Perceptual distance in Norwegian retroflexion Sverre Stausland Johnsen Phon circle, MIT Nov 2 2009 Norwegian retroflexes • In Urban East Norwegian (UEN), a laminal coronal series /t d n s/ contrasts with a retroflex series /T D N S/ • /kɑt/ ‘cat’ - /kɑT/ ‘map’ • /ɾɔːd/ ‘advice’ - /ɭɔːD/ ‘lord’ • /tʉːn/ ‘yard’ - /tʉːN/ ‘gymnastics’ • /mɑːs/ ‘nagging’ - /mɑːS/ ‘Mars’ Norwegian retroflexion • Retroflexes can also be derived across morpheme boundaries • When a morpheme ends in /-ɾ/, and the following morpheme begins with /t d n s/, the sequence surfaces as /T D N S/ • /ʋɔːɾ-tæjn/ > /ʋɔː-Tæjn/ ‘spring sign’ • /ʋɔːɾ-dɑːg/ > /ʋɔː-Dɑːg/ ‘spring day’ • /ʋɔːɾ-nɑt/ > /ʋɔː-Nɑt/ ‘spring night’ • /ʋɔːɾ-suːɽ/ > /ʋɔː-Suːɽ/ ‘spring sun’ Rate of retroflexion • Two experiments tested how often retroflexion is applied • The results revealed the following hierarchy: d/n > sk > st > s (> = ‘undergoes retroflexion significantly more often than’) • This means that /d/ is more likely to alternate with /D/ than /s/ is to alternate with /S/ Perceptual distance • Steriade (2001, 2009) proposes that the greater the perceptual distance between two forms x and y, the less likely x and y are to alternate • Could imply that /s/ alternates less with /S/ than /d/ with /D/ because the perceptual distance in /s/-/S/ is greater than in /d/-/D/ • If so … Perceptual distance hierarchy • Then the perceptual distance hierarchy should be the inverse of the retroflexion hierarchy • Retroflexion hierarchy: d/n > sk > st > s • Hypothesized perceptual distance hierarchy: s > st > sk > d/n (& t) = The perceptual distance /s/-/S/ is greater than the perceptual distance /st/-/ST/, etc. Perceptual experiment • 12 UEN subjects in an AX discrimination task • Stimuli were two groups of /ɑCɑ/ words: 1) C = /s st sk t d n/ 2) C = /S ST SK T D N/ • Amplitude of the vowels was RMS equalized • Trial overlaid with babble noise (S/N ca. –7 dB) • /ɑsɑ/ - /ɑSɑ/ • 192 trials x 12 subjects = 2304 trials * * * * • Perceptual hierarchy from the experiment: s > sC > t/d/n • sC = st > sk ? • In the experiment /st/ and /sk/ were treated the same • Could be the result of the relatively clear distinction between the sibilants in /st/ - /ST/ and /sk/ - /SK/ • In the experiment, the /s-S/ distinction trumps any other distinctions, so /st/ and /sk/ come out the same • If so, /st/ and /sk/ should be the same when only the sibilant is presented • Subjects were presented with only /ɑs/ and /ɑS/, excised from the original /ɑstɑ/-/ɑskɑ/ sets p = .98 • If /st/ is different from /sk/, then the difference lies in the following consonant • Test whether the remaining /t-T/ is more distinct than /k-K/ • If /c/ can be distinguished from /C/, it means that they have different phonetic qualities correlating with the quality of the preceding sibilant (/s/-/S/) • Speakers should be able to identify the preceding sibilant from the quality of the stop • The perceptual distance /c/-/C/ was therefore measured by how successfully subjects identified the preceding sibilant as /s/ or /S/ • Presented as an identification task • No added noise • /Tɑ/ /tɑ/ • 96 trials x 12 subjects = 1152 trials * * • The hypothesized perceptual distance hierarchy is confirmed The question How can perceptual distance influence phonological production? • Speakers have a detail rich representation of words and categories (Goldinger 1998) • This representation is continuously updated • When perceiving a token, various factors might a) Make correct lexical access difficult b) Prevent correct lexical access altogether • Perceptual distance is such a factor or Marslen-Wilson et al. 1996 • Result: • x-y pairs where a non-word y differed from a word x only in the initial segment. The greater the perceptual distance between x and y, the less y activated word x • Implication: • The greater the perceptual distance in x-y, the greater the chance that y is not identified as x (with a different pronunciation) • Whenever UEN [Suːɽ] is accessed as a token of the word /suːɽ/, it is recognized as /suːɽ/ with a different pronunciation • But with the large perceptual distance between /suːɽ/ and /Suːɽ/, tokens like [Suːɽ] will on occasion not be recognized as the word /suːɽ/ • For [Nɑt]-/nɑt/, where the perceptual distance is very small, this happens much less often • Words in /s-/ will be updated with [S]-tokens less often than words in /n-/ are updated with [N]-tokens • Over time, this can accumulate to significant differences (Wedel 2006) • Phonological productions are directly influenced by speakers’ own detailed representations (Goldinger 1998, Pierrehumbert 2002) • If these representations contain fewer [S]tokens relative to /s/ than [N] to /n/, then speakers will replicate that distribution in production • In short, people say what they hear … Summary • The rate of retroflexion of a coronal /c/ is inversely correlated with the perceptual distance between /c/ and the retroflex /C/ • The greater the distance /c/-/C/, the more often a retroflexed [C]-token will not be identified with the /c/-word • As a direct result, speakers replicate this pattern in their own production References • Goldinger, S. 1998. Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. • Marslen-Wilson, W., H. Moss & S. van Halen. 1996. Perceptual distance and competition in lexical access. • Pierrehumbert, J. 2002. Word-specific phonetics. • Steriade, D. 2001. Directional asymmetries in place assimilation. • Steriade, D. 2009. The phonology of perceptibility effects. • Wedel, A. 2006. Exemplar models, evolution, and language change.