Personality, Perfectionism, and Attitudes Toward Eating in Parents of Individuals with Eating Disorders D. Blake Woodside,1* Cynthia M. Bulik,2 Katherine A. Halmi,3 Manfred M. Fichter,4 Allan Kaplan,1 Wade H. Berrettini,5 Michael Strober,6 Janet Treasure,7 Lisa Lilenfeld,8 Kelly Klump,9 and Walter H. Kaye9 1 Department of Psychiatry, Toronto General Hospital, Toronto, Canada Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia 3 New York Presbyterian Hospital-Westchester, Weill Medical College of Cornell University, White Plains, New York 4 Klinik Roseneck, Hospital for Behavioral Medicine, Munich, Germany 5 Center for Neurobiology and Behavior, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania Neuropsychiatric Institute and Hospital, School of Medicine, University of California at Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California 7 Institute of Psychiatry, Maudsley and Bethlem Royal Hospital, London, England 8 Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, Georgia 9 Eating Disorders Module, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania 2 6 Accepted 27 November 2001 Abstract: Objective: This study compares personality and eating-related traits in parents of probands with eating disorders, with age-band matched healthy normal controls. Method: Data were abstracted from an international genetic study of anorexia nervosa. Information was available for the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (MPS), the Eating Disorders Inventory (EDI), and the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI). Comparisons were done by multivariate analysis of variance. Results: Mothers of probands showed elevated levels of perfectionism on the MPS and more concerns about weight and shape on the EDI compared with controls. Mothers who had daughters with diagnoses other than the restricting subtype of anorexia nervosa showed elevated levels of perfectionism on the MPS. Conclusion: These data are compatible with the notion that some personality traits, such as perfectionism, and weight and shape concerns may cluster in families of probands with eating disorders. Ó 2002 by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Int J Eat Disord 31: 290 299, 2002; DOI 10.1002/eat.10032 Key words: personality traits; eating-related traits; parents; genetic factors *Correspondence to: D. Blake Woodside, Toronto General Hospital, 200 Elizabeth Street, 8EN-219, Toronto, Ontario Canada M5G 2C4. E-mail: b.woodside@utoronto.ca Ó 2002 by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Traits in parents 291 INTRODUCTION A well-recognized problem in the investigation of genetic factors in complex psychiatric disorders is phenotypic definition. Diagnostic categories in the 4th ed. of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), although clinically reliable, may not map well onto possible genetic determinants of psychiatric illnesses. This issue becomes especially relevant when investigations have reached the point of attempting to identify specific susceptibility genes, where the precise definition of ``affected'' status has critical effects on the outcome of analyses. There may be little empirical basis to guide the fashion in which an expanded definition of phenotype is to be determined. Studies of associated factors may lead to some informed guesses. However, family-genetic studies have rarely, if ever, been performed on anything except phenotypes defined by clinical symptom counts. Studies that examine the parents of affected probands are one way in which to increase the information available to make informed choices as to expanded phenotypic definition. In the eating disorders, some studies have examined Axis I pathology in parents (Lilenfeld et al., 1998; Strober, Lampert, Morrell, Burroughs, & Jacobs, 1990). There is also a rich literature comprising studies that focus on how family environment, family interactions, and parenting skills (Humphrey, 1986; Kog & Vandereycken, 1989; Woodside & Shekter-Wolfson, 1990) might affect the onset of eating disturbance. An extensive clinical literature describes purported personality types in parents with anorexia nervosa 6 (AN; Bruch, 1973; Minuchin, Rosman, & Baker, 1978; Selvini-Palazzoli, 1974). However, studies of the personality of parents of individuals with AN are rare. Lilenfeld et al. 7 (2000) reported elevated rates of perfectionism in the first-degree relatives of bulimic probands. Otherwise, there has been essentially no examination in parents of affected individuals of those dimensions of temperament or cognitive/psychological domains believed to be important in the development of eating disorders. This paper explores dimensions of personality, temperament, and eating attitudes and behaviors in parents of individuals with AN compared with controls. Moreover, we explore whether differences exist between parents of individuals with the restricting subtype of AN versus parents of individuals with AN accompanied by binging and/or purging. These findings may offer insight into temperamental traits that may be trans8 mitted through families that have an increased risk for liability to AN. METHODS Participants The sample reported on here is a subset of those participants from the multisite, international Price Foundation genetic study of AN (Kaye et al., 2000). This genetic study includes 196 relative pairs affected with AN, bulimia nervosa (BN), or eating disorder not otherwise specified (ED-NOS) recruited from seven sites in North America and Europe including Pittsburgh, New York, Los Angeles, Toronto, London, Munich, and Philadelphia. Sample ascertainment and recruitment strategies are discussed in detail elsewhere (Kaye et al., 2000). Parents of probands were also recruited for blood sampling and the completion of a battery of psychometric tests. Probands and sibs were accepted into the study even if parents did not agree to participate or were unavailable for some reason. 292 Woodside et al. Male or female probands meeting modified (i.e., criterion D, amenorrhea, not required) DSM-IV criteria for AN were identified through treatment facilities and advertisements. Upon initial screening, probands were questioned about eating disorders (i.e., AN, BN, or ED-NOS) in their male and female relatives. Permission to contact first, second, and third-degree relatives with suspicion of an eating disorder was then sought and the relative(s) were subsequently contacted. If upon initial screening, the proband met modified DSM-IV criteria for AN and the identified relative(s) met DSM-IV criteria for AN, BN, or ED-NOS, then the proband and relative(s) were included in the Price genetic study and administered an assessment battery including the assessments described below. However, if only the proband or relative appeared to meet criteria, then neither individual was included in the Price study. A total of 185 parents (78 fathers and 107 mothers) were available for the present study. No parents were probands or affected relatives in the study: that is, we did not accept parent/child affected relative pairs. However, because parents were not interviewed directly about their own lifetime history of psychopathology, it is possible that some parents did have an eating disorder. Institutional ethics approval was obtained for the study at each participating site and all subjects and parents gave written consent to participate in the study. Control Data The primary goals of the Price Foundation genetic study of AN were to use familybased association and linkage studies to identify susceptibility genes for the development of AN. For these types of analyses, community control data are not required. As a result of this ascertainment strategy, we did not have access to community control women directly matched to the parents of the probands in the sample. Consequently, for the current study, we obtained control data from a variety of sources. For the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI), data were obtained from the normative TCI database provided by Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic, and Wetzel (1994). Control data on the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale and the Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI) were obtained from a previous family study performed in Pittsburgh (Lilenfeld et al., 1998). Control subjects were matched for gender and age-band matched to the parent groups. Measures TCI Parents completed the 240-item TCI version 9 (Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993). The TCI has been normed in a large U.S. national probability sample (Cloninger et al., 1994) and shows acceptable internal consistency (range = .76 .89; Cloninger et al., 1993). MPS The MPS (Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990) is a 35-item, factor analytically developed self-rating instrument that consists of an overall assessment of perfectionism, as well as six specific dimensions of perfectionism. These dimensions include concern over mistakes, high personal standards, high perceived parental expectations, high perceived parental criticism, doubt about quality of performance, and finally, organization, order, and precision. The coefficients of internal consistency for the factor scales Traits in parents 293 range from .77 to .93 and the reliability of the overall perfectionism scale is .90 (Frost et al., 1990). The MPS has been found to discriminate successfully between subjects with and without eating disorders (Srinivasagam et al., 1995). EDI-2 The EDI-2 (Garner, 1990) is a 91-item, standardized self-report measure consisting of 11 subscales that assess specific cognitive and behavioral dimensions of eating disorders: Drive for Thinness, Bulimia, Body Dissatisfaction, Ineffectiveness, Perfectionism, Interpersonal Distrust, Interoceptive Awareness, Maturity Fears, Asceticism, Impulse Regulation, and Social Insecurity. The last three subscales are new to the revised edition of the EDI. The original EDI showed good internal consistency, as well as good convergent and discriminant validity (Garner, Olmsted, & Polivy, 1983). Alpha coefficients for the eight original subscales range from .82 to .90. Internal consistency for the three new subscales is fair to good, with alpha coefficients between .70 and .80 (Garner, 1990). The EDI has been used in numerous studies and has been found to discriminate successfully between subjects with and without eating disorders (Garner et al., 1983). Data Analytic Approach All analyses were performed separately for mothers and fathers. The first set of analyses compared all parents of restricting anorexia nervosa (RAN) probands to controls. Some authors have suggested that individuals with the binging or purging forms of AN 9 differ from those with the restricting form (Garfinkel et al., 1980; Garner, Garner, & Rosen, 1993). To attempt to achieve greater homogeneity in the parent groups, we conducted a secondary analysis. We divided parents of AN probands into two groups. The first group, called RAN parents, were parents who had children in the study with only a diagnosis of RAN and no history of binging or purging. The second group, labeled mixed parents, included all other possible combinations. For example, these parents could have one child with RAN and another child with BN or ED-NOS. The breakdown of these subgroups is shown in Table 1. All data were tested for normality. Square root transformations were calculated for nonnormally distributed data. A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed separately for the MPS, EDI, and TCI. Univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were then performed for the subscales. Confidence intervals were calculated for the three groups using Tukey's studentized range test. A significance level of p < .05 was set for all comparisons. We chose not to correct for multiple comparisons given that this was an exploratory study on a relatively small but unique sample. Table 1. Mothers Fathers Breakdown of parents into subgroups Total RAN Mixed 107 78 20 12 87 66 Note: RAN parents have only RAN children entered into the study as subjects. Mixed parents have at least one child entered into the study with an eating disorder diagnosis other than RAN. RAN = restricting anorexia nervosa. 294 Woodside et al. RESULTS Means and standard deviations for the MPS, EDI, and TCI are presented in Table 2. Because of the unique nature of this study, and the relatively small sample sizes, univariate test results are shown for information even when the relevant overall multivariate test is not significant. An analysis of results by site (Pittsburgh, New York, Toronto, Los Angeles, Munich, and London) was not performed because of the small number of subjects from several sites. In the first set of analyses comparing all mothers and fathers of anorexic probands with controls (Table 3), mothers of anorexic probands demonstrated elevated scores on the overall MPS and EDI (MANOVA, F = 3.75, 6 df, p = .002 and F = 2.59, 8 df, p = .01, respectively). Fathers scored signi®cantly differently on the TCI (F = 2.10, 7 df, p = .04) than male controls. Univariate comparisons for mothers on the MPS showed elevated scores on the subscales Concern over Mistakes (F = 6.84, 1 df, p = .01) and Parental Criticism (F = 9.03, 1 df, p = .003) compared with controls. For the EDI, mothers had elevated scores on Drive for Thinness (F = 9.38, 1 df, p = .003), Ineffectiveness (F = 4.39, 1 df, p = .04), and Interoceptive Awareness (F = 4.92, 1 df, p = .03) compared with controls. Fathers of the AN probands showed increased scores on the Self-Directedness subscale of the TCI (F = 8.04, 1 df, p = .005) compared with controls. Subgroup Analyses The second set of analyses compared RAN, mixed, and control parents. Again, the overall MANOVA for mothers was significantly different for the MPS (F = 2.43, 12 df, p = .005, with mixed mothers having the highest overall scores. Univariate comparisons across the three groups showed the mixed mother group to have elevated scores compared with controls on Concern over Mistakes (F = 5.60, 2 df, p = .005) and Parental Criticism (F = 6.28, 2 df, p = .003). Although the subscale Personal Standards showed a signi®cant difference across the three groups of mothers (F = 3.56, 2 df, p = .03), none of the groups differed signi®cantly from one another. Fathers did not show any significant differences across the three groups. For fathers, the overall MPS MANOVA showed a trend toward differences (F = 1.762, 12 df, p = .06), with RAN fathers having lowered scores on Parental Expectations (F = 3.53, 2 df, p = .03) relative to controls and lowered scores on Doubts about Actions (F = 3.62, 2 df, p = .03) relative to mixed AN fathers. DISCUSSION We examined dimensions of temperament, personality, psychological functioning, and eating-related pathology in parents of individuals affected with AN. The most salient differences between mothers of individuals with AN and female controls were greater perfectionism and higher levels of some aspects of eating disordered-type attitudes and behaviors. Fathers of individuals with AN differed minimally from males, with the exception of elevated perfectionism in fathers of offspring with RAN. Our limited sample size for fathers may have precluded the detection of other significant differences. Comparisons of personality and eating attitudes between mothers of anorexic probands and controls Controls (n 248) M a All AN Mothers (n 107) SD M SD RAN Type (n 20) M SD Mixed Type (n 87) M SD All AN Vs. Controls F(df) RAN Vs. Mixed Vs. Controls p d F(df) p MPS Overall CM PS PE PC DA O 58.52 14.81 18.19 11.29 6.39 7.84 23.55 15.25 4.94 5.33 4.68 2.68 2.95 4.25 68.95 19.34 20.40 11.48 9.06 8.78 22.79 24.46 9.07 6.49 5.13 4.57 4.06 4.79 59.75 16.17 18.06 9.89 7.53 7.76 21.17 22.63 7.15 7.07 4.28 4.33 3.56 5.94 70.94 20.02 20.91 11.82 9.39 8.99 23.14 24.51 9.33 6.28 5.26 4.58 4.15 4.47 3.75(6) 6.84(1) 3.73(1) 0.06(1) 9.03(1) 1.13(1) 0.28(1) .002 .01 .06 .80 .003 .29 .60 2.43(12) 5.60(2) 3.56(2) 0.84(2) 6.28(2) 1.35(2) 1.48(2) .005 .005e .03 .43 .003e .26 .23 EDIb Overall DT B BD I P IED IEA MF 0.90 0.58 10.61 1.00 3.10 1.65 0.45 1.16 1.27 0.85 7.68 1.71 2.76 1.62 1.18 1.19 3.85 1.15 9.02 2.97 4.06 2.06 2.02 1.78 5.36 2.99 8.22 5.13 3.97 3.22 3.90 3.21 2.21 0.21 7.64 2.00 3.03 1.14 0.86 0.79 3.07 0.58 7.82 4.08 2.65 1.17 1.70 1.05 4.16 1.33 9.28 3.16 4.25 2.23 2.24 1.96 5.65 3.23 8.32 5.32 4.16 3.46 4.16 3.45 2.59(8) 9.38(1) 1.14(1) 0.78(1) 4.39(1) 1.49(1) 0.52(1) 4.92(1) 1.09(1) .01 .003 .29 .38 .04 .22 .47 .03 .30 1.49(16) 5.84(2) 1.69(2) 0.67(2) 2.58(2) 1.37(2) 1.14(2) 3.50(2) 1.59(2) .11 .004e .19 .52 .08 .26 .32 .03e .21 TCIc Overall NS HA RD P SD C ST 18.04 15.12 17.61 5.54 32.68 36.88 16.99 5.64 7.00 3.81 1.90 7.30 4.65 6.13 18.27 15.61 17.90 5.16 34.19 36.51 16.66 5.47 7.43 3.83 2.00 7.92 5.09 6.30 16.09 14.67 17.83 5.39 33.12 36.64 15.37 5.52 7.29 3.51 1.92 6.42 4.03 6.10 18.75 15.82 17.92 5.11 34.42 36.49 16.94 5.37 7.49 3.92 2.02 8.23 5.31 6.34 1.72(7) 0.17(1) 0.27(1) 0.36(1) 2.70(1) 1.93(1) 0.71(1) 0.12(1) .10 .68 .61 .55 .10 .17 .40 .73 1.49(14) 1.86(2) 0.31(2) 0.18(2) 1.51(2) 1.10(2) 0.39(2) 0.59(2) .11 .16 .74 .83 .22 .34 .68 .55 295 Note: RAN = restricting anorexia nervosa; AN = anorexia nervosa. F and p values that are bold are signi®cant at p £ 0.05. a MPS scales: CM = Concern over mistakes; PS = Personal Standards; PE = Parental Expectations; PC = Parental Criticism; DA = Doubts about Actions; O = Organization. b EDI subscales: DT = Drive for Thinness; B = Bulimia; BD = Body Dissatisfaction; I = Ineffectiveness; P = Perfectionism; ID = Interpersonal Distrust; IA = Interoceptive Awareness; MF = Maturity Fears. Overall Fs and ps are for overall multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) signi®cance. c TCI subscales: NS = Novelty Seeking; HA = Harm Avoidance; RD = Reward Dependence; P = Persistence; SD = Self-Directedness; C = Cooperativeness; ST = SelfTranscendence. Overall Fs and ps are for overall MANOVA signi®cance. d Overall Fs and ps refer to overall MANOVA result. Frost MPS means and SDs are presented only for information and were not tested separately. e Mixed AN > controls. Traits in parents 15 Table 2. Comparisons of personality and eating attitudes between fathers of anorexic probands and controls 296 15 Table 3. Fathers All AN Fathers (n = 78) Controls (n = 24) M a SD M SD RAN Type (n = 12) M SD Mixed Type (n = 66) All AN Vs. Controls M F(df) SD f RAN Vs. Mixed Vs. Controls p F(df) p .07 .42 .58 .03 .24 .61 .06 1.76(12) 0.72(2) 0.21(2) 3.53(2) 1.94(2) 3.62(2) 1.84(2) .06 .49 .81 .03d .15 .03e .16 MPS Overall CM PS PE PC DA O 73.83 19.83 22.96 13.58 8.42 9.04 23.67 16.11 6.23 5.12 3.57 2.76 2.56 4.99 68.43 18.50 22.16 11.46 7.58 8.78 21.57 18.72 7.18 5.47 4.61 3.62 3.65 4.61 61.67 17.25 21.75 9.92 6.17 6.58 21.25 17.43 7.21 6.28 4.32 1.80 2.43 4.39 69.78 18.85 22.24 11.78 7.86 9.22 21.63 18.81 7.20 5.34 4.64 3.83 3.71 4.69 2.02(6) 0.65(1) 0.31(1) 4.78(1) 1.40(1) 0.27(1) 3.69(1) EDIb Overall DT B BD I P ID IA MF 1.38 0.63 4.08 0.54 3.63 2.75 0.29 2.08 1.95 1.28 3.34 0.93 2.55 1.54 0.62 2.06 1.33 0.53 4.01 1.14 4.16 2.79 0.71 1.73 1.95 1.48 4.75 2.40 3.69 3.13 1.37 2.76 0.80 0.00 3.80 0.10 3.80 3.10 0.32 1.40 1.93 0.00 6.09 0.32 2.97 2.28 0.74 2.07 1.43 0.63 4.04 1.33 4.22 2.73 0.78 1.79 2.74 1.59 4.52 2.56 3.83 3.28 1.45 2.88 0.74(8) 0.01(1) 0.08(1) 0.01(1) 1.40(1) 0.42(1) 0.00(1) 2.04(1) 0.33(1) .66 .94 .78 .94 .24 .52 .95 .16 .57 0.76(16) 0.27(2) 0.86(2) 0.02(2) 2.19(2) 0.27(2) 0.07(2) 1.61(2) 0.26(1) .73 .76 .43 .98 .12 .76 .93 .21 .77 TCIc Overall NS HA RD P SD C ST 17.89 13.20 14.71 5.39 31.86 34.28 15.53 6.37 7.30 4.43 2.12 8.22 6.83 6.53 16.78 12.99 15.11 5.09 34.97 35.22 14.03 6.02 7.36 3.74 2.08 7.19 5.85 5.79 15.33 11.92 13.80 5.50 34.69 33.73 13.00 6.26 5.88 3.93 1.88 7.14 6.85 4.95 17.07 13.21 15.38 5.00 35.03 35.52 14.24 5.98 7.65 3.67 2.12 7.26 5.65 5.96 2.10(7) 1.32(1) 0.11(1) 0.51(1) 0.78(1) 8.04(1) 1.00(1) 2.67(1) .04 .25 .74 .48 .38 .005 .32 .10 1.31(14) 1.08(2) 0.17(2) 1.00(2) 0.63(2) 4.02(2) 0.88(2) 1.52(2) .20 .34 .84 .38 .54 .02 .42 .22 Woodside et al. Note: RAN = restricting anorexia nervosa; AN = anorexia nervosa. F and p values that are bold are signi®cant at p £ 0.05. a MPS scales: CM = Concern over mistakes; PS = Personal Standards; PE = Parental Expectations; PC = Parental Criticism; DA = Doubts about Actions; O = Organization. b EDI subscales: DT = Drive for Thinness; B = Bulimia; BD = Body Dissatisfaction; I = Ineffectiveness; P = Perfectionism; ID = Interpersonal Distrust; IA = Interoceptive Awareness; MF = Maturity Fears. Overall Fs and ps are for overall multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) signi®cance. c TCI subscales: NS = Novelty Seeking; HA = Harm Avoidance; RD = Reward Dependence; P = Persistence; SD = Self-Directedness; C = Cooperativeness; ST = SelfTranscendence. Overall Fs and ps are for overall MANOVA signi®cance. d RAN < Controls. e Mixed AN > RAN. f Overall Fs and ps refer to overall MANOVA result. Frost MPS means and SDs are presented only for information and were not tested separately. Traits in parents 297 Perfectionism has been noted frequently as a key clinical feature of individuals with AN 10 (Janet, 1903; King, 1963; Palmer & Jones, 1939) and the trait appears to persist even after weight restoration and recovery (Bastiani, Rao, Weltzin, & Kaye, 1995; Srinivasagam et al., 1995). Although true prospective studies have not been conducted, retrospective clinical reports of premorbid personality in individuals with AN have noted perfectionistic tendencies (Bruch, 1978). Our observation of elevated perfectionism in mothers (and to a lesser extent fathers) of individuals with AN has several possible interpretations. First, it is conceivable that perfectionism is an environmentally transmitted trait and that parental perfectionism ``flows down'' to the offspring generation via an environmental pathway such as modeling. Second, as we have no data on perfectionism in the parents prior to the development of AN in their offspring, it is also possible that pervasive perfectionism in the offspring could increase perfectionistic tendencies in parents, given that environmental transmission can be bidirectional (Kendler, 1998). Third, and perhaps most likely, perfectionism could be a genetically mediated personality trait that is transmitted through families and increases liability to the development of AN. Expanding this line of thought, it has been demonstrated that AN is familial (Lilenfeld et al., 1998; Strober, Freeman, Lampert, & Diamond, 2000). Moreover, twin studies have provided preliminary evidence for the heritability of AN (Holland, Hall, Murray, Russell, & Crisp, 1984; Holland, Sicotte, & Treasure, 1988; Klump, Miller, Keel, McGue, & Iacono, 2000). What we do not yet know is precisely what is inherited. Is AN itself the ``unit'' that is transmitted? Conversely, might a personality trait, such as perfectionism, as suggested by this study, be transmitted through families and represent a vulnerability factor for the development of AN? One family study has shown an association between a personality construct, obsessive-compulsive personality disorder and AN, pointing to a possible shared etiology (Lilenfeld et al., 1998). Our examination of differences between mothers of restrictor anorexic offspring only and mothers of mixed anorexic offspring suggested that mothers of mixed offspring have higher levels of perfectionism as reflected in the total MPS score as well as across a variety of subscales. Although the stereotype of the individual with RAN is of extreme perfectionism, at least one other study that compared subtypes of individuals with AN has found that perfectionism (as well as other indicators of severity) is often higher in individuals with nonrestricting AN (Garner et al., 1993). This study suggests that nonrestricting AN may actually represent a more severe form of the illness. If this is the case, then one might expect the level of pathology observed in parentsÐespecially traits that may be of etiological significance to ANÐto be greater in the parents of the more severely ill group. The fact that mothers of individuals with AN had elevated scores on the EDI Drive for Thinness, Ineffectiveness, and Interoceptive Awareness subscales could index either their own eating-related pathology or could reflect the heightened awareness of weight and shape issues as well as the emergence of a sense of ineffectiveness that is a not infrequent outcome of having more than one child with AN. Another possibility is that the results could be the consequence of the aggregation of subthreshold pathology in parents, as has been suggested by Strober et al. (2000). Finally, Fairburn, Cooper, Doll, and Welch (1999) noted an excess of childhood perfectionism and negative self-evaluation in individuals with AN. These experiences could certainly be affected by the personality of the parents of the child with AN. The current design does not allow for a differentiation between these alternative explanations. 298 Woodside et al. It is of interest that there were few differences detected on the TCI on any of the analyses performed. Fathers showed a trend toward a difference on the comparison to controls, but only one difference was detected on univariate tests of specific scales, that is, higher levels of self-directedness, a construct measuring the extent to which an individual 12 is autonomous and integrated. Another report by our group (Klump et al., 2000) noted higher levels of self-directedness in individuals with RAN, compared with subjects with the purging or bulimic form of AN. The current study has several limitations. First, the absence of a control group that was ascertained in a fashion similar to the study group may have introduced some bias into the results. Second, the subgroup analysis suffers from rather small sample sizes that make it difficult to interpret the results. The sample was ascertained using a sib-pair design, which may have produced a group of individuals who are not typical for the population of anorexia individuals as a whole, thus reducing the generalizability of these data. Finally, eating disorders were not ascertained in parents, especially mothers. It may be that the elevated scores demonstrated by mothers on a number of measures might be caused by undiagnosed eating disorders in mothers. The study also has several strengths. First, although still limited in sample size, we were able to collect personality and eatingrelated information on a substantial number of parents of individuals with eating disorders. Second, we used stringent diagnostic criteria for identifying and diagnosing probands and affected relatives. Despite our aggressive recruitment, given the rarity of AN, our sample size for parents (especially for the subgroup of parents with restrictor offspring only) remains quite small. Nonetheless, this study suggests that perfectionism may be a worthwhile candidate for a transmissible trait that may be of etiological relevance to AN and one that may assist in refining our phenotypic definition of AN for future genetic studies. REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Bastiani, A.M., Rao, R., Weltzin, T., & Kaye, W. (1995). Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 147 152. Bruch, H. (1973). Eating disorders: Anorexia nervosa, obesity, and the person within. New York: Basic Books. Bruch, H. (1978). The golden cage: The enigma of anorexia nervosa. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Cloninger, C.R., Przybeck, T.R., & Svrakic, D.M. (1991). The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: U.S. normative data. Psychological Report, 69, 1047 1057. Cloninger, C.R., Przybeck, T.R., Svrakic, D.M., & Wetzel, R.D. (1994). The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University. Cloninger, C.R., Svrakic, D.M., & Przybeck, T.R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975 990. Fairburn, C.G., Cooper, Z., Doll, H.A., & Welch, S.L. (1999). Risk factors for anorexia nervosa. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 468 476. Frost, R.O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy Research, 14, 449 468. Gar®nkel, P.E., Moldofsky, H., & Garner, D. (1980) The heterogeneity of anorexia nervosa: Bulimia as a distinct 13 subgroup. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 1036 1040. 1 Garner, D.M. (1990). Eating Disorder Inventory-2 professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Garner, D.M., Garner, M.V., & Rosen, L.W. (1993). Anorexia nervosa ``restrictors'' who purge: Implications for subtyping anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 13, 171 185. Garner, D.M., Olmsted, M.P., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development and validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia and bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2, 15 34. Traits in parents 299 2 Gershon, E.S., Schreiber, J.L., Hamovit, J.R., Dibble, E.D., Kaye, W., Nurnberger, J.I. Jr., Anderson, A.E., & Ebert, M. (1984). Clinical ®ndings in patients with anorexia nervosa and affective illness in their relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 1419 1422. Holland, A.J., Hall, A., Murray, R., Russell, G.F.M., & Crisp, A.H. (1984). Anorexia nervosa: A study of 34 twin pairs. British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 414 419. Holland, A.J., Sicotte, N., & Treasure, J. (1988). Anorexia nervosa: Evidence for a genetic basis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 32, 561 571. Humphrey, L.L. (1986). Family relations in bulimic-anorexic and nondistressed families. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 223 232. 3 Janet, P. (1903). Les obsessions et la psychosthenie. (Obsessions and psychoasthemia). Paris: Felix Alcan. Kaye, W.H., Lilenfeld, L.R., Berrettini, W.H., Strober, M., Devlin, B., Klump, K.L., Goldman, D., Bulik, C.M., Halmi, K.A., Fichter, M.M., Kaplan, A., Woodside, D.B., Treasure, J., Plotnicov, K.H., Pollice, C., Rao, R., & McConaha, C.W. (2000). A genome-wide search for susceptibility loci in anorexia nervosa: Methods and sample description. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 794 803. Kendler, K.S. (1988). Indirect vertical cultural transmission: A model for nongenetic parental in¯uences on the liability to psychiatric illness. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 657 665. King, A. (1963). Primary and secondary anorexia nervosa syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry, 109, 470 475. Klump, K.L., Bulik, C.M., Pollice, C., Halmi, K., Fichter, M.M., Berrettini, W.H., Devlin, B., Goldman, D., Strober, M., Kaplan, A.S., Woodside, D.B., Treasure, J., Shabbout, M., Lilenfeld, L.R.R., Plotnikov, K.H., & Kaye, W.H. 4 (2000). Temperament and character in women with anorexia nervosa. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 188, 559 567. Klump, K.L., Miller, K.B., Keel, P.K., McGue, M., & Iacono, W.G. (in press). Genetic and environmental in¯uences on anorexia nervosa in a population-based twin sample. Psychological Medicine. Kog, E., & Vandereycken, W. (1989). Family interaction in eating disorder patients and normal controls. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8, 11 23. 5 Lilenfeld, L.R., Kaye, W.H., Greeno, C.G., Merikangas, K.R., Plotnicov, K., Pollice, C., & Nagy, L. (1998). A controlled family study of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 603 610. Lilenfeld, L.R.R., Devlin, B., Bulik, C.M., Strober, M., Berrettini, W., Fichter, M., Goldman, D., Halmi, K., Kaplan, A.S., Treasure, J., Woodside, D.B., & Kaye, W.H. (2000). Deriving behavioural phenotypes in an international multicenter genetic study of eating disorders. Psychological Medicine, 30, 1399 1410. Minuchin, S., Rosman, B., & Baker, L. (1978). Psychosomatic families: Anorexia nervosa in context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Palmer, H.D., & Jones, M.S. (1939). Anorexia nervosa as a manifestation of compulsion neurosis. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 41, 856 866. Selvini-Palazzoli, M. (1974). Self starvation. New York: Aronson. Srinivasagam, N., Kaye, W., Plotnicov, K., Greeno, C., Weltzin, T., & Rao, R. (1995). Persistent perfectionism, symmetry, and exactness after long-term recovery from anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1630 1634. Strober, M., Freeman, R., Lampert, C., & Diamond, J. (2000). Controlled family study of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: Evidence of shared liability and transmission of partial syndromes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 393 401. Strober, M., Lampert, C., Morrell, W., Burroughs, J., & Jacobs, C. (1990). A controlled family study of anorexia nervosa: Evidence of familial aggregation and lack of shared transmission with affective disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 239 253. Woodside, D.B., & Shekter-Wolfson, L.F. (1990). Parenting by patients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 303 309.