Explicit Instruction and Learning Communities Frances B. Bessellieu New Hanover County Schools Martin A. Kozloff John S. Rice Watson School of Education University of North Carolina at Wilmington Wilmington, NC July, 2000 1 Explicit Instruction and Learning Communities Abstract Introduction The concept of learning community is an important one. For example, in contrast to the concepts "mastery learning" or "effective instruction," it directs the attention of educators to a larger set of variables that include: 1. The social context (e.g., the group and group processes) in which teaching and learning occur. 2. The social positions, roles, and identities of students within the group. 3. The symbols, norms, and moral principles that refer to and guide group activities above and beyond instruction on "content" itself. 4. The nature of students' and teachers' involvement in the group process; e.g., attention, interest, self-control, enthusiasm, risk-taking, effort, persistence, sharing, trust, and protection. Although clearly not constituting, in and of themselves, sufficient conditions for student achievement (certainly, critical, as well), the quality of teacher communication is this article will present evidence which suggests that these more sociological variables that describe the group and its members are necessary conditions for student achievement. Moreover, there is, we will argue, a powerful link between curriculum and instruction (i.e., what is taught, and how it is taught) and the creation and maintenance of a positive learning community. The purpose of this article is twofold. First, drawing on sociological theory, we will more broadly and precisely define the concept of learning community than is customarily the case, so that it may be used in a more 2 formal way to guide research, school reform, classroom instruction and evaluation. Second, drawing upon data gleaned from curricular reform initiatives, first, at a single elementary school; and, second, from a summer school initiative involving 486 students and 59 teachers, working at twenty elementary schools in one school district in southeastern North Carolina, the paper will show how an explicit instruction curriculum, Direct Instruction, fosters learning communities. Implicit and Explicit Instruction and Community Despite its potential general applicability in school reform, in planning and evaluating classrooms and instruction, and in training teachers, the concept of learning community has been used primarily to describe and prescribe classrooms and forms of instruction that are constructivist in nature – i.e., approaches oriented by induction, and by implicit instructional strategies (DeVries & Zan, 1994; Erickson & Brandes, 1998; Evans, 1988; Franquiz & Reyes, 1998; Marshall, 1998; McClurg, 1998; Parsons, 1998; Pouravood, 1997). Much of the implicit instruction discourse on learning communities is characterized by terms such as the child-centered classroom, developmentally appropriate practices, community of inquiry, knowledge construction, collective expertise, progressive discourse, autonomy, ownership, and so on. Tacitly, and – often – overtly, the writing in this vein argues that other approaches (using more explicit forms of instruction) require authoritarian teacher-student relationships (rather than communities) and damage (rather than foster) children's cognitive and moral development. For example, in making a case for the constructivist classroom, Brooks and Brooks assert, A constructivist framework challenges teachers to create environments in which they and their students are encouraged to think and explore. 3 This is a formidable challenge. But to do otherwise is to perpetuate the ever-present behavioral approach to teaching and learning. (Brooks & Brooks, 1993) In a similar vein, Udvari-Solner and Thousand write: Constructivism challenges the assumptions and practices of reductionism that have pervaded our educational practices for generations. In a deficit-driven reductionist framework, effective learning takes place in a rigid, hierarchical progression... Learning, then, is an accumulation of isolated facts. (Udvari-Solner & Thousand, 1995) Our contention is that positions such as these stem from and perpetuate fundamental misperception regarding explicit instruction. This misperception, itself resting upon a false dichotomization of “good” versus “bad” forms of instruction, is of vital importance, both for pre-service teachers and, most critically, for school children. In the latter case, research has shown – as will be discussed in some depth later in this article – (1) that all children can and do benefit from explicit instruction, particularly when learning skills, rules, and concepts upon which much subsequent learning depends; and (2) that implicit instruction, particularly for at-risk children coming to school with serious disadvantages in foundational skills, can be nothing short of disastrous. In regard to pre-service teachers, to the extent that their preparation as future teachers hinges upon this false dichotomy, they are likely to be ill-equipped to serve those children who do poorly under a regime of implicit instruction. In short, no one instructional strategy is appropriate for all children and/or for all content; and no one instructional strategy fosters the creation and maintenance of a learning community. The dichotomization of “good” versus “bad” forms of instruction is interwoven with an implied dichotomization of the notion of community itself. The Brooks and Brooks quote cited earlier, for example, refers to the 4 classroom community, again, as an environment in which all “are encouraged to think and explore.” Examined as a rhetorical device, this passage plainly characterizes the environment to which they refer as a genuine community of learners; the accompanying message is that no community is possible under the aegis of its opposite number, “the ever-present behavioral approach,” which, implicitly, either discourages thinking and exploration or encourages something other than thinking and exploration. Whether issuing from naivete or ideology, or some combination of the two, there are a number of untenable assumptions underlying the discussion and conceptualization of learning communities propounded by Brooks and Brooks, and fellow travelers. Perhaps the most important of these assumptions is that communities – “learning,” or otherwise – not only do not require social control, but that social control must be altogether absent from a social situation for a genuine community to exist. Thus, the problem with, again, “the ever-present behavioral approach” – with its putative emphasis upon teacher direction, explicit presentation of key ideas, student memorization of said ideas, and practice until mastery or fluency is realized – is that the teacher, and the curriculum, exerts control over the individual, rather than allowing her the freedom to explore her own agenda. (Kohn [Dates] is an especially ardent critic of behavioral approaches; it is, however, not clear to what extent teacher education is in fact dominated by behaviorism: see, for example, Kramer 1991; Sykes 1995; Gross 2000). The latter point leads to a second problem with the conceptualization of community informing the implicit instruction advocates’ writings. The problem, here, centers on the assumed nature of the learners comprising the community. The assumption is that the classroom is populated by what we call “good faith learners” and/or “rational actors.” In the former instance, the 5 assumption is that implicit learning communities are inhabited by learners with natural curiosity, enthusiasm for learning, a desire to “explore,” and a naturally cooperative attitude. Plainly, this ignores a substantial minority of students in any classroom, perhaps especially at higher grade levels. Moreover, each of the phrases we have used to characterize the good-faith learner comprises additional assumptions: e.g., the learner certainly does have some degree of natural curiosity, but that neither logically nor necessarily means that she is curious about the subject she is expected to learn. She may well be more interested in “exploring” what is in her class mate’s desk than in, say, why ice melts. Implicit learning communities also assume a classroom population of what is roughly the equivalent of the “rational actor” – an ideal-typical construct from free-market economics theory. This rational actor, it is assumed, will behave reasonably and act predictably because, among other things, she has “perfect” knowledge: that is, she has all the information she needs to make rational choices. This, of course, is a huge assumption, whether in economics or in education. As with the “good faith learner,” a substantial minority of the students in any given classroom will not fit the rational actor prototype. Indeed, some proportion of children, as is well-known, enter school utterly unprepared for what they will be asked to do, and/or unaware of even why they are there. These children experience implicit instruction – which relies heavily upon powers of inference and induction which at-risk children generally do not possess – as a fundamentally anomic experience (Durkheim 1933, 1951; Merton 1936), in which they are unsure what to do with their curiosity, about what they should explore, and so on. Nor will they get much guidance along those lines. These, of course, are students at considerable risk for both educational disadvantage and dramatically reduced life-chances. 6 In light of the inadequate conceptualization of community informing much of the implicit instruction oeuvre, it seems prudent to examine what we mean by community in greater detail. The next section undertakes that examination. The Elements of Community Sociological theory helps to explain the essential components of communities. First, Georg Simmel (1909) described the origins of "society" and the socializing effects of society in an early paper entitled "Social interaction as the definition of the group in time and space." Simmel wrote: Society exists whenever several individuals are in reciprocal relationship. This reciprocity arises always from specific impulses or by virtue of specific purposes...That which constitutes "society" is evidently types of reciprocal influencing. Any collection of human beings whatsoever becomes "society," not by virtue of the fact that in each of the number there is a life-content which actuates the individual as such, but only when the vitality of these contents attains the form of reciprocal influencing. (Simmel, 1909) The implications of Simmel’s points in relation to schooling are significant and quite clear. Children come to school and to classroom activities with different impulses, purposes and skills. As noted, no small number of children come to school with no idea of what school is for or of how they are supposed to behave, and with little skill in school behavior. Lessons and classroom activities, the class day, and school itself, are all, or can all be forms, templates, or scripts. Within these forms, teachers and students exert reciprocal influence on each other. A "society," in effect, comes into 7 existence. With repetition of familiar forms of social interaction, each member learns what to look and listen for, how to understand what he or she sees and hears, when it is his or her turn and what he or she is supposed to do within this society. Gradually, Simmel says, "the isolated side-by-sideness of the individuals (is transformed) into definite forms of with-and-for-one-another" (Simmel, 1909) – i.e., an experience of "we," which is to say, an element of community. Simmel, however, does not explain how repeated performance of teacher-student interactions within lessons fosters common understandings and skills. George Herbert Mead does address this issue. Interactions between teacher and students, or among students, are what Mead (1956) called "social acts." Social acts have three phases: 1) one person initiates a social act with a "gesture" (vocal or non-vocal); 2) the second person interprets the meaning of the gesture; and 3) the second person then makes an "adjustive response" (reaction) based on his or her interpretation of the initiating gesture. The first person interprets the meaning of the second person's adjustive response, and makes his or her own adjustive response, and the sequence continues. For Mead, the meaning of a gesture is pragmatic meaning – its significance; i.e., literally what the gesture signifies about what will happen or what is supposed to happen next. For example, the meaning (significance) of a smile from one person is that he or she is likely to approach the person smiled at and is likely to speak in a pleasant tone (signifying sentiments of liking). When interacting persons come to share the meaning of a gesture (i.e., they respond to it the same way), the gesture is called a "significant symbol." For example, both teachers and children understand (can predict) the meaning of (i.e., what follows from) a teacher saying, "Listen," or "My turn," or "Your turn," or "Say it with me." 8 And teachers and students come to share the meaning of correctly read words versus incorrectly read words – students' namely, the teacher will enact a correction routine and students will then "get it right." These significant symbols have at least three functions: 1. They are what members communicate about. 2. They are how members communicate. 3. They are a large part of the culture of the group or "society." They define "us." As persons (now group members) come to better understand the meaning or significance of gestures, they are better able to "take the attitude" of the person making the gesture; i.e., to see things as he or she does, and therefore to predict what he or she will do next or what he or she expects the receiver of the gesture to do next. Schutz (1970) calls this the assumption of a "reciprocity of perspectives"; i.e., each person assumes that if she trades places with the other person, she will see what the other person sees. This assumption of a reciprocity of perspectives fosters "intersubjectivity," or a "we-feeling," which appears to be much what Simmel meant by "with-and-for-one-another." The common understanding by members that they are no longer individuals alone, but members of a "we," sharing the ability to take the attitude of other members, and sharing a number of significant symbols, gives members an identity (a self) as members, and also an incentive to protect the group by: (1) self-controlling order-disturbing action; (2) attempting to control order-disturbing actions of other members (e.g., reminding other members of the rules, as shown in the earlier examples); and (3) enthusiasm about participation. This, of course, fosters learning the content of lessons. Emile Durkheim offers a similar argument in The Division of Labor in Society (1933) and in Suicide (1951), making still more explicit the role the 9 community always plays in organizing and shaping individual experience and perception. The influence of society is what has aroused in us the sentiments of sympathy and solidarity drawing us toward others; it is society which, fashioning us in its image, fills us with religious, political and moral beliefs that control our actions. To play our social role we have striven to extend our intelligence and it is still society that has supplied us with tools for this development by transmitting to us its trust fund of knowledge. (Durkheim, 1951: pp. 211-212.) If one substitutes the word “community” for “society” in Durkheim’s passage, the salient points are, we think, quite clear. Although Americans, and particularly Americans of baby-boom age or younger, chafe against the notion that society is “fashioning us in its image,” the fact remains that this is a function played by every culture/society in the history of the species. It is this function which is manifested in cultural diversity; i.e., a Japanese person is distinct from an American, an Arapaho, or a Syrian not on the basis of innate personality differences, but on the basis of differences in the ways that their respective cultures organize personality.i Geertz (1973, p. 50) makes the point quite cogently: Our capacity to speak is surely innate; our capacity to speak English is surely cultural. Smiling at pleasing stimuli and frowning at unpleasing ones are surely in some degree genetically determined . . . but sardonic smiling and burlesque frowning are equally surely predominantly cultural . . . . Between the basic ground plans for life that our genes lay down . . . and the precise behavior that we in fact execute . . . lies a complex of significant symbols under whose 10 direction we transform the first into the second, the ground plans into the activity. Durkheim, continuing this same line of thinking, avers that Where collective sentiments are strong, it is because the force with which they affect each individual conscience is echoed in all the others, and reciprocally. The intensity they attain therefore depends on the number of consciences which react to them in common...But for a group to be said to have less common life than another means that it is less powerfully integrated; for the state of integration of a social aggregate can only reflect the intensity of the collective life circulating in it. It is the more unified and powerful the more active and constant is the intercourse among its members. (Durkheim, 1951: pp. 201-202) In brief, Durkheim's argument is that: (1) to the extent members share values and the meaning of symbols, and (2) to the extent that interaction is frequent (so that the experience of sharing meanings is common and, indeed, partly defines the group itself), there will be (3) stronger group cohesion, which (4) increases the value of the group for its members and reduces the likelihood of deviant behavior. This is not merely for fear of aversive consequences for deviant behavior, but because the existence of the group is valuable and the self is defined in part by group membership. Schutz makes still more contributions to an understanding of community, and by extension, of learning communities. For Schutz (1970), members gradually develop a common understanding of their "reality." not merely that they share the meaning of gestures. It is They develop shared meanings ("typifications") of: (1) time (lesson time, quiet time, active time, time when we are real smart); (2) space (lesson space, personal space); (3) objects (our materials, my materials); (4) activities (enjoyable, boring, individual, group); and (5) persons (members, nonmembers). By acting 11 according to these typifications (e.g., settling down when lesson time begins in lesson space), teachers and students: (1) enact these typifications; (2) communicate to each other the vitality and the moral force of these typifications ("Shhh, Jimmy. The teacher is telling us something."); and (3) continue to sustain the cohesion of the group and their identities and skills as members. In addition, learning communities can be seen as "provinces of meaning" for their members. realities – Schutz (1970) argues that we live in multiple the reality of dreams, illness, the blues, religious experience, and the workaday world of everyday life. Each "reality" or province is somewhat separated from the others by time and space, and by the special activities used for entering or enacting the province and for leaving it. For example, members of a congregation engage in certain actions that enable them to exit the world of daily life and enter the reality of religious experience. Similarly, lessons can be seen as a province of meaning separate from the world of the playground, the street, and the home. Each of the theorists discussed in this brief overview of the elements of community emphasize themes of reciprocity, mutuality, collective identification, shared beliefs and sentiments. Each, in other words, tells us that community is more than contiguity or mere aggregation, i.e., more than individuals gathered in physical space; rather, a community comprises collectively-held purpose, a set of dispositions, orientations, and motivations common to a group of people. It is, in fact, these dispositions, orientations, and motivations that define persons as a member of a group, and, by extension, as not a member of some other group. The shared dispositions (etc.) make possible the intersubjectivity, reciprocal influencing, and ability to take the attitude of the other, of which any meaningful use of the term community depends. 12 A final quote, from the eminent social theorist and intellectual historian, Robert Nisbet, provides a fitting summary to this section of the article. Providing an overview of intellectual theory on the theme of community – an overview covering the thought of Rousseau, Lammenais, Coleridge, Hegel, Comte, LePlay, Marx, Tonnies, Weber, Durkheim, and Simmel – Nisbet observes that The word [community] . . . encompasses all forms of relationship which are characterized by a high degree of personal intimacy, emotional depth, moral commitment, social cohesion, and continuity in time. Community is founded on man conceived in his wholeness rather than in one or another of the roles, taken separately, that he may hold in a social order. It draws its psychological strength from levels of motivation deeper than those of mere volition or interest, and it achieves its fulfillment in a submergence of individual will that is not possible in unions of mere convenience or rational assent. Community is a fusion of feeling and thought, of tradition and commitment, of membership and volition. It may be found in, or be given symbolic expression by, locality, religion, nation, race, occupation, or crusade. Its archetype, both historically and symbolically, is the family, and in almost every type of genuine community the nomenclature of family is prominent. (Nisbet 1966, pp. 47-48). Having completed, now, this discussion of the key elements of community, it is possible to turn to one of the core issues addressed in this article: the powerful relationship between curriculum and learning communities. Our contention is that the building – and to a somewhat lesser extent, the maintenance – of a learning community is greatly aided by 13 the use of explicit instruction and, by logical extension, of a curriculum designed to be delivered explicitly. The curriculum to be discussed here, because it was the curriculum selected by the New Hanover County School District in southeastern North Carolina – is Direct Instruction (DI). In what follows, we will provide a relatively brief overview of Description of Direct Instruction Direct Instruction grew out of the work of Siegfried Englemann and Carl Bereiter with disadvantaged children (1966). Over the past 30 years, it has been developed for teaching elementary through secondary language, reading, math, higher-order thinking (reasoning), writing, science, social studies, and legal concepts. Indeed, Direct Instruction provides complete K-6 curricula in reading and math. The teaching methods and materials have been rigorously tested in numerous experiments and field trials. For example, Direct Instruction was compared with eight other models in the largest education evaluation ever conducted, called Follow Through (1967-1995; one billion dollars; 75,000 children per year in 120 communities), sponsored by the then U.S. Office of Education and conducted by the Stanford Research Institute. A major source of data was scores on the Metropolitan Achievement Test, the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory, and the Intellectual Achievement Responsibility Scale. The main results were as follows (Adams & Engelmann, 1996; Becker & Carnine, 1981; Bock, Stebbins, and Proper, 1977; Watkins, 1997). 1. Children who were taught reading, spelling, and math with Direct Instruction were far superior in achievement to children taught with any other method in both basic and higher-order conceptual skills (e.g., problem solving). Most of the other "innovative" models did far worse even than nonDI control schools. 14 2. Disadvantaged children taught with Direct Instruction moved from the 20th percentile on nationally-standardized tests to the 50th percentile. In other words, Direct Instruction made them regular students in achievement. However, the standing of disadvantaged children receiving some of the other (still used) nonDI curricula decreased relative to the rest of the country. 3. Children taught with Direct Instruction developed higher self-esteem and a stronger sense of control of their learning than did children receiving the other forms of instruction. This despite the fact that some of the other curricula focused on self-esteem. 4. Finally, follow-up studies have been conducted with students taught with Direct Instruction. For example, Myer (1984) followed children (predominantly Black or Hispanic) in the Ocean Hill-Brownsville section of Brooklyn who had been taught reading and math using Direct Instruction in elementary school. At the end of the 9th grade, these students were still one year ahead of children who had been in control (nonDirect Instruction) schools in reading, and 7 months ahead of control children in math. (1988). Similar results were found by Gersten, Keating and Becker Former Direct Instruction students continued to out-perform children who had received traditional instruction. In addition, in contrast to comparison groups of children who had not received Direct Instruction in earlier years, former Direct Instruction students had higher rates of graduating high school on time, lower rates of dropping out, and higher rates of applying and being accepted into college (Darch, Gersten, & Taylor, 1987; Meyer, Gersten, & Gutkin, 1983). Following are some of the main features of Direct Instruction. 1. Direct Instruction focuses on cognitive learning--concepts, propositions, 15 strategies, and operations; e.g., solving problems and writing essays. is not rote learning. It This is evident in the earliest Direct Instruction curricula for language, reading, and math. 2. Curriculum development involves three analyses: the analysis of knowledge, the analysis of teacher-student communication, and the analysis of (student) behavior. The curriculum developer first analyzes a knowledge system (e.g., mathematics, literature) into logical classes and relationships. Next, these are transformed into the precise wording of teacher presentations (instructional communications) designed to be faultless; i.e., so logically clear that students will induce (construct) the proper generalizations and discriminations and correctly use the concepts, propositions, and strategies. Finally, the curriculum developer specifies activities of students (e.g., answers to questions, responses to math problems and story texts) that will indicate whether students have made the proper generalizations and discriminations, and correctly used concepts, propositions and strategies. The curriculum consists of teacher-student communications during tasks (e.g., first grade students write words that describe pictures) ordered into lessons arranged into skill tracks (e.g., picture comprehension) within levels (e.g., Reading Mastery II). 3. Instruction teaches concepts, strategies, and operations to greater mastery and generality than typically is the case. organizing instruction around big ideas. This is facilitated by For example, big ideas in a Direct Instruction science curriculum include "the nature of science, energy transformations, forces of nature, flow of matter and energy in ecosystems, and the interdependence of life" (Kameenui & Carnine, 1998, p. 119). 4. Concepts are not taught in isolation from each other. Instead, 16 instruction involves strategic integration within and across subjects. For example, the concepts density, heat, and pressure overlap in a science curriculum. Instruction on each concept is a strand leading to a larger concept (e.g., convection cell) that integrates the strands. As a big idea, convection is illustrated with air circulating in a room, liquid boiling in a pot, and mantle, ocean and ocean-land convection. 5. The analysis of knowledge (numbers 2-4, above) is used to create student-teacher communications that are "faultless" (logically clear) so that all students will: a. Grasp the concepts and their relationships; and b. Engage in activities (e.g., reading, solving math problems) that reveal understanding and provide practice. 6. Lessons (e.g., 10 to 45 minutes) are arranged logically so that students first learn what they need to grasp later concepts. 7. Lessons (typically in small groups) are formatted so teachers know what to say to provide faultless communication, and what to ask that enables students to reveal understanding and/or difficulties. The strategy to help students get concepts (e.g., a balanced equation) is at first explicit, or conspicuous, so students learn to use the strategy themselves. In other words, Direct Instruction teaches students to think skillfully. 8. Lessons (e.g., on reading) are followed by independent and small group activity (e.g., writing stories) to give students practice and generalize skills to new materials. 9. Gradually, instruction moves from a teacher-guided to a more student-guided format. This is called mediated scaffolding. The move to less scaffolding is achieved by teaching students problem-solving strategies, fading assistance, and introducing more complex contexts--to help students distinguish essential and inessential details. In other 17 words, Direct Instruction fosters independence and higher-order thinking. 10. Short proficiency tests are used about every ten lessons to ensure that all students have mastered the material and to determine which concepts need firming. Frequent evaluation sustains the quality of instruction and students' education; it prevents the drift towards mediocrity or failure. 11. Direct Instruction systematically addresses all eight phases of learning. These include acquisition, fluency building, endurance, behavioral momentum, generalization (exemplars are different except for the features that define "same") and discrimination (exemplars are similar except for the features that define "different"); retention and maintenance. 12. A school does not use Direct Instruction all day. Rather, Direct Instruction would most likely be used at the beginning of some class periods, to review previous concepts and give students instruction on new concepts that build on previous learning. The rest of a class period would be individual or small group work on generalizing or adapting what was learned to new material or problems. The essential thing is that students get the main concepts, principles, strategies and operations before they are asked to use them. 13. Math and reading instruction are done in temporary ability groups that may have students from different grade levels coming together for one hour, and then returning to their home rooms. tracks. These groups are not A student might be in a fast achievement group in math and in the average-pace group in reading. However, if a student's progress accelerates, she would be moved to the fast group for reading. 14. The absolute outcome of instruction on any lesson must be mastery. Every student in the group must be able to perform the skill 18 independently and without mistakes--firm and fluent. Much research has shown that mastery occurs when lessons have the following phases. a. Attention and Focus. No program can be successful unless everyone is focused at the start of every lesson. attention and focus directly. If necessary, the teacher teaches Usually, this does not take long. Lessons begin with an attention signal such as, "Okay, everyone, watch this." b. Orientation or Preparation. The teacher orients students to the lesson by pointing out how the lesson builds on prior work. This is written into teacher presentation scripts. c. Model. In the next phase, the teacher demonstrates concepts, propositions, strategies and/or operations addressed in that lesson. The teacher makes the concepts, rules and strategies explicit or conspicuous. The particular ways teachers demonstrate are carefully designed; they are called "formats." For example, in a lesson on multiplying fractions, the teacher says, "You can see the problem 3-fourths times 2-fifths. The multiplication problem for the top is 3 times 2. 6 on top. 20. That's 6. You write The multiplication problem for the bottom is 4 times 5. You write 20 on the bottom. That's I'll read the whole equation: 3-fourths times 2-fifths equals 6-twentieths." In some lessons, the model is repeated several times with different examples so the children do not focus on irrelevant aspects of the demonstration that would cause them to induce or learn the wrong rule. For instance, if all the denominators in the math lesson were "2," the students might conclude that the rule only applies to fractions that are halves. Demonstrations are carefully designed to eliminate such ambiguous communications. Again, a major premiss in Direct Instruction 19 is that students learn what they are taught. If students make errors, they were taught errors. d. Lead. Often, the next step is leading students through the operation just modeled. This step is guided practice; teacher and students work problems, sound out new words, or read passages together. The teacher transitions to having students respond alone when she feels the time is right. This often occurs as a group response, but every individual in the group must respond precisely on signal. However, students are never singled out for corrections--to prevent embarrassment or even the accidental reinforcement of attention-seeking. Instead, if one student hesitates or responds incorrectly, the whole group goes through a brief correction procedure until all children are firm. The point is for students to internalize the concepts, principles and strategies previously modeled by the teacher so they can apply them to more complex/advanced concepts in subsequent lessons. This facilitates generalization, adaptation and maintenance. e. Test. "Tests" occur immediately after the teacher stops demonstrating and leading. She looks for accurate and quick (firm, fluent) actions from students in response to her signals (e.g., questions). When students appear to be firm, the teacher gives opportunities for students to more independently use what they appear to have learned. This "test" does two things: a) It gives students a chance to practice with less scaffolding or assistance (the principle of "mediated scaffolding"); and b) It enables the teacher to identify precisely what each student gets and does not get, so she can prepare error correction procedures. Continuing with multiplying fractions, the teacher now says, "Your turn. Use lined paper. Copy problem A and write the answer. Remember, multiply the top numbers and write the answer on top. Then 20 multiply the bottom numbers and write that answer on the bottom. Raise your hand when you've worked problem A. 2/9 x 4/2 = ___ Check your work. on the top. You multiplied the top numbers, 2 times 4, and got 8 Then you multiplied 9 times 2 and got 18 on the bottom. 2-ninths times 4-halves equals 8-eighteenths. Raise your hand if you got everything right. 2/9 x 4/2 = 8/18 f. Feedback. The teacher provides timely and genuine praise for new learning, reading and solving problems without errors, and persistence. g. Error correction. Teachers are alert to mistakes and teach students to identify and correct them. learned. This is because uncorrected errors will be And if mistakes linger uncorrected, it is harder for students to learn new material. Helping students correct errors does not make students dependent on the teacher. In fact, correction procedures teach students to observe and improve their own behavior. The general error correction procedure is first to identify whether errors are the result of a student's inattention and/or whether errors are errors of facts, component skills or strategies. If a student is paying too little attention, the teacher might increase praise for small increases in the attention and/or would praise other students for paying attention, to provide models for a less attentive student. In either case, the teacher uses the same correction procedure; that is, she models the correct answers or procedures again, leads students through the tasks again, and then "tests" learning by giving new examples. If the teacher decides that a student has not mastered previous skills, she may drop back to a previous lesson and teach the prerequisites. If it is an attention or 21 behavior problem, she repeats the routine and uses praise as described above. h. Additional material. The lesson continues with additional material (e.g., multiplication problems) using the same strategies. generalization. This fosters A major technique to show how things are alike (i.e., the same concepts, propositions, rules, strategies and operations apply) is to compare two things that are very different, except for a shared critical element, and treat them alike; e.g., call them the same thing. However, to show how things are different (i.e., to foster discrimination), the teacher juxtaposes examples (e.g., math problems, passages of poetry, plants) that are very similar, except for an unshared critical element, and treats them differently. i. In future lessons, the teacher introduces, models, leads and tests problems in which different strategies are used. Then the teacher gives students problems in which students alternate between one and another strategy. This teaches students to discriminate types of problems and to select relevant strategies. The Discovery of Direct Instruction Learning Communities This paper grew out of research conducted by the first author on a pilot implementation of Direct Instruction curricula--Reading Mastery and Language for Learning--in three classes (K, 1 and 2) in an elementary school whose population consists almost entirely of disadvantaged children. Direct Instruction is usually understood primarily as a form of communication that helps students to acquire complex knowledge systems. However, one of the first findings of the research was marked change in the social behavior of children. For example, as early as the placement testing phase of the implementation (October, 1998), students were told that the testing would help their teachers select the right reading materials for the children. The 22 students expressed interest in the testing process, making sure that every student had the opportunity to meet with the researcher. When the first author (who conducted most of the testing) periodically visited classrooms, the students asked when they would be starting to "do reading." When the materials arrived and were distributed to classrooms, the students were excited that they could begin "reading" now. Students were then placed in groups according to their skill level, with a teacher who had chosen to teach a particular curriculum (reading or language) based on her interests and expertise. After a three week transition phase, teachers reported several interesting observations. First, the students were quite attentive, and disruptive behavior had decreased significantly. Second, students were making sure that all group members were getting the opportunity to answer questions and respond to the prompts. Third, equity and equal participation became increasingly important to the children. their turn, for example. Students would prompt other students to take Fourth, students also became increasingly concerned that everyone participate appropriately. Fifth, students self-correcting and helping others became routine in the groups. For example, when the teacher said, "We can play 'Say it fast' if everyone says this together.", the children would visible straighten up , turn their eyes toward the teacher and remind those sitting near them to pay attention. This suggests that group contingencies became a powerful reinforcer within the reading group. More specific examples of changes that reflect the development of learning communities include the following. 1. Increased attention overall to lesson activities. 2. Precise attention to signals from the teacher; e.g., "Get ready"; or "Your turn" or "My turn"; and hand gestures. 23 3. Precise attention to the behavior of other students--especially skill at distinguishing between on-task and off-task behavior and between more proficient and less proficient behavior. 4. A decrease in off-task behavior. 5. An increase in generally helpful behavior, such as helping other students to give right answers or changing seating position so that other students can see the teacher or the book she is holding. 6. A great deal of enthusiasm about the lessons. In other words, Direct Instruction lessons seem to foster precisely the skills (perceptual, action), sentiments, and moral principles that enable students and teachers to participate competently in them. The lessons are not adequately understood merely as one-way teacher "transmission" of information, or even as two-way transmission of information. Instead, lessons are better understood as a larger whole--namely a learning community--with shared understanding of aims, norms about appropriate behavior and about the roles of students and teacher, values, and skills. This is exactly what happens in Direct Instruction lessons, which are scripted and which use the same formats (sequences of tasks and wordings) from lesson to lesson. For example, teachers and students are supposed to engage in certain behaviors in response to each other. lesson the teacher says, "What word?" As part of a reading Students say the word, and the teacher acknowledges the correct response. Or students make an incorrect response, and the teacher corrects the error in a typical way (e.g., by giving the correct answer and then re-asking the question). These interaction sequences are highly regular from moment to moment and from lesson to lesson. The result is that students learn not only subject matter (e.g., to read), but also learn the "rules" and skills for social behavior in lessons. fact, when teachers make mistakes, students invariably correct them. In 24 These corrections reveal students' "commonsense" understanding of the social order and moral obligations of lessons and the group. Following are some examples. The first set are examples of students "correcting" of the errors of a substitute teacher. 1. "Sit on the chair." "Miss S sits on the chair." "Not on the floor here?" (Substitute teacher) "No, we gotta all see." 2. "You gotta get us ALL to say it." 3. "We get to read a whole thing (points to a column of words) by ourself." Three other students lean in and touch the columns of words on the presentation book. "Me and then JT and then N and then..." 4. "Hold the book up here." (higher) "Not on your knee." 5. "JT said it wrong. Make him do it again." "He said 'Oh.' It's 'Ah.'" "He's gotta do it again." 6. "You ain't pointin' at the word." 7. "That's not the sentence we're on." (referring to the story book) 8. "We can't do this Take Home!" "Yeah, we didn't finish the story!!" "I can't answer this question. It's later. We didn't finish." The next set of examples reveal students' knowledge of appropriate behavior during lessons, and how they attempt to preserve the social order. 9. "Sit up, N (a student), you're slowin' us down." 10. "It's not your turn, it's the teacher's turn now." 11. " It's not MY turn, it's your turn." 25 12. "Wait, E goes next, then S." 13. "I will help T." 14. "Watch me, T. I can do it first, then you can do it." 15. "Wait for C and S, they didn't say it good enough." Interestingly, the concept of significant symbol is recognized by two of the major curriculum developers and theorists of Direct Instruction. Speaking of teacher communication, Engelmann and Carnine (in Theory of instruction , 1991), write: The communication must also provide a signal that accompanies each example that has the quality to be generalized...(W)e must use some form of signal to tell the learner, in effect, that these examples are the same...The signal, typically a behavior such as saying "red" for all examples that are red, also provides the learner with the basis for communicating with us. (Engelmann & Carnine, 1991: p. 5) Note that the high frequency and regular form of social acts (teacher-student communication) in Direct Instruction lessons, combined with clarity (faultlessness) in these communications, makes it even more likely that events will acquire common meaning--become significant symbols. This is shown below in an excerpt from the curriculum called Corrective Reading. Decoding B1, lesson 1. Teacher: You're going to say the first sounds in hard words. Teacher: Listen: slip. Say it. (Signal) Students: Slip Teacher: Good! Teacher: My turn to say the first sound in (pause) slip. sss. Say the first sound. (Signal) Your turn. 26 Students: sss. Teacher: Great! Teacher: My turn to say the next sound in slip: lll Your turn. Say that sound. (Signal). lll Students: lll In summary, as a form of communication embedded within a larger form of social organization (lessons), Direct Instruction appears to create a true learning community in which members have: 1) high expectations of achievement for themselves and the group; 2) clearly established standards and accountability; 3) shared symbols; and 4) shared conceptions of time, space, action objects and activities. It is not possible at this time to assert which has stronger causal influence--i.e., whether increasing achievement fosters a learning community or whether a developing learning comunity is a necessary condition for achievement. However, this is a topic for future research. References Adams, G.L., & Engelmann, S. (1996). Research on Direct Instruction: 25 years beyond DISTAR. Seattle, WA: Educational Achievement Systems. Becker, W., & Carnine, D.W. (1981). Direct instruction: A behavior theory model for comprehensive educational intervention with the minority. S.W. Bijou & R. Ruiz (Eds.), Behavior modification: In Contributions to education (pp. 145-210). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Bereiter, C., & Engelmann, S. (1966). Teaching disadvantaged children in the preschool. Engelwood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bock, G., Stebbins, L., & Proper, E. (1977). Education as experimentation: A planned variation model (Volume IV-A & B). Effects of follow through 27 models. Washington, D.C.: Abt Associates. Brooks, J.G., & Brooks, M.G. (1993). In search of understanding: The case for constructivist classrooms. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Darch, C., Gersten, R., & Taylor, R. (1987). County Direct Instruction program: elementary programs. Evaluation of Williamsburg Factors leading to success in rural Research in Rural Education, 4, 111-118. DeVries, R., & Zan, B. (1994). Moral classrooms, moral children: creating a constructivist atmosphere in early education. Teachers College Press. Durkheim, E. (1933). Press. The division of labor in society. New York: The Free (Originally published in 1893.) Durkheim, E. (1951). Suicide. New York: The Free Press. (Originally published in 1897.) Engelmann, S., & Carnine, D. (1991). edition). Eugene, OR: Theory of instruction (revised ADI Press. Erickson, G.L. & Brandes, G. (1998). Developing and Sustaining a Community of Inquiry among Teachers and Teacher-Educators. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 44, 1, 38-52 Evans, M. (1988). Transferring the Excitement of What Is Learned in a Community of Teacher Researchers to Classroom Communities of Pupils. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association. San Diego, CA, April 13-17. Finn, C.E., & Ravitch, D. (1996). Educational reform 1995-1996. A report from the Educational Excellence Network. http://www.edexcellence.net/library/epciv.html Franquiz, M.E. & Reyes, Maria de la L. (1998). Creating Inclusive Learning Communities through English Language Arts: From "Chanclas" to "Canicas." Language Arts, 75, 3, 211-20. 28 Gersten, R., Keating, T., & Becker, W.C. (1988). Direct Instruction model: students. Continued impact of the Longitudinal studies of Follow Through Education and Treatment of Children, 11, 318-327. Kameenui, E.J., & Carnine, D.W. (1998). Effective teaching strategies that accomodate diverse learners. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill. Marshall, S.P. (1998). Creating Conditions for Developing and Nurturing School Leaders. NASSP Bulletin, 82, 595, 75-84 Talent: The Work of McClurg, L.G. (1998). Building an Ethical Community in the Classroom: Community Meeting. Young Children, 53, 2, 30-35 The social psychology of George Herbert Mead. Mead, G.H. (1956). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Meyer, L. (1984). Long-term academic effects of the Direct Instruction Project Follow Through. Elementary School Journal, 84, 380-394. Meyer, L., Gersten, R., & Gutkin, J. (1983). Direct instruction: Follow Through success story in an inner-city school. A Project Elementary School Journal, 84, 241-252. Parsons, S. (1998). A Science Education Learning Community Story Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching. San Diego, CA, April 19-22. Pouravood, R.C. (1997). Chaos, Complexity, and Earning Community: What Do They Mean for Education? School Community Journal, 7, 2, 57-64 Schutz, A. (1970). On phenomenology and social relations. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Simmel, G. (1909). Social interaction as the definition of the group in time and space." American Journal of Sociolgy, XV, 296-298. Udvari-Solner, A., & Thousand, J.S. (1995). "Promising practices that foster inclusive education. In R.A. Villa & J.S. Thousand (Eds.), Creating 29 inclusive schools (pp. 87-109). Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Watkins, C. (1997). Project Follow Through: A case study of contingencies influencing instructional practices of the educational establishment. Cambridge, MA: i. Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies. This is a point that tends to get lost on Americans because of our collective, and powerful, emphasis on individualism. Note, however, the paradox implied in the preceding sentence: the great value we place on individual rights and freedoms is a collectively-held value. As such, however ironic it appears at face value, the most fiercely individuated among us are, in actuality, the most thoroughly enculturated. Moreover, our individualistic penchant has, in the past thirty-plus years become even more pronounced, as scores of scholars have repeatedly emphasized (see, for example, Gross 1985; Sennett 1976; Martin 1982; Rieff 1966; Zilbergeld 1982; Boyer 1985; Veroff, Douvan, & Kulka 1981; Russell 1993; Rice 1996; Inglehart 1977, 1991) – a point that also calls into question the putatively “rigid” and controlling essence of American culture.