Effects of Acute Tryptophan Depletion on Mood in Bulimia Nervosa Walter H. Kaye, Kelly A. Gendall, Madelyn H. Fernstrom, John D. Fernstrom, Claire W. McConaha, and Theodore E. Weltzin Background: The present study investigated the role of serotonin in the pathophysiology of bulimia nervosa (BN) by studying the affective and appetitive responses of women ill with BN to an acute tryptophan depletion (ATD) paradigm. Methods: Twenty-two women with BN and 16 healthy control women (CW) were studied on 2 separate days during the follicular stage of the menstrual cycle. Participants drank a control mix of essential amino acids (100 g ⫹ 4.6 g tryptophan) on one day and a tryptophan deficient (100 g ⫺ 4.6 g tryptophan) mixture (ATD) on the other in a double-blind fashion. Mood/appetite ratings and blood samples were taken at baseline and at intervals up to 420 minutes. Participants were then presented with an array of foods and were allowed to binge and vomit if they desired. Results: CW and BN women had a similar and significant reduction in plasma tryptophan levels and the tryptophan: LNAA ratio after ATD. After ATD, the BN women had a significantly greater increase in peak (minus baseline) depression, mood lability, sadness and desire to binge compared to the CW. BN subjects and CW had similar peak changes in mood after the control amino acid mixture. BN subjects and CW consumed similar amounts of food after the two amino acid treatments. Conclusions: Women with BN seem more vulnerable to the mood lowering effects of ATD, suggesting they have altered modulation of central 5-HT neuronal systems. Biol Psychiatry 2000;47:151–157 © 2000 Society of Biological Psychiatry Key Words: Bulimia nervosa, tryptophan depletion, serotonin, mood From the Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania. Address reprint requests to Walter H. Kaye, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, 3811 O’Hara St, Pittsburgh, PA 15213-2593. Received January 11, 1999; revised April 20, 1999; accepted April 22, 1999. © 2000 Society of Biological Psychiatry Introduction B ulimia nervosa (BN) is a disorder that most commonly occurs in adolescent women who are of normal body weight (American Psychiatric Association 1994). BN is characterized by binge eating, purging, and a distorted body image, and is often associated with neuroendocrine abnormalities (Gwirtsman et al 1983) as well as comorbid depression, anxiety and obsessions (Lilenfeld et al 1998). The pathophysiology of BN is poorly understood. One hypothesis postulated that bingeing behavior is precipitated by a reduction in serotonin (5-HT) activity in the brain (Jimerson et al 1990; Kaye et al 1988). This notion is based on several types of experimental evidence. First, in general, reducing transmission across brain 5-HT synapses by pharmacologic means is known to stimulate food intake (Blundell 1984). Second, several studies support the possibility that ill BN women have reduced 5-HT activity as suggested by blunting of prolactin response after drugs with serotonergic actions (Brewerton et al 1992; Goldbloom et al 1990; Jimerson et al 1997; Levitan et al 1997). Whereas one study found BN subjects had normal levels of the 5-HT metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5HIAA) in cerebrospinal fluid (Kaye et al 1990), another study found this metabolite was low in subjects who binged more than twice daily (Jimerson et al 1992). Third, numerous double-blind, placebo controlled studies have found that a wide range of antidepressant medications are effective in reducing binge and purge frequency (Mitchell et al 1993; Walsh 1991). It is important to note that antidepressants with noradrenergic activity also are efficacious in BN suggesting that other neurotransmitter systems may also be involved. The development of the acute tryptophan depletion (ATD) paradigm is of interest because this method has been reputed to transiently reduce brain 5-HT synthesis and release in humans (Delgado et al 1990; Young et al 1985). The synthesis of 5-HT depends on the availability of its amino acid precursor, tryptophan. Tryptophan is transported across the blood brain barrier by a competitive transport mechanism and is converted to 5-HT by trypto0006-3223/00/$20.00 PII S0006-3223(99)00108-0 152 W.H. Kaye et al BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2000;47:151–157 phan hydroxylase. Because tryptophan hydroxylase is normally only half saturated with its substrate, manipulations that lower the supply of tryptophan to the brain reliably reduce 5-HT synthesis (Biggio et al 1974; Fernstrom and Wurtman 1971; Moja et al 1989; Young et al 1989). The ATD paradigm has been used to manipulate 5-HT production through the ingestion of a tryptophan deficient amino acid mixture or diet (Young 1991). This technique relies on the occurrence of protein synthesis; normally when a protein meal is ingested, a large proportion of the digested large neutral amino acids (LNAA) are synthesized into labile protein stores in the muscle and liver. When a tryptophan deficient protein mixture is ingested, tryptophan is drawn from the circulation, enabling synthesis of labile protein stores to continue with the net result being a reduced plasma tryptophan:LNAA ratio. The tryptophan depletion technique can lower plasma tryptophan by 70% to 80% after 5 hours (Young et al 1985). It has been estimated that such a reduction in plasma tryptophan can reduce central 5-HT levels by 60% to 70% (Moja et al 1989). The behavioral effects of ATD include the instigation of depressive symptoms in normal (Smith et al 1997; Young et al 1985), remitted depressed (Delgado et al 1990), and obsessive-compulsive (Barr et al 1994) individuals. In addition, recent findings suggest that women may be more susceptible to the mood lowering effects of ATD than men (Smith et al 1997). Fewer studies have measured the effects of ATD on appetite; no changes were found in healthy women in a study by Oldman et al (1994), whereas Young et al (1985) found a decline in protein intake after ATD. We hypothesized that the ATD experimental treatment should precipitate or facilitate bingeing and dysphoric mood in BN women. Previous studies from our group found that ATD increased irritability, labile mood, retarded affect, and caloric intake in BN women (Weltzin et al 1994a, 1995). By contrast, Oldman et al (1995) found no significant effects of tryptophan depletion on mood, appetite, or food intake in women with BN who were abstinent from binge eating and purging. The purpose of this study was to replicate and extend our initial findings in a larger sample of BN and healthy control women. Methods and Materials Participants Twenty-two women who met criteria for DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association 1994) BN were recruited from the eating disorders unit of Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic for this study. For comparison purposes, 16 healthy control women (CW) matched to the BN women for age and weight, were recruited from the staff of Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic and through advertisements. These subjects were a com- bination of those previously reported and new subjects. Results from 10 BN and 10 CW have been previously reported (Weltzin et al 1995). This study reports on an additional 12 BN and 6 CW subjects. All participants provided written informed consent before commencement of the study, and the study obtained approval from the University Institutional Review Board on the Use of Humans as Experimental Subjects. Depression was measured using the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al 1961). BN women were medication free for at least 4 weeks before the study. ATD Procedure Participants were studied within the first 10 days after the onset of their most recent menstrual period. Each subject was studied during a sequential two-day period on 2 separate occasions. On the first day, subjects ate standardized meals consisting of 1500 calories (kcal) (27 g protein and 164 mg tryptophan). No food was consumed after 9:00 PM. On the second day, an indwelling venous catheter was inserted into the antecubital vein at 6:30 AM. At 8:00 AM, participants drank an amino acid (AA) mixture within 5 minutes. Blood samples were drawn at 7:30 AM (baseline) and 1, 2, 4, and 7 hours after ingestion of the AA mixture. The subjects were allowed water but no food until 3:00 PM. Psychological Testing Psychological state was assessed at baseline and 2, 4, and 7 hours after ingestion of the AA mixtures using ratings adapted from those previously described (Asberg et al 1978; Murphy et al 1982; Overall et al 1962). Ratings were conducted by two Psychiatrists (TEW and WHK) who were blind to the treatment condition. At each time-point, using a 6-point scale, a number of psychological variables were assessed including: anxiety, depression, mood lability, obsessionality, irritability, and desire to overeat and purge. Peak changes in mood ratings (peak change from baseline at 4 or 7 hours after AA ingestion) were compared for each individual mood rating. Assessment of Feeding At 1500 hours, subjects were presented with food choices to assess the effects of ATD on feeding behavior. The original subjects (10 BN and 10 CW) previously reported by Weltzin et al (1995) selected their own food from a vending apparatus in the Human Feeding Laboratory (HFL) (Weltzin et al 1991). The vending apparatus in the HFL contained 38 different food and drink items. The food items represented typical binge and non binge foods and are described in detail elsewhere (Kaye et al 1992). Food items were weighed before dispensing so that the amount of calories and macronutrients consumed could be calculated from the uneaten food. The vending machine could be restocked so that a virtually unlimited amount of food was available. The participants were instructed to select as much of any of the foods they wanted and were allowed to binge and vomit if they so desired. The new 12 BN and 6 CW subjects were given similar Tryptophan Depletion in Bulimia BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2000;47:151–157 153 Table 1. Participant Characteristics Variable Age (years) BMI % Average body weight % Low lifetime average weight % High lifetime average weight Age of illness onset (years) Binge frequency (times per week) Purge frequency (times per week) Dieting frequency (times per week) Beck Depression Inventory CW (n ⫽ 19) BN (n ⫽ 22) t p 24.5 ⫾ 5.6 22.0 ⫾ .3 105.1 ⫾ 9.2 95.2 ⫾ 7.2 109.2 ⫾ 9.3 — — — — 1.8 ⫾ 2.4 21.2 ⫾ 5.0 21.2 ⫾ .4 99.9 ⫾ 11.3 90.6 ⫾ 5.8 121.1 ⫾ 21.0 15.9 ⫾ 4.5 9.9 ⫾ 6.7 14.9 ⫾ 6.9 6.3 ⫾ 11.5 18.6 ⫾ 10.8 1.5 1.0 1.2 0.8 1.9 — — — — 4.4 0.10 0.3 0.3 0.4 0.07 — — — — 0.0001 Values are mean ⫾ SD. instructions to those above, but did not use the HFL procedure. Rather, they were presented with a selection of packaged foods, on a tray, consisting of a wide range of normal meal foods (chicken, beef, cottage cheese, yogurt, bread, vegetables, fruit, beverages) and items that are typically preferred during binge eating episodes such as cookies, chocolate, chips, and ice cream (Hadigan et al 1989). Again, food items were weighed before being given to subjects so that the amount of calories and macronutrients consumed could be calculated from the uneaten food. The total energy content of the food provided on the trays was set at 3705 kilocalories. AA Mixtures Using a counterbalanced, double-blind design, two AA mixtures were administered. Both AA mixtures were formulated in our laboratory using purified AA (Ajinomoto USA, Teaneck NJ) by a method reported in detail elsewhere (Weltzin et al 1994a). Each mixture contained a total of 100 g of the following essential and nonessential AA: L-alanine; L-arginine; L-cysteine; L-glycine; Lhistidine; L-isoleucine; L-leucine; lysine; L-methionine; L-phenylalanine; L-proline; L-serine; L-threonine; L-tyrosine; and Lvaline. The ATD mixture did not contain tryptophan (the 0 g tryptophan mixture or “active” mixture), whereas the control mixture did contain tryptophan (the 4.6 g tryptophan mixture or “control mixture”). The addition of 4.6 g of tryptophan has previously been shown to prevent a reduction in the plasma tryptophan:LNAA ratio (Weltzin et al 1994a). Plasma AA Measurements Total plasma tryptophan concentrations were quantified fluorometrically (Denckla et al 1967). The plasma levels of other LNAAs (phenylalanine, tyrosine, leucine, isoleucine and valine) were measured using a Beckman Model 63000 AA analyzer, equipped with a fluorometer to detect O-phthalaldehyde-derivatized AA. Plasma samples were precipitated with sulfosalicylic acid (50 mg/ml plasma), and the supernatant passed through 0.22 mm centrifuge filters (Microfilterfuge Tubes, Rainin Instruments, Woburn, MA). Aminoethyl cysteine was added as an internal standard before sample precipitation (Fernstrom et al 1987). Mean plasma tryptophan:LNAA ratio responses after the control and the deficient amino acid drinks were calculated from the sum of measurements taken at 1, 2, 4, and 7 hours. The change in the tryptophan:LNAA ratio was calculated by subtracting the baseline ratio from the calculated mean plasma tryptophan:LNAA response. Data Analysis and Statistics Analyses were conducted both within and between groups. Group comparisons for age and weight were done using group t-tests. Plasma tryptophan levels and the ratio of tryptophan to the sum of LNAA (Tryptophan: LNAA ratio) were analyzed by repeated measures ANOVA and Tukey’s post hoc t tests. Comparisons of plasma tryptophan and tryptophan:LNAA ratios, after AA ingestion, to those at baseline were done using Newman–Keuls (NK) multiple comparison procedure. Because of a lack of normal distributions, feeding and mood data were analyzed using nonparametric procedures for paired (Wilcoxon signed ranks test) and group comparisons (Mann–Whitney U test). Regression analyses were undertaken to determine if changes in mood and appetite were associated with BECK scores. Correlational analyses were used to examine any relationships between changes in tryptophan:LNAA ratios and changes in mood variables. All the data are presented as mean ⫾ standard deviation. Results Participant Characteristics There were no significant differences between the two groups for most demographic variables (Table 1). The BN women were slightly, but not significantly, younger than the CW women, and there was a trend for the BN group to have a greater percent high lifetime average weight. The BN women had significantly elevated levels of depression compared to the control women. Amino Acid Patterns and Mood and Behavior Ratings Baseline levels of tryptophan and the tryptophan:LNAA ratios were similar in CW and BN subjects (see Table 2). 154 W.H. Kaye et al BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2000;47:151–157 Table 2. Plasma Amino Acids and Mood Ratings: Absolute Mean Baseline Measurements and Peak Changes after a Control Amino Acid Mixture and after Acute Tryptophan Depletion (ATD) Control women (n ⫽ 16) Variable Plasma amino acids Tryptophan (nmol/ml) Tryptophan/LNAA ratio Mood states Anxious Depressed Mood lability Obsessional Irritable Desire to overeat Desire to purge Women with bulimia nervosa Baseline Control ATD Baseline Control ATD 60 ⫾ 13 0.13 ⫾ 0.03 ⫹134 ⫾ 62 ⫹0.03 ⫾ 0.05 ⫺35.6 ⫾ 9.2e ⫺0.11 ⫾ 0.03e 53 ⫾ 16 0.13 ⫾ 0.04 ⫹158 ⫾ 60.4 ⫺0.01 ⫾ 0.03 ⫺29 ⫾ 9.7e ⫺0.12 ⫾ 0.03e 0.5 ⫾ 0.7 0.1 ⫾ 0.5 0⫾0 0⫾0 0.3 ⫾ 0.6 0⫾0 11.2 ⫾ 24.2 ⫺0.5 ⫾ 0.8 ⫹0.3 ⫾ 0.9 ⫹0.1 ⫾ 0.5 ⫹0.2 ⫾ 0.5 ⫺0.06 ⫾ 1.1 ⫹0.3 ⫾ 0.7 ⫹7.2 ⫾ 11.8 ⫺0.3 ⫾ 0.6 ⫹0.3 ⫾ 0.8 ⫹0 ⫾ 0 ⫹0 ⫾ 0 ⫹0.8 ⫾ 1.5d ⫹0.1 ⫾ 0.5 ⫺2.1 ⫾ 27.2 1.5 ⫾ 1.6a 0.9 ⫾ 0.9a 0.05 ⫾ 0.2 1.0 ⫾ 1.4b 0.5 ⫾ 1.2 0.3 ⫾ 0.9 12.6 ⫾ 18.2 ⫺0.2 ⫾ 1.4 ⫹0.2 ⫾ 1.1 ⫹0.3 ⫾ 0.9 ⫺0.4 ⫾ 1.1 ⫹0.2 ⫾ 1.5 ⫹0.5 ⫾ 0.9 ⫹9.2 ⫾ 22 ⫹0.2 ⫾ 1.1 ⫹1.2 ⫾ 1.3b ⫹0.9 ⫾ 1.2c,d ⫹0.2 ⫾ 1.0 ⫹1.7 ⫾ 1.9d ⫹1.0 ⫾ 1.2a ⫹18.9 ⫾ 30b,d Values are mean ⫾ SD. In Control and ATD columns, ⫹ indicates an increase compared to baseline and ⫺ indicates a decrease compared to baseline. For between group comparisons (CW vs. BN Women), group t comparing the same condition: ap ⬍ .05, bp ⬍ .01, cp ⬍ .001. For within group comparisons (control vs. ATD), paired t: dp ⬍ .05, ep ⬍ .001. Similarly, amino acid levels after both interventions were similar for the CW and BN groups. In both CW and BN groups, ATD produced a significant decline in plasma tryptophan concentrations (58% and 55%, respectively) and in the plasma tryptophan:LNAA ratio compared with the ingestion of a control amino acid mixture (85% and 92%, respectively). At baseline, the BN women had significantly higher ratings of anxiety, depression, and obsessionality than the controls (Table 2). For mood levels after both interventions, baseline values were subtracted from peak values (change score). Compared to their response to the control amino acid mixture, BN women had significantly higher levels of mood lability, irritability, and desire to purge after ATD. By contrast, CW only had a significant increase in irritability after ATD as compared to the control amino acid mixture. After ATD, the BN women showed significantly elevated changes in depression, mood lability and desire to overeat and purge compared to the CW women. There were no significant differences in mood changes between BN and CW women after the control amino acid mixture. Regression analyses indicated that changes in mood and appetite ratings after ATD were not associated with the higher Beck Depression Inventory scores in the BN women compared to the CW women (F ⫽ 0.24, p ⫽ .63). Correlational analyses also failed to reveal significant relationships between peak changes in tryptophan:LNAA ratios and peak changes in any of the mood variables in the CW or in the BN groups. Test Meal Intake There were no significant differences in the intake of calories or the intake of any macro nutrients between the two amino acid treatments for either group. Discussion This paper confirms and expands previous reports (Weltzin et al 1994b, 1995) that found that ATD increased negative mood states in BN women compared to healthy controls. Specifically, we found increased susceptibility to depression, mood liability, and desire to overeat and purge after ATD in women with BN compared to controls. These data suggest that women with BN are vulnerable to the mood lowering effects of tryptophan depletion. This corroborates results from other studies that have shown that individuals predisposed to other, possibly serotonergically-mediated psychiatric disorders are particularly susceptible to the depressant effects of tryptophan depletion (Benkelfat et al 1994; Moreno et al 1995; Young et al 1985). We found that ATD produced mild irritability in healthy CW. Other investigators have also found that the ATD paradigm can have mild mood-lowering effects in healthy individuals who do not have psychiatric diagnoses (Barr et al 1997; Smith et al 1987, 1997; Young et al 1985). Studies that have not found this have used a relatively low dose of amino acids (Delgado et al 1989; Moreno et al 1995; Oldman et al 1994; Saloman et al 1997) or demonstrated differential effects in males and females (Ellenbogan et al 1996). ATD was associated with an increased desire to overeat and purge in the BN women. Despite these self reports, food intake was not increased in the BN women after ATD. Differences in the test meal presentation may explain discrepant findings between this current study and our previous report (Weltzin et al 1995), that did find increased food intake after ATD. In the current investigation, presentation of a set selection of foods on a tray may have been more inhibiting than the previously used HFL procedure that permitted access to a virtually unlimited Tryptophan Depletion in Bulimia amount of food. It should also be noted that the small proportion of patients that completed the test-meal procedure may have limited the ability to detect changes in food intake, that tend to be modest after ATD (Oldman et al 1995; Young et al 1988). Alternatively, although reduced 5-HT activity may contribute to negative affect and desire to eat in BN women, it may not in itself be sufficient to precipitate binge eating. The presence of certain other mediating circumstances, such as a high degree of food deprivation, distorted cognitions or perceived negative experiences of the self or of interpersonal interactions (Heatherton 1991; Powell et al 1996), may be necessary to mediate binge eating in the context of reduced 5-HT activity. In addition, the laboratory setting may have been inhibiting for some subjects. What mechanisms could explain the sensitivity of BN patients to tryptophan depletion? It is thought that aberrations in nutritional status (Laessle et al 1988) or dieting can reduce tryptophan availability (Anderson et al 1990; Goodwin et al 1990; Walsh et al 1995; Weltzin et al 1995; Wolfe et al 1997). Baseline plasma tryptophan levels and tryptophan ratios were similar between eating disordered women and controls. Thus, it is unlikely that reduced availability of tryptophan at baseline for conversion to 5-HT accounted for our findings. Still, it is possible that there were differences in bioavailable tryptophan stores that may not have been evident in plasma. Several studies have shown that women with BN have an altered neuroendocrine response to 5-HT agents (Brewerton et al 1992; Goldbloom et al 1990; Halmi et al 1993; Jimerson et al 1997; Levitan et al 1997; McBride et al 1991). Our group (Kaye et al 1998) has found that 5-HT abnormalities persist after recovery from BN. In addition, a recent study showed that a dysphoric response to ATD persisted in remitted BN women (Smith et al 1999). Together these studies suggest that a disturbance of 5-HT neuronal function may be trait-related and contribute to the pathogenesis of BN. We hypothesize that the 5-HT neuronal system in individuals with BN is inherently unstable and poorly modulated. This instability results in the 5-HT system fluctuating erratically rather than having a more precise compensation that buffers the effects of diet or stress as is found in the control women. Dieting (or ATD) may then cause a reduction of 5-HT signal transmission in BN subjects that, in turn, may contribute to binge eating and dysphoric mood (Jimerson et al 1992; Kaye et al 1988). In line with our theory, Jimerson et al (1992) have proposed a model in which individuals with BN may binge eat as a means of self-modulating 5-HT activity. In brief, binge episodes may increase the tryptophan:LNAA ratio in plasma, that in turn increases tryptophan availability to the brain, resulting in increased 5-HT synthesis and release (Fernstrom et al 1971, 1972, 1978). BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2000;47:151–157 155 Thus, individuals with BN may use dieting and binge eating to modulate an unstable 5-HT system. In summary, people with an inherent modulatory defect in 5-HT function may be prone to develop an eating disorder. Because of their modulatory 5-HT defect, they cannot respond appropriately and precisely to stress or stimuli, or modulate their affective states. They may learn that extremes of dietary intake, by effects on plasma TRP, are a means by which they can crudely modulate their brain 5-HT functional activity. Overall, the present study adds to the expanding body of evidence suggesting that aberrations in serotonergic modulation exist in individuals with BN. The tryptophan depletion paradigm demonstrates that the mood of BN women is sensitive to acute changes in 5-HT activity. Further investigations using larger samples may be required to elucidate the definitive effects of tryptophan depletion on appetite and food intake in eating disordered women. Financial support: NIMH #2 R01 MH 42984-04; “The Neurobiology of Feeding Behavior in Bulimia.” References American Psychiatric Association (1994): Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Anderson IM, Parry-Billings M, Newsholme EA, Fairburn CG, Cowen PJ (1990): Dieting reduces plasma tryptophan and alters brain 5-HT function in women. Psychol Med 20:785– 791. Asberg M, Montgomery SA, Perris C, Schalling D, Sedvall G (1978): A comprehensive psychopathological rating scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 271:5–27. Barr LC, Goodman WK, McDougle CJ, Delgado PL, Heninger GR, Charney DS, et al (1994): Tryptophan depletion in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder who respond to serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:309 – 317. Barr LC, Heninger GR, Goodman W, Charney DS, Price LH (1997): Effects of fluoxetine administration on mood response to tryptophan depletion in healthy subjects. Biol Psychiatry 41:949 –954. Beck A, Ward C, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erlbaugh J (1961): An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4:561–571. Benkelfat C, Ellenbogen MA, Dean P, Palmour RM, Young SN (1994): Mood-lowering effects of tryptophan depletion. Enhanced susceptibility to the mood-lowering effect of tryptophan depletion in young male adults at genetic risk for major affective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:687– 697. Biggio G, Fadda F, Fanni P, Tagliamonte A, Gessa GL (1974): Rapid depletion of serum tryptophan, brain tryptophan, sero- 156 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2000;47:151–157 tonin and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid by a tryptophan free diet. Life Sci 14:1321–1329. Blundell JE (1984): Serotonin and appetite. Neuropharmacology 23:1537–1551. Brewerton TD, Mueller EA, Lesem MD, Brandt HA, Quearry B, George T, et al (1992): Neuroendocrine responses to mchlorophenylpiperazine and L-tryptophan in bulimia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49:852– 861. Delgado PL, Charney DS, Price LH, Landis H, Heninger GR (1989): Neuroendocrine and behavioral effects of dietary tryptophan restriction in healthy subjects. Life Sci 45:2323– 2332. Delgado PL, Price LH, Aghajanian GK, Landis H, Heninger GR (1990): Serotonin function and the mechanism of antidepressant action. Reversal of antidepressant-induced remission by rapid depletion of plasma tryptophan. Arch Gen Psychiatry 47:411– 418. Denckla WH, Dewey HK (1967): Determination of tryptophan in plasma, liver and urine. J Lab Clin Med 69:160 –169. Ellenbogan MA, Young SN, Dean P, Palmour RM, Benkelfat C (1996): Mood response to acute tryptophan depletion in healthy volunteers: sex differences and temporal stability. Neuropsychopharmacology 15:465– 474. Fernstrom JD, Faller DV (1978): Neutral amino acids in the brain: changes in response to food ingestion. J Neurochem 30:1531–1538. Fernstrom JD, Fernstrom MH, Grubb PE (1987): Twenty-fourhour variations in rat blood and brain levels of the aromatic and branched-chain amino acids: chronic effects of dietary protein content. Metabolism 36:643– 650. Fernstrom J, Wurtman R (1971): Brain serotonin content: increase following ingestion of carbohydrate diet. Science 174:1023–1025. Fernstrom JD, Wurtman RJ (1972): Brain serotonin content: physiological regulation by plasma neutral amino acids. Science 178:414 – 416. Goldbloom D, Garfinkel P, Katz R, Brown G (1990): The hormonal response to 5-hydroxytryptophan in bulimia nervosa. Psychosom Med 52:225–226. Goodwin GM, Cowen PJ, Fairburn CG, Parry-Billings M, Calder PC, Newsholme EA (1990): Plasma concentrations of tryptophan and dieting. Br Med J 300:1499 –1500. Gwirtsman HE, Roy-Burne P, Yager J (1983): Neuroendocrine abnormalities in bulimia. Am J Psychiatry 140:559. Hadigan CM, Kissileff HR, Walsh T (1989): Patterns of food selection in women with bulimia. Am J Clin Nutr 50:759 – 766. Halmi KA, McBride PA, Sunday SR (1993): Serotonin responsivity and hunger and satiety in eating disorders: a psycho neuroendocrine and metabolic approach. Proceedings of the 2nd International Symposium on Disorders of Eating Behavior. Pavia, Italy, pp 123–131. Heatherton TF, Baumeister RF (1991): Binge eating as an escape from self-awareness. Psychol Bull 110:86 –108. Jimerson DC, Lesem MD, Hegg AP, Brewerton TD (1990): Serotonin in human eating disorders. Ann NY Acad Sci 600:532–544. Jimerson DC, Lesem MD, Kaye WH, Brewerton TD (1992): W.H. Kaye et al Low serotonin and dopamine metabolite concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid from bulimic patients with frequent binge episodes. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49:132–138. Jimerson DC, Wolfe BE, Metzger ED, Finkelstein DM, Cooper TB, Levine JM (1997): Decreased serotonin function in bulimia nervosa. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:529 –534. Kaye WH, Ballenger JC, Lydiard RB, Stuart GW, Laraia MT, O’Neil P, et al (1990): CSF monoamine levels in normal weight bulimia: evidence for altered noradrenergic activity. Am J Psychiatry 147:225–229. Kaye WH, Greeno CG, Moss H, Fernstrom J, Fernstrom M, Lilenfeld LR, et al (1998): Alterations in serotonin activity and psychiatric symptomatology after recovery from bulimia nervosa. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:927–935. Kaye WH, Gwirtsman H, George DT, Obanzek E, Brewerton TD, Jimerson DC, et al (1988): Altered feeding behavior in bulimia: Is it related to mood and serotonin? In: Walsh BT, editor. Eating Behavior in Eating Disorders. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press Inc, pp 201–216. Kaye WH, Weltzin TE, McKee M, McConaha C, Hansen D, Hsu LKG (1992): Laboratory assessment of feeding behavior in bulimia nervosa and healthy women: methods for developing a human-feeding laboratory. Am J Clin Nutr 55:372–380. Laessle RG, Schweiger U, Pirke KM (1988): Depression as a correlate of starvation in patients with eating disorders. Biol Psychiatry 23:719 –725. Levitan RD, Kaplan AS, Joffe RT, Levitt AJ, Brown GM (1997): Hormonal and subjective responses to intravenous metachlorophenylpiperazine in bulimia nervosa. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:521–527. Lilenfeld LR, Kaye WH, Greeno CG, Merikangas K, Plotnikov KH, Pollice C, et al (1998): A controlled family study of anorexia and bulimia nervosa: psychiatric and effects of proband comorbidity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:603– 610. McBride PA, Anderson GM, Khait VD, Sunday SR, Halmi KA (1991): Serotonergic responsivity in eating disorders. Psychopharmacol Bull 27:365–372. Mitchell JE, Raymond N, Specker S (1993): A review of controlled trials of pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy in the treatment of bulimia nervosa. Int J Eating Disorders 14:229 – 247. Moja EA, Cipollo P, Castoldi D, Tofanetti O (1989): Doseresponse decrease in plasma tryptophan and in brain tryptophan and serotonin after tryptophan-free amino acid mixtures in rats. Life Sci 44:971–976. Moreno F, Strayer L, Heninger GR, Delgado PL (1995): Mood response to tryptophan depletion and vulnerability to depression. Soc Neuroscience 21:194. Murphy DL, Pickar D, Alterman IS (1982): Methods for quantitative assessment of depression and manic behavior. In: Burdock EI, Sudilovsky A, Gershon S, editors. The Behavior of Psychiatric patients. New York: Marcel Dekker, pp 355. Oldman A, Walsh A, Salkovskis P, Fairburn CG, Cowen PJ (1995): Biochemical and behavioral effects of acute tryptophan depletion in abstinent bulimic subjects: a pilot study. Psychol Med 25:995–1001. Tryptophan Depletion in Bulimia Oldman AD, Walsh AES, Salkovskis P, Laver DA, Cowen PJ (1994): Effect of acute tryptophan depletion on mood and appetite in healthy female volunteers. J Psychopharmacol 8:8–13. Overall JE, Gorham DR (1962): The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychol Rep 10:799 – 812. Powell AL, Thelen MH (1996): Emotions and cognitions associated with bingeing and weight control behavior in bulimia. J Psychosom Res 40:317–328. Salomon RM, Miller HL, Krystal JH, Heninger GR, Charney DS (1997): Lack of behavioral effects of monoamine depletion in healthy subjects. Biol Psychiatry 41:58 – 64. Smith KA, Clifford EM, Hockney RA, Clark DM, Cowen PJ (1997): Effect of tryptophan depletion on mood in male and female volunteers: a pilot study. Hum Psychopharmacol 12:111–117. Smith KA, Fairburn CG, Cowen PJ (1999): Symptomatic relapse in bulimia nervosa following acute tryptophan depletion. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:171–176. Smith SE, Pihl RO, Young SN, Ervin FR (1987): A test of possible cognitive and environmental influences on the mood lowering effects of tryptophan depletion in normal males. Psychopharmacology 91:451– 457. Walsh AES, Oldman AD, Franklin M, Fairburn CG, Cowen PJ (1995): Dieting decreases plasma tryptophan and increases the prolactin response to d-fenfluramine in women but not men. J Affective Disord 33:89 –97. Walsh BT (1991): Psychopharmacologic treatment of bulimia nervosa. J Clin Psychiatry 52:34 –38. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2000;47:151–157 157 Weltzin TE, Fernstrom JD, McConaha C, Kaye WH (1994a): Acute tryptophan depletion in bulimia: effects on large neutral amino acids. Biol Psychiatry 35:388 –397. Weltzin TE, Fernstrom MH, Fernstrom JD, Neuberger SK, Kaye WH (1995): Acute tryptophan depletion and increased food intake and irritability in bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 152:1668 –1671. Weltzin TE, Fernstrom MH, Kaye WH (1994b): Serotonin and bulimia nervosa. Nutr Rev 52:399 – 408. Weltzin TE, Hsu LKG, Pollice C, Kaye WH (1991): Feeding patterns in bulimia nervosa. Biol Psychiatry 30:1093–1010. Wolfe BE, Metzger ED, Stollar C (1997): The effects of dieting on plasma tryptophan concentration and food intake in healthy women. Phys Behav 61:537–541. Young S (1991): The 1989 Borden Award Lecture. Some effects of dietary components (amino acids, carbohydrate, folic acid) on brain serotonin synthesis, mood, and behavior. Can J Phys Pharmacol 69:893–903. Young SN, Ervin FR, Pihl RO, P. F (1989): Biochemical aspects of tryptophan depletion in primates. Psychopharmacology 98:508 –511. Young SN, Smith S, Pihl RO, Ervin FR (1985): Tryptophan depletion causes a rapid lowering of mood in normal males. Psychopharmacology 87:173–177. Young SN, Tourjman SV, Teff KL, Pihl RO, Anderson GH (1988): The effect of lowering plasma tryptophan on food selection in normal males. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 31: 149 –152.