Letters to the Editor Lamotrigine and Clozapine for Bipolar Disorder TO THE EDITOR: Treatment-resistant bipolar disorder can be one of the most challenging conditions to manage. There is a need to develop more effective treatment strategies, including complex regimens of combination therapy. Clozapine has been used to treat bipolar disorder that has been nonresponsive to lithium and anticonvulsant therapy, but it appears to be less effective in treating the depressed phase of the illness (1). Lamotrigine has recently been shown to possess efficacy in the treatment of bipolar depression without increasing the rate of switching into mania (2, 3). We report here a case of a patient with bipolar I disorder who was partially responsive to lithium and divalproex whose treatment was augmented with lamotrigine and clozapine. Mr. A, a 46-year-old man with bipolar I disorder, was referred for follow-up care after hospitalization for 7 months for treatment of mania accompanied by grandiose delusions. During hospitalization he was treated with 300 mg/day of quetiapine, 1200 mg/day of lithium (serum level=1.1 meq/liter), and 2000 mg/day of divalproex (serum level=65 µg/ml). He had previously been unresponsive to adequate trials of carbamazepine, fluoxetine, nortriptyline, amitriptyline, protriptyline, tranylcypromine, bupropion, methylphenidate, L-thyroxine, risperidone, olanzapine, and cognitive behavior psychodynamic psychotherapy. Since life charting suggested that Mr. A was due to cycle into severe depression, lamotrigine therapy was begun and titrated to 150 mg/day over 8 weeks. His depression disappeared after 21 days of treatment. After a 5-day period of euthymia Mr. A cycled into mania, which led to the initiation of clozapine treatment, starting at 25 mg/day and gradually increasing to 450 mg/ day. After augmentation of divalproex and lamotrigine therapy with clozapine, his mood stabilized. Lithium and quetiapine treatment were then gradually discontinued. Previous side effects, which included tremors, weight gain, and listlessness, subsided with the discontinuation of lithium. After 5 months of treatment Mr. A elected to decrease his dose of clozapine to 200 mg/day because of excessive daytime fatigue; he subsequently relapsed into a mild 2-month depression that disappeared after clozapine therapy was resumed. Adjunctive methylphenidate, 20 mg b.i.d., was used to manage his persistent fatigue. Despite the side effects of excessive salivation and daytime fatigue, he has tolerated the combination of divalproex, lamotrigine, and clozapine and remained without symptoms of mania for 7 months, after a 34-year history of periodic mania and a 10-year history of continuous circular cycling. This case suggests that complex regimens of combination therapy can be safe and effective in the treatment of patients with complex, lifelong histories of treatment-resistant bipolar I disorder. The use of divalproex, lamotrigine, and clozapine in this patient was reasonably well tolerated. Controlled trials are needed to more definitely explore the safety and efficacy of combination therapy in the treatment of bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 157:9, September 2000 References 1. Calabrese JR, Kimmel SE, Woyshville MJ, Rapport DJ, Faust CJ, Thompson PA, Meltzer HY: Clozapine for treatment-refractory mania. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:759–764 2. Calabrese JR, Bowden CL, Sachs GS, Ascher JA, Monaghan E, Rudd GD: A double-blind placebo-controlled study of lamotrigine monotherapy in outpatients with bipolar I depression. Lamictal 602 Study Group. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60:79–88 3. Calabrese JR, Bowden CL, McElroy SL, Cookson J, Andersen J, Keck PE Jr, Rhodes L, Bolden-Watson C, Zhou J, Ascher JA: Spectrum of activity of lamotrigine in treatment-refractory bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1019–1023 JOSEPH R. CALABRESE, M.D. PRASHANT GAJWANI, M.D. Cleveland, Ohio Paroxetine and Irritable Bowel Syndrome TO THE EDITOR: Treatment with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors has been associated with gastrointestinal side effects, including exacerbation of irritable bowel syndrome (1). In contrast, we describe apparent improvement of irritable bowel syndrome correlating with paroxetine treatment and independent of antidepressant response. Mr. A was 44 years old when he saw his primary care physician for cramping abdominal pain, excessive flatus, frequent diarrhea, and an inability to gain weight (6 feet, 155 lb). These symptoms, present for 30 years, had intensified over 1 year with increased stress. A colonoscopy with a biopsy revealed mild active colitis. Treatment with mesalamine, 800 mg t.i.d., for 9 weeks was not beneficial. Irritable bowel syndrome was diagnosed, and treatment with dicyclomine, 10 mg t.i.d., for 17 weeks was unsuccessful. At a psychiatric evaluation Mr. A complained of gastrointestinal symptoms, low back pain, and recent stress. He met the criteria for major depressive disorder of moderate severity. Psychotherapy was initiated, and paroxetine, 10 mg/day, was given for 2 weeks, increased to 20 mg/day for 3 weeks, then increased to 30 mg/day. After 2 weeks at 20 mg/day the irritable bowel symptoms fully disappeared, and Mr. A’s depression was partially relieved. His depressive symptoms completely disappeared only after 2 weeks at the 30-mg/day dose. After 1 year of treatment with paroxetine, 30 mg/day, and with his irritable bowel syndrome and depressive symptoms in remission, Mr. A’s paroxetine therapy was tapered over 3 months. At 5 mg/day he experienced a relapse of postprandial indigestion and loose stools (two to four per day). One week after he discontinued paroxetine, his typical crampy abdominal pain and diarrhea returned, without a relapse of the depression. These symptoms remitted when Mr. A restarted paroxetine, 30 mg/day. He remained asymptomatic for the next 14 months. Then a second slow taper was attempted. At 10 mg/day the irritable bowel symptoms returned, without depression. Maintenance therapy at 20 mg/day has controlled his irritable bowel syndrome symptoms for the past 6 months, and he has reached his highest weight ever, 167 lb. The patient’s irritable bowel syndrome disappeared with paroxetine treatment and twice returned when the dose was reduced. The slow taper of paroxetine makes it unlikely that 1523 LETTERS TO THE EDITOR the relapses were actually withdrawal symptoms. The mechanism by which paroxetine may improve irritable bowel syndrome is unclear. On the basis of in vitro studies, paroxetine has been found to have anticholinergic effects that might improve bowel symptoms, but clinically, this effect appears minimal (2). For this patient, treatment with an anticholinergic agent, dicyclomine, was ineffective, which raises the possibility that paroxetine may have improved his irritable bowel syndrome by means of a serotonergic mechanism. To our knowledge, there are only two other reports of irritable bowel syndrome improving with serotonergic antidepressant treatment (3, 4). References 1. Efremova I, Asnis G: Antidepressants in depressed patients with irritable bowel syndrome (letter). Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155: 1627–1628 2. Pollock BG, Mulsant BH, Nebes R, Kirschner MA, Begley AE, Mazumdar S, Reynolds CF III: Serum anticholinergicity in elderly depressed patients treated with paroxetine or nortriptyline. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:1110–1112 3. Emmanuel NP, Lydiard RB, Crawford M: Treatment of irritable bowel syndrome with fluvoxamine (letter). Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:711–712 4. Clouse RE, Lustman PJ, Geisman RA, Alpers DH: Antidepressant therapy in 138 patients with irritable bowel syndrome: a fiveyear clinical experience. Aliment Pharmacol Ther 1994; 8:409– 416 MICHAEL A. KIRSCH, M.D. ALAN K. LOUIE, M.D. San Francisco, Calif. Nonconvulsive Status Epilepticus After ECT TO THE EDITOR: Nonconvulsive status epilepticus after ECT is a rare complication that has been reported in the literature (1–6). We report a case that developed after an index ECT treatment. Ms. A, an 87-year-old woman, was admitted to a geriatric psychiatry unit with a diagnosis of delirium due to a urinary tract infection. She had a history of major depression and was maintained with desipramine, 50 mg b.i.d., and sertraline, 100 mg/day. The urinary tract infection was treated with trimethoprim/sulfamethoxazole, 160/ 800 mg b.i.d., for 5 days. The delirium precipitated a major depressive episode, and Ms. A was referred for ECT. A computed tomography scan of her head revealed moderate cerebral atrophy and minor periventricular deep white matter disease but no focal lesions. The first ECT treatment, delivered in the right unilateral configuration, lasted 75 seconds. Fifteen minutes after the procedure ended Ms. A had a generalized tonic-clonic seizure that was terminated with methohexital, 60 mg i.v. Once the seizure ended, she became more alert. On the psychiatry unit Ms. A was obtunded and had slight clonic movements of the left side of her face. Lorazepam, 2 mg i.v., was administered and followed by loading with phenytoin, 1.4 mg i.v. An EEG revealed no normal background activity and almost continuous electrographic seizures over the right hemisphere of the brain. Ms. A was transferred to the intensive care unit, where repeat EEGs showed no signs of ictal activity. The neurology service felt that further ECT was not an option given the focality of her seizure. Ms. A was discharged to a nursing home, where she was eventually lost to follow-up. 1524 A MEDLINE search for nonconvulsive status epilepticus revealed six articles involving five patients (1–6). Including our own patient, three of these were 70 years of age and older (1, 3, 5) and three received ECT in the right unilateral position (1– 3). All cases were identified after changes in mental status after ECT. Two of the patients completed multiple treatments before developing post-ECT nonconvulsive seizures (2–4). Ours was the only patient with localizing findings on EEG. Further treatments have been performed on some of these patients without recurrence of posttreatment seizures (1, 3, 5). A retrial of ECT for our patient was discussed; however, because of the focality of the seizures, this was not pursued. We present this case as an unexpected event developing after a first ECT treatment. Further research into possible predictive factors for unexpected post-ECT seizures is needed. References 1. Crider BA, Hansen-Grant S: Nonconvulsive status epilepticus as a cause for delayed emergence after electroconvulsive therapy. Anesthesiology 1995; 82:591–593 2. Grogan R, Wagner DR, Sullivan T, Labar D: Generalized nonconvulsive status epilepticus after electroconvulsive therapy. Convuls Ther 1995; 11:51–56 3. Hansen-Grant S, Tandon R, Maixner D, DeQuardo JR, Mahapatra S: Subclinical status epilepticus following ECT. Convuls Ther 1995; 11:134–138 4. Rao KMH, Gangadhar BN, Janakiramaiah N: Nonconvulsive status epilepticus after the ninth convulsive therapy. Convuls Ther 1993; 9:128–129 5. Solomons K, Holliday S, Illing M: Non-convulsive status epilepticus complicating electroconvulsive therapy (letter). Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1998; 13:731–734 6. Varma NK, Lee SI: Nonconvulsive status epilepticus following electroconvulsive therapy. Neurology 1992; 42:263–264 KEVIN SMITH, M.D. GEORGE KEEPERS, M.D. Portland, Ore. Cyproheptadine for Posttraumatic Nightmares TO THE EDITOR: Several selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, tricyclic antidepressants, benzodiazepines, and monoamine oxidase inhibitors have been prescribed to treat recurrent posttraumatic nightmares, but no overwhelming results have been reported (1). A few authors have suggested that cyproheptadine, a serotonin 2 (5-HT2) antagonist with antihistaminic properties, might be an interesting alternative treatment (2–4). We describe here the effects of cyproheptadine on EEG sleep measures and its serum level after successful treatment. We have no knowledge of any earlier reports on EEG data or therapeutic serum levels of cyproheptadine. Ms. A was a 29-year-old West African asylum seeker who fled to the Netherlands after having been raped and nearly killed by rebels. She fully met the DSM-IV criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder. She had severe nightmares about the rape up to 5 nights a week. A drug-free standard night polysomnography was performed. Sleep latency was 15 minutes. The first REM period (lasting 9 minutes) occurred 85 minutes after the onset of sleep. After 77 minutes of stage 2 sleep with a cyclic alternating sleep pattern, a second REM period was recorded (58 minutes). A third REM period (24 minutes) ap- Am J Psychiatry 157:9, September 2000 LETTERS TO THE EDITOR peared after 18 minutes of slow-wave sleep. Consecutively, a 56-minute cyclic alternating sleep pattern was recorded, interrupted by many short arousals and followed by a fourth REM period. After 12 minutes Ms. A woke up in panic from her “usual nightmare.” At that time no signs of sleep paralysis or EEG abnormalities were present on polysomnography. Despite considerable improvement after psychotherapy, Ms. A’s nightmares continued. Cyproheptadine therapy was prescribed at up to 12 mg at 10:00 p.m. Her nightmares soon became less frightful, and they decreased to less than one a week. Her serum level of cyproheptadine 12 hours after intake of 12 mg was 6 µg/liter. A second polysomnography was conducted. Sleep latency was 10 minutes. Non-REM stage 2 and slow-wave sleep were interrupted by three arousals. The first REM period (13 minutes) occurred after 144 minutes of sleep, followed by slow-wave sleep with two periods showing a cyclic alternating sleep pattern. After 70 minutes REM sleep reappeared for 6 minutes. A final REM period lasted for 10 minutes after a light 5-minute sleep. No nightmares were reported. The main differences between the two sleep recordings were the paucity of deep sleep during the first night and the percentages of REM sleep—30.4% and 12.6%, respectively (normal values=20%–25%) (5). Moreover, during the first polysomnography, REM sleep appeared earlier and more predominantly in the middle third of the night. The second polysomnography showed a nearly normal sleep architecture. In accordance with earlier reports, it seems that cyproheptadine might be of considerable value in the treatment of posttraumatic nightmares. References 1. Friedman MJ: Drug treatment for PTSD: answers and questions. Ann NY Acad Sci 1997; 821:359–371 2. Harsch HH: Cyproheptadine for recurrent nightmares (letter). Am J Psychiatry 1986; 143:1491–1492 3. Brophy MH: Cyproheptadine for combat nightmares in posttraumatic stress disorder and dream anxiety disorder. Mil Med 1991; 156:100–101 4. Gupta S, Austin R, Cali LA, Bhatara V: Nightmares treated with cyproheptadine (letter). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1998; 37:570–571 5. Chokroverty S: An overview of sleep, in Sleep Disorders Medicine: Basic Science, Technical Considerations, and Clinical Aspects, 2nd ed. Edited by Chokroverty S. Boston, ButterworthHeinemann, 1999, pp 7–20 RONALD J.P. RIJNDERS, M.D. DAVID M. LAMAN, M.D. HANS VAN DIUJN, M.D., PH.D. Noordijkerhout, the Netherlands crease stages of slow-wave sleep without altering total sleep time (3) and improve sleep outcome (4). We conducted a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial of cyproheptadine for treating sleep problems found in PTSD. The participants were male Vietnam veterans who had current combat-related PTSD according to the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (5) and who also reported at least moderately severe nightmares on the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (6). The exclusion criteria included current substance abuse, use of a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, mania or hypomania, and any medical condition that contraindicated the use of cyproheptadine. After complete description of the study to subjects, written informed consent was obtained. Sixty-nine subjects were enrolled in this 2-week trial across two sites. Posttreatment data on the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale, the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, and a nightmare questionnaire were available for 60 subjects. The drug and placebo groups did not differ at pretreatment on severity scores on the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (F=2.06, df=1, 56, p=0.16), total scores on the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (F≈0.00, df=1, 56, p≈1.00), or nightmare severity (F=2.80, df=1, 56, p=0.10). When adjusted for pretreatment scores by analysis of covariance, posttreatment scores on the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (F=0.06, df=1, 55, d=0.14, p=0.81) and scores for nightmare severity (F=1.92, df=1, 55, d= 0.37, p=0.17) were nonsignificantly higher (worse) in the treatment group than in the placebo group, and scores on the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index showed marginally poorer sleep in the treatment group than in the placebo group (F= 3.68, df=1, 55, d=0.58, p=0.06). Cyproheptadine serum levels (determined by gas chromatography/mass spectrometry) were available at one site for 14 of 15 treated subjects. Partial correlation analysis, controlling for pretreatment scores, showed a marginally significant correlation of higher cyproheptadine levels with a worsening of Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index scores (r=0.47, p=0.051) but no significant correlation with scores on the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (r=0.21, p=0.25) or scores for nightmare severity (r=0.24, p=0.22) (in one-tailed tests). Contrary to expectation (1, 2), cyproheptadine does not appear to be an effective treatment for sleep problems or combat-related PTSD and may even exacerbate sleep disturbance. Although the study group was relatively small, low power is an unlikely explanation for our nonsignificant findings because of the trend for poorer sleep in the treatment group and the likely correlation of cyproheptadine levels with worsening of sleep. Our results reinforce the need for skepticism about open-label or anecdotal findings and for careful scientific trials to replicate uncontrolled studies. References Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Sleep Difficulty TO THE EDITOR: Patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) frequently report difficulty falling asleep, decreased sleep duration, and trauma-related nightmares. Effective pharmacotherapeutic treatments for these problems have not been identified. Open-label trials suggest that cyproheptadine may be a promising treatment (1, 2). Cyproheptadine acts as a histamine 1 (H1) and serotonin 2 (5-HT2) receptor antagonist. Evidence indicates that 5-HT2 antagonists inAm J Psychiatry 157:9, September 2000 1. Brophy MH: Cyproheptadine for combat nightmares in posttraumatic stress disorder and dream anxiety disorder. Mil Med 1991; 156:100–101 2. Harsch HH: Cyproheptadine for recurrent nightmares (letter). Am J Psychiatry 1986; 143:1491–1492 3. Idzikowski C, Mills F, Glennard R: 5-Hydroxytryptamine-2 antagonist increases human slow wave sleep. Brain Res 1986; 378:164–168 4. Adam K, Oswald I: Effects of repeated ritanserin on middleaged poor sleepers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1989; 99:219– 221 1525 LETTERS TO THE EDITOR 5. Blake DD, Weathers F, Nagy LM, Kaloupek DG, Klauminzer G, Charney DS, Keane TM: The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale. J Trauma Stress 1995; 8:75–90 6. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF III, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ: The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989; 28:193–213 SCOTT JACOBS-REBHUN, M.D. PAULA P. SCHNURR, PH.D. MATTHEW J. FRIEDMAN, M.D., PH.D. ROBERT PECK, M.D. MICHAEL BROPHY, M.D. DWAIN FULLER, B.S. White River Junction, Vt. Weight Gain With Anorexia Nervosa TO THE EDITOR: Because of limitations in treatment resources, patients with anorexia nervosa are often asked to gain weight in intensive outpatient programs rather than in traditional inpatient treatment settings. Little is known about the efficacy of intensive outpatient treatment for weight gain in patients with anorexia nervosa. Using a retrospective chart review of patients with anorexia nervosa, we matched 23 inpatients and 23 patients in intensive outpatient treatment by diagnosis and age, comparing admission and discharge weights, lengths of stay, and rates of weight gain. The inpatient and outpatient groups did not differ significantly on age, age at onset of eating disorder, or duration of illness. The inpatient group weighed significantly less at admission than the outpatient group (mean percent of ideal body weight=71%, SD=9, versus mean=80%, SD=6) (t=–4.0, df=44, p<0.0001). However, the inpatients gained 15% of their ideal body weight during 46 days (SD=27) of hospitalization, at a rate of 0.3% of ideal body weight per day. By comparison, the patients in intensive outpatient treatment gained only 1.4% of ideal body weight during 69 days (SD=45) of treatment, at a rate of 0.01% of ideal body weight per day (difference in weight gain: t=5.9, df=44, p<0.0001). There was a linear relationship between weight gain and length of stay for the inpatients (r=0.77, p=0.0001) but not for the outpatients (r=–0.07, n.s.). In fact, only seven of the 23 outpatients showed a gain of more than 5% of ideal body weight during treatment. The remaining 16 outpatients showed little weight gain or even lost weight during treatment. Moreover, the outpatients who successfully gained weight all did so at a lower rate than the inpatient group with the lowest percentage weight gain. The subgroups of outpatients and inpatients who gained weight did not differ significantly from the outpatients and inpatients who did not gain weight on any pretreatment variable. In summary, subjects gained significantly more weight at a faster rate during inpatient treatment than during intensive outpatient treatment. Intensive outpatient treatment was less effective in promoting weight gain and thus may be more expensive over time. It is important to note that the inpatients were supervised during 35 meals a week, whereas the outpatients were supervised for three to 13 meals a week. It is well known that people with anorexia nervosa are resistant to eating a normal number of calories, not to mention gaining weight. Underweight patients with eating disorders tend to eat little during unsupervised meals; this is likely to account 1526 for the large differences in weight gain between the outpatient and inpatient groups. The patients receiving intensive outpatient treatment who gained weight did so at a rate similar to that reported by the Toronto Hospital partial-hospitalization eating disorder programs (1, 2). We are not aware of other studies comparing weight gain in inpatients and outpatients with anorexia nervosa in intensive treatment. However, a number of clinical centers have informally reported similar frustration in trying to promote weight gain in intensive outpatient programs, as reported on an e-mail chat line of the Academy for Eating Disorders. Further research on the efficacy of eating disorder treatment programs in restoring weight in patients with anorexia nervosa is greatly needed. On the basis of our findings, such studies are likely to show that, in the long run, inpatient treatment is more cost-effective than intensive outpatient treatment for restoring weight in underweight patients with anorexia nervosa. References 1. Piran N, Kaplan A, Garfinkel PE: Evaluation of a day hospital program for eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 1989; 8:523–532 2. Kaplan AS, Olmsted MP: Partial hospitalization, in Handbook of Treatment for Eating Disorders, 2nd ed. Edited by Garner DM, Garfinkel PE. New York, Guilford Press, 1997, pp 354–360 AMY DEEP-SOBOSLAY, M.ED. LISA M. SEBASTIANI, B.A. WALTER H. KAYE, M.D. Pittsburgh, Pa. Rapid-Cycling Bipolar Disorder in Children TO THE EDITOR: There has recently been an increase in the number of reports focusing on pediatric bipolarity. An issue of current debate is whether there are presentations of this condition that are unique to youths (1). The retrospective life charting method (2) has been used to describe the longitudinal history of bipolar disorder in adults. We are not aware of any published cases that have utilized this technique in children. We gave the retrospective life charts to parents of youngsters seen at our institution who met the DSM-IV criteria for bipolar disorder type I to assess the longitudinal course of pediatric bipolarity. Specifically, we wished to ascertain whether pediatric bipolar disorder is a cyclic condition. These procedures were approved by the institutional review board of the University Hospitals of Cleveland. All guardians provided written informed consent, and all patients provided assent before participation. The parents of 10 outpatients who met the full DSM-IV criteria for a lifetime diagnosis of bipolar disorder type I were instructed on how to complete retrospective life charts, and the parents of each child were given a life chart to complete for their child. These patients’ mean age was 12.1 years (range=8– 17). Six of these youths were boys. Through the retrospective life charts, the parents of each child described a cyclic, biphasic course in which periods of both mania and depression were present. Seven youths experienced rapid cycling. In the year before assessment, eight patients had continuous cycling. During this time, the mood episodes were too numerous to count in three of these children. The seven other Am J Psychiatry 157:9, September 2000 LETTERS TO THE EDITOR patients had an average of 2.1 episodes of depressed mood, which lasted an average of 2.9 months. These seven youths also had an average of 2.7 episodes of elated mood, with a mean length of 3.6 months. When we used the life charting method’s severity scale of 0–4, in which a score of 0 indicates no dysfunction and a score of 4 indicates severe dysfunction, the mean severity of the elevated moods was 1.7, and the mean severity of the depressed moods was 1.4. Both of these scores indicate mild to low-moderate symptom severity. It is likely that these last results reflected the fact that some of the children were receiving treatment before their assessment. Overall, these results suggest that children who meet the DSM-IV criteria for bipolar disorder type I indeed do have a cyclic, biphasic disorder that is associated with high rates of rapid and continuous cycling. These findings also suggest that the life charting method may be a useful tool in studying the longitudinal course of children and adolescents with bipolar disorder. References 1. Biederman J, Klein RG, Pine DS, Klein DF: Resolved: mania is mistaken for ADHD in prepubertal children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1998; 37:1091–1099 2. Roy-Byrne P, Post RM, Uhde TW, Porcu T, Davis D: The longitudinal course of recurrent affective illness: life chart data research from patients at the NIMH. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 1985; 317:5–34 ROBERT L. FINDLING, M.D. JOSEPH R. CALABRESE, M.D. Cleveland, Ohio Markers for Schizophrenia TO THE EDITOR: As Michael Davidson, M.D., and colleagues (1) pointed out, identifying individuals premorbidly who will later develop schizophrenia offers the prospect of early intervention, the hope of a reduced risk of dangerous behaviors and hospitalization, and the possibility of improved prognosis and better treatment response. The authors presented a potentially important set of factors that, when combined, yield high sensitivity, specificity, and positive predictive value for identifying individuals at high risk for schizophrenia from a general population. A serious difficulty in the development of a screening tool for schizophrenia in the general population is posed by false positives. Incorrectly identifying an individual as “at risk” may have adverse consequences, including unwarranted stigmatization, a negative impact on eligibility to obtain health care insurance or to pursue other opportunities, unnecessary anxiety and worry for the individual and his or her family, and the avoidance of age-appropriate challenges (2). Caution must thus be exercised in applying screening methods for a disorder with low incidence, because even with high specificity, more individuals identified by a screening test as “at risk” will be false positive than true positive. This two-by-two table describes a hypothetical general population of 10,000 individuals. Assume that 1% will develop schizophrenia and that the screening test for schizophrenia has a specificity of 99% and a sensitivity of 75%. In this example, the screening test identified 174 individuals as at risk for illness; 75 (43%) are correctly classified, and 99 (57%) are incorrectly classified. Thus, even with high specAm J Psychiatry 157:9, September 2000 Actual Result Test Prediction Will become ill Will not become ill Total Will Become Ill Will Not Become Ill Total 75 25 100 99 9,801 9,900 174 9,826 10,000 ificity, more individuals identified by the screening test are false positive than true positive. Furthermore, as specificity decreases, the proportion that are false positive rapidly increases. For example, if the specificity were 90%, then the screening test would identify 1,065 individuals as at risk, 990 (93%) of whom would be false positive. Dr. Davidson and colleagues reported a “validated specificity” of 99.7% for their screening tool. Sensitivity and specificity are not absolute values but vary with cutoff scores. Measurement error and other factors make it unlikely that the screening tool would have perfect or near-perfect specificity when used to screen for schizophrenia in other populations. Even with near-perfect specificity (99.7%), the test predicted that 103 individuals would become ill, of whom 30 individuals (29%) were false positive. The authors concluded that the screening tool can predict predisposition to schizophrenia and “identifies apparently healthy individuals who will manifest the disease later who are not prodromal to psychosis.” I suggest that care should be exercised in applying any screening strategy for schizophrenia to a general population, given the problem posed by false positives and the potential harm that could result from falsely identifying a healthy individual as at risk for the devastating consequences of schizophrenia. Instead, sequential screening strategies may be valuable, where initial screening identifies high-risk individuals who may then participate in more definitive diagnostic evaluation (3). References 1. Davidson M, Reichenberg A, Rabinowitz J, Weiser M, Kaplan Z, Mark M: Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently health male adolescents. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1328–1335 2. Yung AR, McGorry PD: Is pre-psychotic intervention realistic in schizophrenia and related disorders? Aust NZ J Psychiatry 1997; 31:799–805 3. McGorry PD: “A stitch in time”…: the scope for preventive strategies in early psychosis. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1998; 248:22–31 DIANA O. PERKINS, M.D., M.P.H. Chapel Hill, N.C. TO THE EDITOR: We read with great interest the article by Dr. Davidson et al. The methodological procedures and results of this study are of special interest to our research team as we have extensively studied predictive markers of this disorder. Dr. Davidson and colleagues commented that a strength of the design of their screening tool is the use of both cognitive and behavioral measures to identify vulnerability for schizophrenia. Gal (1) stated that these screening instruments are highly reliable and valid predictors of the constructs that they purport to measure. However, the criterion used in validation is based on the “soldier’s rank upon his discharge from the compulsory service period” (1, p. 80). This leaves our research team concerned about the appropriateness of the use of this 1527 LETTERS TO THE EDITOR measure in the present study. Although this instrument was documented to be a valid predictor of rank, we are uncertain of its utility in predicting IQ. Furthermore, although Dr. Davidson et al. commented on the similarities between the subtests of the WAIS and the subtests of their cognitive battery, no psychometric data were provided by Gal establishing the measures’ convergent validity. Dr. Davidson and colleagues established a cutoff of the lowest two quintiles in the social functioning scale for accurately predicting membership in the patient group. These cutoffs have no apparent statistical or conceptual validity, as the authors failed to indicate whether patients in the second quintile differed statistically from patients falling into the third quintile. This overlap between the second and third quintiles for the patient group reduced the sensitivity in predicting behavioral markers for schizophrenia. Moreover, it is unclear how the authors determined this cutoff and if it is applied to the other measures in this study. Assuming that Dr. Davidson et al. applied a similar method in evaluating the other measures, we believe that this application is misleading in identifying subtle predictors of schizophrenia. For example, with “organizational ability” and “interest in physical activity,” the extreme rating of “1” robustly distinguishes between groups; however, in ratings 2–5, the differences are not consistently evident. It appears that only extremely small differences between these constructs may be useful as markers in predicting vulnerability to developing schizophrenia. In summary, we agree with Dr. Davidson and colleagues’ conclusion that abnormalities in social functioning, organizational ability, interest in physical activity, individual autonomy, and intellectual functioning can identify individuals who will manifest schizophrenia in the future. The degree of overlap between patients and nonpatients and the absence of psychometric support for the study’s measures reduce the sensitivity of this model in predicting predisposition to schizophrenia. Reference 1. Gal R: The Selection, Classification and Placement Process: A Portrait of the Israeli Soldier. Westport, Conn, Greenwood Press, 1986, pp 77–96 ERIC A. STORCH, B.A. KATHLEEN GALEK, M.A. JESSICA GHIGLIONE, B.A. MERAV GUR, B.A. LOURDES FALCO, J.D. NELLY ALIA-KLEIN, B.A. STEPHANIE FAGIN, B.A. New York, N.Y. Dr. Davidson and Colleagues Reply TO THE EDITOR: Dr. Perkins raises an important issue regarding the prediction of low-prevalence events. Since there is no biological marker specific to schizophrenia and the prevalence of the disease is low, any screening tool for schizophrenia must establish extremely high specificity to be applicable to an entire population. Indeed, our proposed screening tool has the appropriate specificity (99.7%), but this value, as Dr. Perkins states, may indeed vary in other populations. We would therefore like to underscore the notion that until this tool is validated in other populations, it should be considered a screening tool rather than a diagnostic marker. Individuals with 1528 scores indicating increased risk for future psychosis should be rescreened periodically, as preliminary data indicate that the predictive value of this tool becomes greater as the time between testing and the first psychotic episode diminishes (1). More important, the values of the screening tool can be confirmed unequivocally only in a true prospective study, which is currently planned. Mr. Storch and colleagues indicate that there is a high degree of overlap between patients and nonpatients that might reduce the sensitivity of the model in predicting schizophrenia. The distribution overlap not only reduces sensitivity but also specificity, which, as already discussed, is a major problem for any screening tool. Matching patients to their nonpatient schoolmates attenuated both the sensitivity and specificity shortcomings. Table 2 in the article presented the overall distribution of the patients and matched nonpatients and therefore did not fully demonstrate the ability of the matching procedure to discern between patients and nonpatients. The power of the matching procedure is better exemplified in Table 1, where, for example, 24% of the patients had scores falling below the lowest range of their matched nonpatients on intellectual functioning. Mr. Storch et al. are also concerned with the validity of the intellectual and behavioral measures. The intellectual measures were all revised Hebrew versions of common measures of verbal and nonverbal intelligence (i.e., shorter versions, as in the case of Raven’s Progressive Matrices—R, the Otis test of mental ability, or similar tests in a penand-paper format [Arithmetic—R and Similarities—R tests]) (2), and scores on these tests have been shown to be equivalent to scores on IQ tests (Gal, 1986). The behavioral measures have been described in more detail in a recent article by our group (1). Regarding the cutoff values, the behavioral measures had a normal distribution in the general population, and the lowest two quintiles therefore represented performance at 1 SD below the mean, a value that seems to be a reasonable cutoff point between normal and subnormal performance (e.g., in intellectual performance) and can be easily applied in clinical settings. References 1. Rabinowitz J, Reichenberg A, Weiser M, Mark M, Kaplan Z, Davidson M: Cognitive and behavioural functioning in men with schizophrenia both before and shortly after first admission to hospital: cross-sectional analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 177:26–32 2. Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd ed. New York, Oxford University Press, 1995 MICHAEL DAVIDSON, M.D. ABRAHAM REICHENBERG, M.A. JONATHAN RABINOWITZ, D.S.W. MARK WEISER, M.D. ZEEV KAPLAN, M.D. MORDEHAI MARK, M.D. Tel Aviv, Israel Medication or Psychotherapy for Severe Depression TO THE EDITOR: Dr. Robert J. DeRubeis and colleagues (1) should be commended for a thoughtful and well-executed study examining the relative efficacy of cognitive behavior therapy and antidepressant medication for treating severely Am J Psychiatry 157:9, September 2000 LETTERS TO THE EDITOR depressed adult outpatients. Although questions may exist regarding the authors’ methodology (i.e., Might random regression models be more appropriate for approaching this data set? Might methodological differences between the studies included in the mega-analysis account, at least in part, for the differences in outcome observed?), their primary finding remains that cognitive behavior therapy appears to fare as well as medication in the acute treatment of severe depression. We agree entirely with their assertion that treatment guidelines should not be based on single studies. A number of important questions, however, remain unanswered. The relative safety of cognitive behavior therapy and antidepressant medications, as well as their effectiveness in preventing relapse and recurrence, for example, was not discussed. As important, it was not clear from the data presented whether the improvements in patient functioning described are clinically significant. As Jacobson and Truax (2) noted, statistical comparisons of group differences in outcome provide little information on the variability of response to treatment within the groups or whether the changes observed are clinically meaningful. It would be interesting to know what percentage of patients within the medication and cognitive behavior therapy groups demonstrated a normative level of functioning at the conclusion of treatment or an elimination of their presenting concerns. This can be defined statistically, and the data appear to be available. An analysis of the clinical significance of the improvements observed would be of interest to practicing clinicians. treatments appearing less effective than standard comparisons of baseline and posttreatment scores suggest, such a finding should be accepted cautiously. A return to normal functioning is a high clinical standard. As Crits-Cristoph (5) observed, “With aspects of patient care that involve death or serious illness, even a small treatment effect might be deemed to be specially important.” This is nowhere more true than in the treatment of severely depressed adults. With this in mind, we suggest that the data analyzed by Dr. DeRubeis et al. (1) may offer new opportunities for empirical inquiry, including examinations of differences in relapse and recurrence and the clinical significance of therapeutic gains. Findings suggest that a large percentage of patients receiving psychotherapy or antidepressant medications make reliable, clinically significant improvements (3, 4). Although examinations of data of clinical significance frequently result in MARK A. REINCEKE, PH.D. CYNTHIA J. EWELL FOSTER, M.A. GREGORY M. ROGERS, PH.D. ROBIN WEILL, PH.D. Chicago, Ill. References 1. DeRubeis RJ, Gelfand LA, Tang TZ, Simons AD: Medications versus cognitive behavior therapy for severely depressed outpatients: mega-analysis of four randomized comparisons. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1007–1013 2. Jacobson N, Truax P: Clinical significance: a statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. J Consult Clin Psychol 1991; 59:12–19 3. Nietzel MT, Russell RL, Hemmings KA, Gretter ML: Clinical significance of psychotherapy for unipolar depression: a meta-analytic approach to social comparison. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987; 55:156–161 4. Ogles B, Lambert M, Sawyer J: Clinical significance of the National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program data. J Consult Clin Psychol 1995; 63:321–326 5. Crits-Christoph P: The efficacy of brief dynamic psychotherapy: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:151–158 Reprints of Letters to the Editor are not available. Am J Psychiatry 157:9, September 2000 1529