Electrophysiological Evidence for the Failure of Salient Stimuli to Capture Attention, even when Presented Rarely Birken Noesen & Mei-Ching Lien Eric Ruthruff Oregon State University N2pc Effect = ERP contralateral to cue– ERP Ipsilateral to cue Experiment 1 (N = 16) Purpose: Determine whether an irrelevant color singleton has the power to capture spatial attention, producing a cuing validity effect and an N2pc, when it appears rarely. Target Display: Each participant responded to either the red letter, the green letter, the blue letter, or the yellow letter (4 groups). The target displays contained two T’s and two L’s. For the red and green target groups, one letter was red, one was green, and two were white. For the blue and yellow target groups, one letter was blue, one was yellow, and two were white. The two colored letters always appeared on opposite sides of the display. 1200-1400 ms 50 ms 100 ms T L L T 50 ms Until Response 100 ms TIME EEG Data Analyses: EEG epochs were time-locked to singleton cue onset. The cue-elicited N2pc was assessed 170-270 ms after cue onset; the targetelicited N2pc was assessed 350-450 ms after cue onset. Sites: O1, O2, PO5, PO6, P5, and P6 Predictions: If a salient-but-irrelevant singleton can capture attention when presented rarely, the cue should produce cue validity effects and N2pc effects. Cue displays for Red/Green targets For the blue and yellow target groups, the color singleton cue was a red box (among green boxes) or a green box (among red boxes). Relevant Cue Only Behavioral Data: Cue displays for Blue/Yellow targets 10% Different Hemifields Relevant + Onset Cue Results and Discussion 650 Mean RT was not significantly Behavioral Data: different between the no-cue condition and the valid and invalid 650 cue conditions, Fs(1,12)≤1.78, 53 ms* 600 ps≥.2072, indicating that the salientbut-irrelevant cue did not capture 550 attention. 600 550 10% Same Hemifield 80% 527 ms 533 ms 533 ms 500 450 400 Valid Invalid Color Singleton Cue No Cue N2pc Effects (cue present trials): * p <.0001 68 ms* 500 450 Relevant Cue Cue-Elicited N2pc Effects -200 N2pc Effects: 0 200 400 600 Cue-Elicited N2pc Effects Relevant Cue -200 3 200 400 600 800 Post-Cue Interval (ms) -3 Cue and target in the same hemifield Cue and target in different hemifields -200 Relevant + Onset Cue This research was supported by funding from Oregon State University Undergraduate Research, Innovation, Scholarship, and Creativity to Birken Noesen. 0 0 Post-Cue Interval (ms) Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030-1044. Horstmann, G., & Ansorge, U. (2006). Attentional shifts to rare singletons. Visual Cognition, 14, 295-325. Lien, M.-C., Ruthruff, E., Goodin, Z., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: Converging evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 509-530. Target-Elicited N2pc Effects -3 800 0 3 Relevant + Onset Cue Relevant cues (in the target color) produced large cue validity effects, regardless of whether there was a simultaneous abrupt onset, ts(9)≥7.68, ps<.0001. Instead of reducing capture by the relevant stimulus, the onset may have actually increased capture, F(1,8)=3.50, p=.09. Target-Elicited N2pc Effects -3 Pooled Valid Invalid 400 Acknowledgements OR Design: Half of the participants responded to the red letter in the target display and the other half responded to the green letter. The cue display contained a box colored the same as the target letter, which would normally capture attention. On 80% of the trials, this relevant cue appeared by itself, and on the other 20% of trials, it had to compete with a simultaneous abrupt onset (the relevant+onset cue). The onset appeared in the same hemifield as the relevant cue (but not the same location) for 10% of the trials, but in the other hemifield for the other 10% of the trials (see below for examples for the target color red). Results and Discussion Cue Display: 80% of the trials (1024 trials in total) contained no cue, whereas The irrelevant color singleton cue produced no N2pc effect during the period 170-270 ms after cue onset, ts(15)<1.0, suggesting that it did not capture 20% of the trials (256 trials in total) contained an irrelevant color singleton. attention, despite being presented rarely. For the red and green target groups, the color singleton cue was a blue box (among yellow boxes) or a yellow box (among blue boxes). References OR Purpose: Examine the ability of rare abrupt onsets (20% of trials) to capture attention away from a relevant cue. uV To assess capture, we measured cue validity effects (e.g., shorter RT for valid trials than invalid trials). We also assessed capture using an electrophysiological measure - the N2pc - believed to reflect the allocation of spatial attention. When attention is directed to the left or right visual field, brain potentials are found to be more negative in the contralateral hemisphere than in the ipsilateral hemisphere. The N2pc is the difference in event-related brain potentials (ERPs) between these hemispheres. It is usually strongest over posterior scalp, about 170-270 ms after stimulus onset. The N2pc effect provides an index of whether and when attention has been directed to the cue. 100 ms Target uV The goal of the present study was to determine whether salient-but-irrelevant objects can trigger the involuntary capture when presented rarely. We used a cuing paradigm, with non-informative cues (25% valid vs. 75% invalid). 1200 ms Cue Tone Feedback Experiment 2 (N=10) Response Time (in ms) The Present Study Fixation uV Can salient objects capture our attention? Some have argued that the salient stimuli, such as color singletons, do not have inherent power to capture our spatial attention unless they contain some properties match those we are currently looking for (contingent capture; e.g., Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992; Lien, Ruthruff, Goodin, & Remington, 2008). Others have argued that frequent present presentation of the salient objects diminished their power to capture attention (e.g., Horstmann & Ansorge, 2006). The present study examined capture by salient objects when presented only occasionally, using both behavioral measures (response time [RT] and proportion of error [PE]) and electrophysiological measures. Sample Event Sequence: The target was red in this example. Response Time (in ms) Introduction University of New Mexico 0 200 400 600 800 0 3 Post-Cue Interval (ms) Cue and target in the same hemifield Cue and target in different hemifields The relevant cue by itself elicited a substantial N2pc effect (-0.398 µV), ts(9)≥2.19, ps≤.05. When competing with an abrupt onset, the relevant cue still elicited a strong N2pc effect (-0.386 µV; ts(9)≥-1.33, ps ≤.22); the difference was not significant, F<1.0. Conclusions Using behavioral and electrophysiological measures, we found no evidence that salient-but-irrelevant objects (color singletons and abrupt onsets) have the power to capture spatial attention, even when presented rarely (20% of trials). We found that capture was driven primarily by top-down control settings, not bottom-up stimulus salience.