males, langur males do not intervene in female conflicts, do not herd females or display towards them and rarely participate in hostile encounters with neighbouring troops (Sommer 1996). Thus, langur females have much less of a reason to appease males and to seek reassurance from them. Langur males compete with females for food, and conflicts can also arise over infants if females perceive males as a threat. However, langur male–female relationships are by and large of a secure nature, with consequently little need for PC affinity. Conflict Modulation Through Avoidance The 43 affinitive interactions within 3 min of PC amongst wild langurs represent only 14.5% of the random expectation. It is difficult to decide how this proportion compares with other studies given the tremendous variation in methodology. Nevertheless, these low levels of PC affinity among wild langurs seem peculiar given that the literature stresses its ubiquity. Our unorthodox finding is not likely to be caused by sampling errors, given that four groups were studied in depth for 11–17 months. Rather, the findings draw attention to differences between studies in captivity and the wild. At first glance, the few field studies of primate PC behaviour seem to support the generalized idea that primates reconcile. However, limited evidence for this claim is revealed on closer scrutiny. All studies used variations of the problematic matched-control (MC) sampling (see Methods), the majority were based on single groups and some on relatively few agonistic events. A review reveals that the majority of studies did indeed document few or no friendly PC contacts (commonly called ‘reconciliation’ in these reports). (1) Vervets, Cercopithecus aethiops (Cheney & Seyfarth 1989): N=299 PC observations; 2% of nonkin and 14% of kin dyads were reconciled. (2) Longtailed macaques, Macaca fascicularis (Aureli 1992): 156 agonistic events; 14% showed PC affinity, 1% in the MC. (3) Moor macaques, Macaca maurus (Matsumura 1996): 48 agonistic events; 40% were reported as reconciled. However, no difference existed between PC and MC affinity once corrected for the small distance of individuals after conflicts. (4) Chacma baboons, Papio ursinus (Cheney et al. 1995; Silk et al. 1997): 514 PC observations; 12% of conflicts with nonkin and 31% with kin were reconciled. However, most reconciliation was vocal and thus nontactile. (5) Olive baboons (Castles & Whiten 1998): of 590 opponent pairs, 85% were excluded because they constituted single observations; 16% of the remaining ca. 90 conflicts were reconciled as opposed to 2% in the MC. (6) Mountain gorillas (Watts 1995): after 504 conflicts in two groups, no reconciliation was observed amongst adult females, adult males or immatures. Only adult female–adult male dyads had above-baseline reconciliation. The rarity or lack of friendly PC reunion in wild primates draws attention to the possibility that conflicts are modulated through avoidance. Our data analysis does not reveal if the lack of PC affinity is correlated with or caused by spatial PC avoidance, but it is hard to imagine that opponents will maintain close PC proximity and just forego affinitive interactions. Aureli (1992, page 335) suggested that ‘withdrawal does not restore the disturbed relationship with the former aggressor’. However, to move and temporarily to stay away is probably at least as effective in terminating the stress experienced in conflicts (Aureli et al. 1989) as a fast PC reunion. Avoidance circumvents the emotional insecurity that accompanies tactile contact with a current adversary. This should be especially uncomfortable for the loser of a conflict although future research will perhaps reveal more subtle, nontactile modes of friendly PC contacts such as the vocal exchanges reported for chacma baboons (Silk et al. 1997). The low-cost option of temporarily avoiding contact with opponents is not easily available to captive primates, and certainly not to the extent of field conditions. This is also true for an early captive study of Hanuman langurs reporting that over half of all aggressive encounters were followed by grooming, although these figures were not compared with baseline expectations (McKenna 1977). Similarly, in wild longtailed macaques, the victim tended to avoid the former aggressor when reconciliation did not take place (Aureli 1992) and avoidance was also a common PC strategy in mountain gorillas (Watts 1995). One could argue that wild primates that travel in relatively cohesive groups, such as langurs, will not easily move away from opponents because this entails SOMMER ET AL.: POSTCONFLICT BEHAVIOUR OF LANGURS considerable risks such as encounters with unfamiliar environments, predators or hostile neighbouring groups. However, the crucial difference to a captive setting is that subordinates can flee far and away if they want to. A dominant attacker in pursuit must similarly shoulder the increased risks associated with leaving the core of the group and is likely to learn this trade-off rapidly, consequently refraining from extended pursuit. The space available to captive animals, on the other hand, is finite. Subordinates are likely to learn that they cannot easily escape from a pursuit and may therefore prefer to approach the animal with whom they had a current conflict. The size of the enclosure matters little in this respect which may explain why macaques show more or less the same patterns of peaceful PC contact under varying population densities (Judge & de Waal 1997). Nevertheless, we do not want to imply that captive studies of PC behaviour do not provide valuable insights; above all, they demonstrate the behavioural flexibility of nonhuman primates in varying environments. However, it is likely that the majority of reports of reconciliation amongst primates are artificially inflated by the conditions of captivity. References Altmann, J. 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour, 49, 227–267. Aureli, F. 1992. Post-conflict behaviour among wild longtailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 31, 329–337. Aureli, F. & van Schaik, C. P. 1991. Post-conflict behaviour in long-tailed macaques: II. Coping with the uncertainty. Ethology, 89, 101–114. Aureli, F. & de Waal, F. B. M. (Eds) 2000. Natural Conflict Resolution. San Diego: California University Press. Aureli, F., van Schaik, C. & van Hooff, J. 1989. Functional aspects of reconciliation among captive long tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology, 19, 39–51. Björnsdotter, M., Larsson, L. & Ljungberg, T. 2000. Post-conflict affiliation in two captive groups of black-and-white guereza Colobus guereza. Ethology, 106, 289–300. Borries, C. 1994. Ecology of female social relationships: Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) and the van Schaik model. Folia Primatologica, 61, 21–30. Borries, C., Sommer, V. & Srivastava, A. 1991. Dominance, age, and reproductive success in free-ranging female Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). International Journal of Primatology, 12, 231– 257. Borries, C., Sommer, V. & Srivastava, A. 1994. Weaving a tight social net: allogrooming in free-ranging female langurs (Presbytis entellus). International Journal of Primatology, 15, 421–443. Borries, C., Launhardt, K., Epplen, C., Epplen, J. T. & Winkler, P. 1999. DNA analyses support the hypothesis that infanticide is adaptive in langur monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 266, 901–904. Call, J., Judge, P. G. & de Waal, F. B. M. 1996. Influence of kinship and spatial density on reconciliation and grooming in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 39, 35–45. Call, J., Aureli, F. & de Waal, F. B. M. 1999. Reconciliation patterns among stumptailed macaques: a multivariate approach. Animal Behaviour, 58, 165–172. Castles, D. & Whiten, A. 1998. Post-conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons. I. Reconciliation, redirection and consolation. Ethology, 104, 126–127. Castles, D., Aureli, F. & de Waal, F. B. M. 1996. Variation in conciliatory tendency and relationship quality across groups of pigtail macaques. Animal Behaviour, 52, 389–403. Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. 1989. Redirected aggression and reconciliation among vervet monkeys, Cercopithecus aethiops. Behaviour, 110, 258–275. Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M. & Silk, J. B. 1995. The role of grunts in reconciling opponents and facilitating interactions among adult female baboons. Animal Behaviour, 50, 249–257. Cords, M. 1993. On operationally defining reconciliation. American Journal of Primatology, 29, 255–267. Cords, M. & Aureli, F. 1993. Patterns of reconciliation among juvenile long-tailed macaques. In: Juvenile Primates (Ed. by M. E. Pereira & L. A. Fairbanks), pp. 271–284. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Dolhinow, P. J. 1972. The North Indian langur. In: Primate Patterns (Ed. by P. J. Dolhinow), pp. 181–238. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Dolhinow, P. J. 1978. A behavior repertoire for the Indian langur monkey (Presbytis entellus). Primates, 19, 449–472. Gallup, G. 1982. Self-awareness and the emergence of mind in primates. American Journal of Primatology, 2, 237–248. Hrdy, S. B. 1977. The Langurs of Abu. Female and Male Strategies of Reproduction. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. Judge, P. 1991. Dyadic and triadic reconciliation in pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina). American Journal of Primatology, 23, 225–237. Judge, P. & de Waal, F. B. M. 1997. Rhesus monkey behaviour under diverse population densities: coping with long-term crowding. Animal Behaviour, 54, 643–662. Kappeler, P. & van Schaik, C. P. 1992. Methodological and evolutionary aspects of reconciliation among primates. Ethology, 92, 51–69. McKenna, J. 1977. Patterns and functions of grooming behavior among the common Indian langur monkey. Kroeber Anthropological Society Papers, 50, 3–12. Matsumura, S. 1996. Postconflict affiliative contacts between former opponents among wild moor macaques (Macaca maurus). American Journal of Primatology, 38, 211–219. Mohnot, S. M. 1974. Ecology and behaviour of the common Indian langur, Presbytis entellus. Ph.D. thesis, University of Jodhpur. Podzuweit, D. 1994. Sozio-O } k̈ologie weiblicher HanumanLanguren (Presbytis entellus), in Ramnagar, Südnepal. Ph.D. thesis, Universität Göttingen. Rajpurohit, L. S. & Sommer, V. 1991. Sex differences in mortality among langurs (Presbytis entellus) of Jodhpur, Rajasthan. Folia Primatologica, 56, 17–27. Rajpurohit, L. S. & Sommer, V. 1993. Juvenile male emigration from one-male troops in Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus): a long-term study of intrasexual competition. In: Juvenile Primates (Ed. by M. Pereira & L. Fairbanks), pp. 86–103. New York: Oxford University Press. Rajpurohit, L. S., Sommer, V. & Mohnot, S. M. 1995. Wanderers between harems and bachelor bands: male Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) at Jodhpur in Rajasthan. Behaviour, 132, 255– 299. Ren, R., Yan, K., Su, Y., Qi, H., Liang, B., Bao, W. & de Waal, F. M. B. 1991. The reconciliation behavior of golden monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellanae roxellanae) in small breeding groups. Primates, 32, 321–327. Silk, J. B. 1997. The function of peaceful post-conflict contacts among primates. Primates, 38, 265–279. Silk, J. B. 1998. Making amends. Adaptive perspectives on conflict remediation in monkeys, apes, and humans. Human Nature, 9, 341–368. 647 648 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 63, 4 Silk, J. B., Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. 1997. The form and function of post-conflict interactions between female baboons. Animal Behaviour, 52, 259–268. Srivastava, A., Borries, C. & Sommer, V. 1991. Homosexual mounting in free ranging female Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Archives of Sexual Behaviour, 20, 487–512. Sommer, V. 1987. Infanticide among free-ranging langurs (Presbytis entellus) at Jodhpur (Rajasthan/India). Recent observations with a reconsideration of hypotheses. Primates, 28, 163–197. Sommer, V. 1988. Male competition and coalitions in langurs (Presbytis entellus) at Jodhpur, Rajasthan, India. Human Evolution, 3, 261–278. Sommer, V. 1989. Sexual harassment in langur monkeys (Presbytis entellus). Competition for ova, sperm, and nurture? Ethology, 80, 205–217. Sommer, V. 1996. Heilige Egoisten: Die Soziobiologie indischer Tempelaffen. Munich: C. H. Beck. Sommer, V. & Mendoza-Granados, D. 1994. Play as an indicator of habitat quality: a field study of langur monkeys (Presbytis entellus). Ethology, 98, 1–16. Sommer, V. & Rajpurohit, L. S. 1989. Male reproductive success in harem troops of Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). International Journal of Primatology, 10, 293–317. Sommer, V., Srivastava, A. & Borries, C. 1992. Cycles, sexuality, and conception in free-ranging female langurs (Presbytis entellus). American Journal of Primatology, 28, 1–27. Thierry, B. 1990. Feedback loop between kinship and dominance: the macaque model. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 145, 511–521. Veenema, H. C., Das, M. & Aureli, F. 1994. Methodological improvements for the study of reconciliation. Behavioural Processes, 31, 29–38. de Waal, F. B. M. 1986a. Conflict resolution in monkeys and apes. In: Primates: The Road to Self-sustaining Populations (Ed. by K. Benirschke), pp. 341–350. Berlin: Springer. de Waal, F. B. M. 1986b. The integration of dominance and social bonding in primates. Quarterly Review of Biology, 61, 459–479. de Waal, F. B. M. 1987. Tension regulation and nonreproductive functions of sex in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). National Geographic Research, 3, 318–335. de Waal, F. B. M. 1989. Dominance ‘style’ and primate social organization. In: Comparative Socioecology (Ed. by V. Standen & R. A. Foley), pp. 243–263. Oxford: Blackwell. de Waal, F. B. M. & Luttrell, L. M. 1989. The similarity principle underlying social bonding among female rhesus monkeys. Folia Primatologica, 46, 215–234. de Waal, F. B. M. & van Roosmalen, A. 1979. Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 5, 55–66. Walters, J. R. & Seyfarth, R. M. 1987. Conflict and cooperation. In: Primate Societies (Ed. by B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham & T. T. Struhsaker), pp. 306–317. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Watts, D. P. 1995. Post-conflict social events in wild mountain gorillas (Mammalia, Hominoidea). I. Social interactions between opponents. Ethology, 100, 139–157.