Downloaded from rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org on January 28, 2014 PROCEEDINGS THE ROYAL O F SO C IE T Y B IO L O G IC A L SCIENCES How superdiffusion gets arrested: ecological encounters explain shift from Levy to Brownian movement Monique de Jager, Frederic Bartumeus, Andrea Kölzsch, Franz J. Weissing, Geerten M. Flengeveld, Bart A. Nolet, Peter M. J. Flerman and Johan van de Koppei Proc. R. Soc. S 2014 281 , 20132605, published 13 November 2013 Supplementary data "Data Supplement" http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/suppl/2013/11/12/rspb.2013.2605.DC1 .h tml References T his artic le cites 29 artic les, 8 o f w h ich can be a c ce ss ed free Qopen access Subject Collections http://rspb.royalsocietypubiishing.Org/content/281/1774/20132605.full.html#ref-list-1 This article is free to access Articles on similar topics can be found in the following collections ecology (1536 articles) R eceive free em ail alerts w hen new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top rig ht-ha nd corn er or the article or click here To subscribe to Proc. R. Soc. B g o to : http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/subscriptions Downloaded from rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org on January 28, 2014 PROCEEDINGS O F ---------- TH E ROYAL SOCIETY rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org Research 3 ® CrossMark Cite this article: d e Jager M, Bartum eus F, Kölzsch A, W eissing FJ, H engeveld GM, N olet BA, Herman PMJ, van d e Koppel J. 2 0 1 4 How superdiffusion g e ts arrested: ecological encou nters explain shift from Lévy to Brownian m ovem en t. Proc. R. Soc. B 281: 20132 605. http ://d x.doi.O rg/10.1098/rsp b .2 0 1 3.2605 Received: 4 O ctober 2013 Accepted: 17 October 2013 Subject Areas: ecology Keywords: Brownian m otion , Lévy w alk, anim al m o vem en t, M ytilus edulis, search efficiency, resource density How superdiffusion gets arrested: ecological encounters explain shift from Lévy to Brownian movement M onique de Jager1-2-4, Frederic Bartum eus5, Andrea Kölzsch6-7, Franz J. W eissing2, G eerten M. H engeveld6-7, Bart A. N olet6-7, Peter M. J. H erm an1 and Johan van de K oppei1-3-7 1S p atial Ecology D e p a rtm e n t, Royal N e th e rla n d s In s titu te fo r Sea R esearch (NIOZ), PO Box 1 4 0 , 4 4 0 0 AC V erseke, T he N e th e rla n d s ^T heo retical B iology G roup, a n d C o m m u n ity a n d C o nserv ation Ecology G roup, C entre fo r Ecological a n d Ev olutio nary S tu d ie s, U niversity o f G ro n in g en , N ijen b o rg h 7, 9 7 4 7 AG G ro n in g e n , T he N e th e rla n d s i n s t i t u t e o f In te g ra tiv e Biology, ETH Zürich, U n iv e rsita e tstra sse 16, 8 0 9 2 Zürich, S w itz erla n d 5 C e n te r fo r A d van ced S tu d ie s o f B lanes (CEAB-CSIC), A ccès Cala S a n t Francesc, 1 4 ,1 7 3 0 0 B lanes, G irona, Spain d e p a r t m e n t o f A nim al Ecology, a n d 7 P ro ject G roup M o v e m e n t Ecology, N e th e rla n d s In s titu te o f Ecology , PO Box 5 0, 6 7 0 0 AB W a g e n in g e n , T he N e th e rla n d s Ecological theory uses Brownian m otion as a default tem plate for describing ecological m ovem ent, despite lim ited m echanistic underpinning. The general­ ity of Brownian m otion has recently been challenged b y em pirical studies that highlight alternative m ovem ent patterns of anim als, especially w h en foraging in resource-poor environm ents. Yet, em pirical studies reveal anim als m oving in a Brownian fashion w h en resources are abundant. We dem onstrate that Einstein's original theory of collision-induced Brownian m otion in physics provides a parsim onious, mechanistic explanation for these observations. Here, Brownian m otion results from frequent encounters betw een organism s in dense environm ents. In density-controlled experiments, m ovem ent patterns of m ussels shifted from Levy tow ards Brownian m otion w ith increasing den ­ sity. W hen the analysis w as restricted to moves n o t truncated b y encounters, this shift d id n o t occur. U sing a theoretical argum ent, w e explain that any m ovem ent pattern approxim ates B rownian m otion at high-resource densities, provided that m ovem ent is in terru p ted u p o n encounters. Hence, the observed shift to Brownian m otion does n o t indicate a density-dependent change in m ovem ent strategy b u t rather results from frequent collisions. O u r results em phasize the need for a m ore mechanistic use of Brownian m otion in ecology, highlighting that especially in rich environm ents, Brownian m otion em erges from ecological interactions, rather th an being a default m ovem ent pattern. Author for correspondence: M onigue d e Jager e-m ail: m onigue.deJager@ usys.ethz.ch Electronic supp lem en tary material is available at h ttp ://d x.d oi.org /10.1098/rsp b .2013.260 5 or 1. Introduction Traditionally, ecologists ap ply Brow nian m otion an d diffusive dispersal as default m odels for anim al m ovem ent [1,2], b o th at in dividual a n d at population levels [3-5]. Recently, how ever, em pirical studies have show n th at anim al m ove­ m ent can strongly deviate from Brow nian m otion [6], revealing superdiffusive, Lévy-like m ovem ent in resource-poor environm ents, b u t stan d ard Brownian m otion w h en resource availability is h igh [7-11]. A nim al ecologists have explained this change from Lévy to Brow nian m otion by an active shift in indi­ v idu al m ovem ent strategy, reflecting the assum ption that different m ovem ent strategies are optim al u n d er different environm ental conditions [10-13]. In h et­ erogeneous, resource-poor environm ents, Lévy m ovem ent w ill typically b e m ore efficient th an a Brownian w alk because it provides faster dispersal an d prevents revisiting the sam e sites [14]. In resource-rich environm ents, a Brownian w alk m ay be equally or even m ore efficient th an a Lévy w alk, because large steps via http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org. © 2 01 3 The A u th o rs. P u b lish ed by th e Royal S ociety u n d e r th e te r m s o f th e C reative C o m m o n s A ttrib u tio n Royal S ociety P u b lis h in g License h ttp ://c re a tiv e c o m m o n s .O rg /lic e n s e s/b y /3 .0 /, w h ich p e rm its u n re stric te d u se , p ro vided th e o riginal a u th o r a n d so u rce a re cre d ite d . Downloaded from rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org on January 28, 2014 2ae (2 . 1) w here a is the length betw een position i an d i + 1, b is the length betw een position i — 1 an d i + 1 an d c is the length betw een positions i — 1 an d i. W henever a w as larger than a threshold angle aT, a new step is considered to start. Follow­ ing Turchin's approach [24], w e used aT = tt/ 5 for our step length calculations, as this value m inim ized autocorrelation betw een subsequent turns. Using other threshold angles did not change our conclusions. We studied the changes in the statistical properties of the observed m ovem ent p attern b y recording 10 individual m ovem ent trajectories for five different density treatm ents each (0, 1.3, 2.0, 3.3 an d 5.2 kg m -2 , approx. 1, 950, 1550, 2500 an d 3850 m ussels m -2 ) d u rin g the initial 300 m in of p at­ tern form ation [23]. W hen a m ussel encountered an obstacle, for exam ple a conspecific, it w as forced to truncate its step, w hich w ill probably alter the properties of the m ovem ent pattern. We u sed the com plem entary cum ulative distribution function (CCDF) of the observed step lengths of each individ­ ual m ussel in the five density treatm ents to illustrate the observed m ovem ent patterns. This CCDF is a preferred m ethod for fitting pow er distributions as it provides a m ore reliable representation of m ovem ent pattern s th an other portraying m ethods [3]. For each step length /, the CCDF(/ ) of the observed step lengths in each density treat­ m ent indicates the fraction of step lengths that w ere at least as long as I. U sing m axim um -likelihood m ethods, w e esti­ m ated the scaling exponent /i of a pow er-law step length distribution, (2 .2 ) 2. Experiments (a) M eth o d s U sing mesocosm experiments, w e investigated how mussel m ovem ent patterns are affected b y m ussel density. Young blue mussels (M. edulis) of approxim ately 1.5 cm in length w ere obtained from w ooden wave-breaker poles on the beaches near Vlissingen, The N etherlands (51°46' N , 3°53' E). After care­ ful separation an d cleaning, the mussels w ere kept in containers an d fed live cultures of diatom s (Phaeodactylum tricornutum) w here I is the step length an d /nlin is the m inim al step length of young m ussels (/nlin < I) [3,18,25,26]. The step length distribution corresponds to a Lévy w alk for 1 < |x < 3 an d it approxim ates a Brownian w alk w hen > 3 [27]. We apply a sim ple pow er-law m odel rather th an a m ore com plex com posite m odel because w e are inter­ ested in the change of general statistical properties w ith m ussel density rather th an in a detailed statistical description of m ussel m ovem ent [18-20]. First, w e kept the m inim al step length constant at the fixed value /min = 3 m m . G iven /min, Proc. /?. Soc. B 281: 20132605 daily. Fresh, unfiltered seaw ater w as supplied to the container at a rate of approxim ately 11 m in -1; a constant w ater tem pera­ ture of 16°C w as m aintained during the experiments. A t the start of each experiment, m ussels w ere spread hom ogeneously over an 80 x 60 cm red PVC sheet in a 120 x 80 x 30 cm con­ tainer. We used a red PVC sheet to provide a contrast-rich surface for later analysis an d considered only the m ovem ents of the m ussels w ithin this 80 x 60 cm arena. The container w as illum inated using fluorescent lam ps. M ussel m ovem ent w as recorded b y photographing the m ussels at 1 m in intervals for a duration of 300 min; w e used a Logitech Q uickCam 9000 Pro w ebcam (w w w.logitech.com ), w hich w as positioned about 60 cm above the w ater surface. We derived the step lengths b y calculating the distance betw een tw o reorientation events (e.g. w here a m ussel clearly changes its direction of movement) using Turchin's angle m ethod [18,24]. First, the observed m ovem ent path is discre­ tized into steps on basis of changes in the angle (a) of the m ovem ent p ath at observed position i using the prior (i — 1) an d the subsequent (i + 1) observed locations as follows: rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org (which are the hallm ark of Lévy m ovem ent) provide little benefit u n d er these circum stances [11]. Physical theory offers an alternative, m ore parsim onious explanation for the occurrence of Brownian m otion in resource-rich environm ents. Einstein, followed b y Langevin, theorized that Brownian m otion in solutes results from col­ lisions betw een particles [15,16]. Likewise, Brownian m otion in ecology m ight result from frequent 'collisions' of anim als w ith the resources they are searching for (food, shelter or conspecifics), or w ith item s that they are trying to avoid (e.g. terri­ tory boundaries [17]). Untangling w hether the observed m ovem ent patterns in searching animals reflect adaptation of intrinsic m ovem ent strategies, or are the consequence of chan­ ging encounter (collision) rates w ith resources, is crucial both for sound mechanistic understanding of Brownian m otion an d for predicting anim al m ovem ent patterns in ecosystems w here resource availability varies in space or time. Here, w e provide evidence that, as in physics, Brownian w alks in anim al m ovem ents can be caused by frequent encoun­ ters, rather than being the result of adaptation to high-density conditions. In density-controlled experim ents w ith young m ussels (Mytilus edulis), w e w ere able to distinguish betw een intrinsic m ovem ent strategy an d the effects of resource density b y separating the m ovem ent steps that w ere truncated by encounters from those that w ere term inated spontaneously. Recently, it w as show n that the individual m ovem ent of young m ussels can be approxim ated b y a sim ple Lévy w alk [18] (or a m ore complex multi-scale w alk, w hich provides an even better fit [19,20]). The m ovem ent of individual m ussels results in a self-organized m ussel bed w ith a regular labyrinth-like pattern w here local aggregation yields protection against w ave stress an d predation w hile it reduces com petition for algal food resources [21-23]. As the m ovem ent of individ­ ual m ussels can be experim entally studied in considerable detail, this system provides a unique opportunity to investigate how anim al m ovem ent patterns are affected by truncation of m oves ow ing to encounters. This pap er is structured as follows. First, w e describe m ove­ m ent of young m ussels observed in density-controlled experiments, revealing that m ovem ent patterns are affected by changes in the density of mussels. By distinguishing betw een obstructed an d unobstructed m ovem ent steps, w e investigate the relationship betw een intended an d realized m ovem ent p at­ terns. Second, w e create an individual-based m odel of self­ organized pattern form ation in m ussel beds to examine w hether m ussel density could cause a change in the efficiency of Brow­ nian and Lévy walks, explaining a possible active shift in m ussel m ovem ent strategy. Third, w e use a general argum ent to dem onstrate that the interplay betw een any intrinsic m ove­ m ent strategy an d frequent ecological encounters will often result in Brownian motion. Downloaded from rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org on January 28, 2014 ( a) 50 n m ussel 2 5 .2kgnW rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org mussel 15 O k g rrr2 2 5 0 -, 40- 200- 3020- 50- 10 - 0-1 0J 20 30 40 50 U 15 (d) (c) & Q U 10 60 0 .5 0 - 0 .5 0 - 0 .2 0 - 0.2 0 - 0 .0 5 - 0.05 - LW — BW 0.01 J 0 . 01-1 0.05 0.20 1.00 5.00 20.00 step length (mm) 8 10 12 14 step length (mm) Figure 1. Step length distributions and m odel fits for m o vem en t trajectories at tw o m ussel den sities. Step len gth freguency distributions o f m ussel 15 in th e 0 kg m - 2 treatm ent (a) and m ussel 2 in th e 5.2 kg m - 2 treatm ent (6), tog eth er w ith an illustration o f th e m o vem en t paths (red dots indicate encounters w ith conspecifics). The fitted lines to th e CCDFs o f th e step len gth s o f m ussel 15 (c) and m ussel 2 ( d ) indicate h o w w ell th e m o vem en t trajectories are represented by a Lévy w alk (LW) and a Brownian w alk (BW). the exponent p, can b e estim ated from the likelihood function [25,26,28,29] l (m , . . . , z„ ) = Um i (2.3) w here {/,-... /„} are the observed step lengths. Taking the natural logarithm of L an d m axim izing w ith respect to |x yields the m axim um -likelihood estim ate (i = 1 + U ■ ln(/;) - In (/min)) . (2.4) To check for the robustness of our results, w e also fitted the observed step length distribution to a pow er law w here the value of /min w as estim ated separately for each individual tra­ jectory (by equating /min w ith the m inim al observed step length). O ur conclusions w ere n o t affected in any way. By labelling steps as truncated w henever the step end ed directly in front of another m ussel, w e w ere able to dis­ tinguish pure, non-truncated steps from those truncated by collisions w ith conspecifics. For the sam e 10 individuals in the five density treatm ents (50 m ussels in total), w e split the steps into truncated a n d non-truncated steps, exam ining the distributions separately. (b) R esults O ur mesocosm experiments illustrate that the observed move­ m ent patterns are strongly affected by mussel density (figures 1 an d 2; see the electronic supplem entary material, table SI). Long steps occur less frequently w ith increasing mussel density (figure 2a). The scaling exponent p, increases w ith mussel density from a value below 2.5 at low densities to values above 3.5 at high densities (figure 2b). As a second test of our hypothesis that observed m ovement trajectories become more Brownian-like w ith increased resource density, w e used the Akaike information criterion for deciding w hether the individual trajectories in each density class were better fitted by a pow er law or by an exponen­ tial distribution (corresponding to a Brownian walk). In 83% of the m ovement trajectories in the lowest density treatment, a Lévy w alk provided a better fit to the step length data than a Brownian walk. By contrast, 75% of the tracks in the high-density treatm ent were better approxim ated by a Brownian w alk than by a Lévy walk. Again, w e conclude that m ovement trajectories become more Brownian-like w ith increasing mussel density. Closer exam ination of the m ovem ent data indicates that the change of step length distribution w ith m ussel density results from the frequent truncation of step lengths at high densities (figure 2c,d). The fraction of truncated steps increases w ith m ussel density (figure 2c), presum ably because the num ber of encounters leading to an interruption of the m ovem ent increases w ith density. W hen only considering non-truncated steps, m ussel m ovem ent does n ot significantly differ b et­ w een density treatm ents (figure 2d). We conclude that the intrinsic m ovem ent strategy of the m ussels does n o t change w ith density an d that the observed change from Lévy-like to Brownian-like m ovem ent results solely from the increased m ussel encounter rates at high density. Proc. R. Soc. B 281: 20132605 10 Downloaded from rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org on January 28, 2014 (b) 1.0 -, rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org È 0.8 0. 1 g 0.6 -1 G G Ü 0 .0 1 - 0.4 - G O k g rrr2 2.0 kg n r 5.2 kg n r 0.001 -I 1 * 10 0.2 - 0 - 100 30 0 step length (mm) 1.3 2.0 3.3 5.2 mussel density (kgm -2) (c) ( d ) 5.0 5.0 all steps 4.5 H 4 .5 - 4.0 4 .0 - 3.5 3.5 3.0 3 .0 - 2.5 H 2 .5 - 2.0 2 .0 1.5 T 0 1- - - - - - - 1- ----- r 1 2 3 4 5 mussel density (kgm -2 ) non-truncated steps only - 1 .5 - i-----------1----------1----------1----------1-------- 1 0 1 2 3 4 mussel density (kgm -2) 5 Figure 2. Effect o f m ussel d en sity on individual m o vem en t trajectories, (a) CCDF o f th e pooled step len gth s o f m oving m u ssels m easured for three d en sity treat­ m en ts. W ith increasing m ussel density, th e fraction o f lon g step s decreases. (6) Estim ated scaling exp on en t ¡a as a function o f m ussel density; ¡x increases w ith m ussel den sity (linear regression, /3 n = 0.73, r = 0 .4 6 , d.f. = 46, p < 0.001; bars indicate average ¡x per den sity group ± s . e . ) and takes on valu es beyond 3 at high den sities, (c) The fraction o f step s th at are truncated by collisions increases w ith m ussel d en sity (bars indicate m ean s + s.e.). (rf) W hen considering th e nontruncated steps only, th e scaling exp on en t ¡x rem ains approxim ately constan t (linear regression, /3 n = 0.18, r = 0, d.f. = 26, p = 0.593; bars indicate average ¡x per d en sity group + s.e.). 3. A model of mussel movement (a) M eth o d s U sing a well-established m odel for m ussel m ovem ent [18], w e investigated w hether an active sw itch from Lévy to Brow­ nian m ovem ent at high densities is m ore efficient th an the persistent use of Lévy m ovem ent. We ran individual-based com puter sim ulations for a range of values of the scaling exponent p, an d at various densities, w here w e repeated each sim ulation 10 tim es to account for stochasticity. W hen­ ever a displacem ent w as restricted b y the presence of a conspecific, the step w as truncated. In each sim ulation, w e determ ined the sum D of all displacem ents required before the m ussels settled in a stable pattern. The inverse of D can b e view ed as a m easure of the patterning efficiency of the m ovem ent strategy u n d er consideration [18,30]. (b) R esults Brow nian m ovem ent is often assum ed to be m ore efficient in dense environm ents; som e researchers thus argue th at ani­ m als sw itch from Lévy to Brownian m ovem ent w h en encountering areas of higher resource density. H ow ever, sim ulations w ith ou r individual-based m odel [18] of m ussel m ovem ent dem onstrate that Lévy m ovem ent is at least as efficient as Brownian m otion at all densities. A t low densities, a Lévy w alk w ith exponent p, ~ 2 is the m ost efficient m ovem ent strategy (figure 3). A t higher densities, all m ove­ m ent strategies w ith 2 < p, < 3 lead to Brownian-like m ovem ent patterns a n d therefore have a sim ilar patterning efficiency; hence, the sim ulations do n o t su p p o rt the h y p o th ­ esis that Brow nian m ovem ent strategies lead to m ore efficient aggregation th an Lévy m ovem ent strategies. This im plies that there is no necessity to sw itch to a Brow nian strategy w ith increasing density an d the m ussels in o u r experim ents do n o t behave suboptim ally w h en using a Lévy w alk at high densities (figure 2d). 4. A general argument By m eans of a general argum ent, it can b e seen th at the tran ­ sition from non-Brow nian to Brow nian m otion at high densities is a general phenom enon an d n ot restricted to m ussel m ovem ent. C onsider a pop u latio n of anim als w here the individuals have a certain intrinsic m ovem ent strategy, for exam ple a Lévy w alk w ith a given exponent p,. If all indi­ v iduals could com plete their m ovem ent steps u ninterrupted, this m ovem ent strategy w o u ld result in a step length dis­ tribution w ith a com plem entary cum ulative distribution function CCDFintended^ ) (as in figure 2a, CCDF(/) corre­ sponds to the probability th at a step is longer th an or equal to /). Suppose now that an anim al term inates its m ovem ent w henever it encounters its desired target, such as food or Proc. R. Soc. B 281; 20132605 3 .2 Downloaded from rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org on January 28, 2014 5. Discussion 77= 500 77= 750 77= 1000 77= 1250 „ = 1500 > o> c » o 0-1 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 A* Figure 3. Patterning efficiency as a function o f th e scaling exp on en t /a in m odel sim u lations for five different m ussel den sities. At low m ussel d en sity {n = 50 0 ), a LW w ith /a « 2 has th e highest patterning efficiency, i.e. this m o vem en t strategy creates a spatial pattern w ith a m inim um o f displace­ m en ts. At higher d en sities, a LW w ith ¡x « 2 still appears op tim al, but m ost other m o vem en t strategies (including a BW) perform equally w ell. Bars indicate m ean s o f 10 sim ulations + s.d.; lines illustrate cubic sm oothin g splin es through th e m odel results. Patterning efficiency, m easured as th e inverse o f th e distance D m oved per m ussel until a pattern w as form ed, w as norm alized by dividing by th e largest efficiency found in all sim ulations. shelter. (The sam e argum ents apply w hen m oves are term i­ nated ow ing to encounters w ith obstacles or the presence of a potential danger, such as a predator or a rival.) If the encounters of the m oving anim als w ith the target objects are random , the probability that an intended step of length I w ill not be term inated is given b y the zero term of a Poisson distribution: e~kAl, w here A is the density of target objects an d k is a constant of proportionality th at reflects aspects such as the search w indow of the anim al or the size an d visi­ bility of the target objects. As a consequence, the CCDF of the realized (and observed) step length distribution is given by CCDFrealized(/) = CCDFintendedj/) ■e^M'. (4.5) As step lengths w ill becom e shorter ow ing to the term in­ ation of steps b y encounters, the realized step length distribution w ill have a different signature than the intended step length distribution. In particular, intended longer steps w ill be term inated m ore often than intended shorter steps, an d the probability that a step is term inated w ill d epen d on the density of target objects. For large densities of the target object, the exponential term becom es dom inant an d forces the tail of the CCDF tow ards the exponential distribution th at is characteristic of Brow nian w alks (figure 4). For example, the CCDF of an intended Lévy w alk w ith exponent /¿intended = 2 results in a realized CCDF that, ow ing to the ter­ m ination of steps b y encounters w ith the target object, resem bles the CCDF of a Lévy w alk w ith a larger exponent /¿realized (figure 4fl). In m ore general term s, an intended m ove­ m ent strategy that is n o t Brow nian at all takes on the signature of Brow nian m otion w hen intended m ovem ent steps are frequently term inated because of a high density of target objects (figure 4b). Proc. /?. Soc. B 281; 20132605 0 .2 - Einstein dem onstrated that Brownian m otion of dissolved particles can b e explained b y heat-driven collisions of these particles w ith the molecules of the liquid [15,16]. Despite obvious differences betw een m ovem ent in particles an d organ­ isms, o u r study show s that in analogy to physics, encounters betw een organism s result in Brownian m otion, in particular w h en found in encounter-rich environm ents. We observed that u n d er controlled, experim ental conditions, m ussel m ove­ m ent patterns shifted from Lévy to Brownian m otion w ith increasing m ussel density. By separating truncated from non-truncated steps, w e w ere able to show that this change in m ovem ent pattern is entirely the consequence of increased encounter rate, as w e d id n o t observe a shift in intrinsic m ovem ent strategy. We furtherm ore dem onstrated the u n i­ versality of this principle w ith a sim ple argum ent, show ing that in general, encounters lead to Brownian m otion in anim al m ovem ent patterns. The shift from Lévy-like to Brownian m ovem ent w ith increasing density has so far been explained as an adaptation to increased resource availability. Anim als are considered to ad a p t to increased encounters w ith food item s b y refraining from large-scale m ovem ent steps, hence leading to adaptive Brownian w alks [12,31]. H ow ever, o ur study provides a differ­ ent perspective on the observed shift from Lévy-like to Brownian movement. W hen encounter rates are low, the observed m ovem ent p attern reflects the intrinsic search strat­ egy, w hich can strongly deviate from Brownian movement. W hen encounter rates are high, the signature of the intrinsic search strategy is lost; large m ovem ent steps are frequently truncated b y encounters an d the m ovem ent p attern resembles Brownian m otion irrespective of the underlying intrinsic strat­ egy. This has im portant im plications for ecological theory, as here Brownian m otion is n o t a default, intrinsic m ovem ent m ode that underlies anim al dispersal b u t em erges from eco­ logical encounters betw een organism s, such as encounters w ith food item s or interference w ith conspecifics, sim ilar to the physical obstruction of m ussel m ovem ent observed in o u r study. The explanation of encounters driving Brownian m otion can clarify observations from a num ber of terrestrial an d marine studies. For instance, studies b y Bartum eus et al. [8], De Knegt et al. [9] an d H um phries et al. [10-11] illustrate that microzooplankton, goats, m arine predators an d albatrosses all exhibit Brownian m otion in areas w ith high food density an d Lévylike m ovem ent in resource-poor environm ents. These studies highlight that an increased prevalence of Brownian m otion in resource-rich environm ents is a general trend in ecological systems. O ur explanation that encounters obscure innate m ove­ m ent strategy into an observed m ovem ent pattern that closely resembles a Brownian w alk rationalizes this universal trend. As a variety of ecological encounters, such as p red a to r-p rey interactions, m ating or aggregation, are prone to occur in real ecosystems, observed anim al m ovem ent patterns w ill always deviate from the em ployed intrinsic m ovem ent strategy. Especially in rich environm ents, resource encounters m ay alter the m ovem ent p attern extensively. Hence, o u r study n ot only illustrates the generality of this principle, b u t also high­ lights the im portance of ecological interactions in shaping m ovem ent patterns of organism s throughout nature. W hile density-dependence of dem ographic processes, such as grow th an d predation, form s the cornerstone of B rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org l.O -i* . « * 0 -8 - . Downloaded from rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org on January 28, 2014 (b) 5.0 n 4.5 Brownian walk 4 .0 •o 3 .5 - s 10-2 " & Q Ü 1 0 -3 Ü _N 3.0 — 2.5 2.0 5 10 50 500 Levy walk - 1.5 2.0 step length 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 ¿'intended Figure 4. Difference b etw een in tend ed and realized step len gth distribution for various den sities o f th e target object, (a) CCDFs o f th e realized step len gth s o f organism s using a LW w ith scaling exp on en t /¿¡„tended = 2 as their intrinsic m o vem en t strategy. Only at zero d en sity, th e realized CCDF corresponds to th e in tend ed CCDF, w h ile th e fatn ess o f th e tail o f th e distribution strongly decreases at higher den sities. The realized CCDF approxim ately correspond to th e CCDF o f a pow er law w ith scaling exp on en t /¿realized = 2.5 , 2.9, 3.0 and 3.5 for th e increasing den sities, respectively. (6) Relationship b etw een intrinsic scaling exp on en t /¿¡„tended and realized scaling exp on en t /¿reanzed for various ob ject den sities. M ovem ent patterns are often classified as a LW w h en th e estim a ted value o f /¿ is b etw een 1 and 3 and as BW w h en /¿ > 3. ecological theory, anim al m ovem ent an d dispersal are ty p i­ cally approxim ated by density-independent linear diffusion, b ased on the assum ption of Brow nian m otion. This study, in com bination w ith previous w ork [7-11,18,23] show s that for m any organism s, this assum ption is n ot valid; b o th m ove­ m ent rates an d m ovem ent characteristics m ay change as a function of the local density of food item s or conspecifics, being either through ecological encounters as advocated in this paper, or through adaptation of m ovem ent [10]. As a consequence, m ovem ent characteristics at the population level m ay change w ith density, for instance from superdiffusive dispersal at low encounter rates, to m ore conservative linear diffusion at h igh encounter rates. This can have im por­ tan t consequences for, for instance, the rate of spread of infectious diseases an d invasive species or the form ation of self-organized patterns. As the underlying m ovem ent strat­ egy w ill often be m asked u n d er high-density conditions an d organism s thus m ight behave differently u n d er low- density conditions, one m ust b e careful n o t to draw too farreaching conclusions from m ovem ent p attern s observed in dense environm ents. A m ore m echanistic und erstan d in g of ecological m ovem ent, facilitated by current im provem ents in techniques to m onitor m oving anim als, w ill greatly expand our ability to examine, m odel an d com prehend anim al m ovem ent pattern s an d their influence on other ecological processes. A c k n o w le d g e m e n ts. W e th a n k V. Jan sen , F. M e y sm an , B. R. S illim an, G. V isw a n a th a n a n d th e a n o n y m o u s referees for th e ir co m m en ts. F or assista n ce, w e th a n k A . V a d e r, J. v a n S oelen a n d H . d e Paoli. F.B. a c k n o w le d g e s th e S p a n ish m in istry of Science a n d In n o v a tio n (REF. 2009-04133, B FU 2010-22337). T his is p u b lic a tio n 5527 of th e N e th e rla n d s In stitu te of Ecology. F u nd in g S ta te m e n t. M .D .J. is s u p p o r te d b y a g ra n t fro m th e N e th e rla n d s O rg a n isa tio n for Scientific R esearch— E a rth a n d Life Sciences (N W O -A LW ), a n d Project G ro u p M o v e m e n t E cology b y th e K N A W S trategic F und. References 1. S k ellam JG. 1951 R an d o m d isp e rsa l in th e o re tic a l 6. 10 9 3 /b io m e t/3 8 .1 -2.1 96 ) 2. 3. N o let BA, M ooij W M . 2 0 0 2 S earch p a th s o f sw a n s m a rin e p re d a to rs. Nature 465,1 0 6 6 - 1 0 6 9 . (do i:10 . 10 3 8 /n a tu re 0 9 1 16) m o v e m e n t a s a c o rre la te d ra n d o m w alk. Oecologia Eco!. 71, 4 5 1 - 4 6 2 . (d o i:1 0 .1 0 4 6 /j.1 3 6 5 -2 6 5 6 .2 0 0 2 . 56, 2 3 4 - 2 3 8 . (d o i:1 0 .1 0 0 7 /B F 0 03 79 69 5) 006 10 .x ) B e n h a m o u S. 2 0 0 7 H ow m a n y a n im a ls really d o th e 7. H u m p h rie s NE et al. 2 0 1 0 E n v iro n m en tal c o n te x t ex p lain s Levy a n d B row nian m o v e m e n t p a tte rn s o f fo ra g in g on sp atially a u to c o rre la te d tu b e rs . J. Anim. 8. Lévy flig h ts re co rd ed in situ. Proc. Natl Acad. Sei. USA 109, 7 1 6 9 - 7 1 7 4 . (d o i:1 0 .1 0 7 3 /p n a s. to reso urce availab ility in m icro zo o p lan k to n . Proc. Natl Acad. Sei. USA 100, 12 7 7 1 - 1 2 7 75 . (d o i:1 0 .1 0 7 3 / 1121201109) 1 2. p n a s.2 1 3 72 43 10 0) 9. H u m p h rie s NE, W eim erskirch H, Q ueiroz N, S o u th all EJ, Sim s DW. 2 0 1 2 F o raging success o f biological B a rtu m e u s F, P e ters F, Pueyo S, M arrase C, C atalan J. Sim s DW et al. 2 0 0 8 Scaling law s o f m a rin e p re d a to r search b eh a v io u r. Nature 451, 1 1. 200 3 Helical Levy w alks: a d ju stin g search in g s ta tis tic 0 6 -1 7 6 9 .1 ) 1 09 8 - 1 1 0 2 . (d o i:1 0 .1 0 3 8 /n a tu re 0 6 5 1 8) 5. 1 0. K areiva PM , S h ig e sa d a N. 1 98 3 A nalyzing insect Levy w alk? Ecology 88, 1 9 6 2 - 1 9 6 9 . (d o i:1 0 .1 8 9 0 / 4. K lafter J, Sokolov IM. 2 00 5 A n o m alo u s diffusion s p re a d s its w in g s. Phys. World 18, 29. p o p u latio n s. Biometrika 38, 1 9 6 - 2 1 8 . (do i:10 . Bell W J. 1991 Searching behaviour, the behavioural ecology o f finding resources. L o nd on , UK: C h ap m an De K negt HJ, H en gev eld GM, v an L a n g ev eld e F, a n d Haii. E d w ard s AM , F reem an MP, B reed GA, Jo n se n ID. d e B oer WF, K irkm an KP. 2 0 0 7 Patch 1 3. 2 0 1 2 Incorrect lik elih oo d m e th o d s w e re u se d to d e n sity d e te rm in e s m o v e m e n t p a tte rn s PJ, S o uza TA, V isw a n a th a n GM . 2011 H ow R aposo EP, B u rtu m eu s F, d a Luz MGE, R ibeiro-N eto infer scalin g law s o f m a rin e p re d a to r search a n d fo ra g in g efficiency o f larg e h erb iv ores. la n d sc a p e h e te ro g e n e ity fra m e s o p tim a l diffusivity b eh a v io u r. PLoS ONE 7, e 4 5 1 7 4 . (d o i:1 0 .1 3 7 1 / Behav. Eco!. 80, 1 0 6 5 - 1 0 7 2 . (d o i:1 0 .1 0 9 3 / in search in g p ro cesses. Comput. Biol. 7, e1 0 0 2 2 3 3 . jo u rn a l.p o n e .0 0 4 5 1 7 4 ) b e h e c o /a rm 0 8 0 ) (d o i:1 0 .1 3 7 1 /jo u rn a l.p c b i.1 0 0 2 2 3 3 ) Proc. R. Soc. B 281: 20132605 1 rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org 0 kg m — 1.3 kg nr 2.0 kg nr 3.3 kg nr 5.2 kg nr Downloaded from rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org on January 28, 2014 14. ra te in biological in tera c tio n s: Lévy v ersu s B row nian c o m m e n t o n 'Lévy w alks evo lv e th ro u g h s tra te g ie s . Phys. Rev. Lett. 88, 0 9 7 9 0 1 . (d o i:1 0 .1 1 0 3 / in tera c tio n b e tw e e n m o v e m e n t a n d e n v iro n m e n ta l P h y sR evL ett.8 8.09 79 01 ) c o m p lex ity '. Science 335, 9 1 8 . (d o i:1 0 .1 1 2 6 /sc ie n c e . Einstein A. 1 90 5 On th e m o v e m e n t o f sm all 1 21 59 03 ) 21. C lau set A, Shalizi CR, N ew m an ME. 2 0 0 9 P o w e r-la w d istrib u tio n s in em p irical d a ta . SIAM Rev. 51, 6 6 1 - 7 0 3 . (d o i:1 0 .1 1 3 7 /0 7 0 7 1 0 1 1 1 ) 2 7. B a rtu m e u s F, d a Luz MGE, V isw a n a th a n GM, C atalan th e m o le c u la r-k in e tic th e o ry o f h e a t. Ann. Phys. 17, m u sse ls o n rocky sho res: in te g ra tin g p ro cess to ra n d o m -w a lk analysis. Ecology 86, 3 0 7 8 - 3 0 8 7 . 5 4 9 - 5 6 0 . (d o i:1 0 .1 0 0 2 /a n d p .l9 0 5 3 2 2 0 8 0 6 ) u n d e rs ta n d p a tte rn . Ecology 82, 3 2 1 3 - 3 2 3 1 . L an gev in P. 1 90 8 On the theory o f Brownian motion. (do i:10 .1 8 9 0 /0 0 1 2 -9 6 5 8 (2 0 0 1 )0 8 2 [3 2 1 3 :P D O M O R ]2 . G iuggioli L, P o tts JR, H arris S. 2 0 1 2 P redicting 22. B ertran d S, B ertran d A, G uevara-C arrasco R, G erlotto O.CO;2) F. 2 0 0 7 S c a le -in v a ria n t m o v e m e n ts o f fish erm e n : H u nt HL, S ch eiblin g RE. 2 0 0 2 M o v em e n t a n d w ave th e s a m e fo rag in g stra te g y as n atu ra l p re d a to rs. o scillatory d y n am ic s in t h e m o v e m e n t o f territo ria l d is lo d g m e n t o f m u sse ls o n a w a v e -e x p o se d rocky a n im a ls. J. R. Soc. Interface 9, 1 5 2 9 - 1 5 4 3 . (do i:10 . sh o re . Veliger 45, 273 - 277 . 23. (d o i:1 0 .1 8 9 0 /0 4 -1 8 0 6 ) 2 8. Eco!. Appl. 17, 3 3 1 - 3 3 7 . (d o i:1 0 .1 8 9 0 /0 6 -0 3 0 3 ) 2 9. E d w ard s AM. 2 0 0 8 U sing lik elih oo d to te s t fo r Lévy Van d e K oppel J, G ascoign e JC, T h eraulaz G, flig h t search p a tte rn s a n d fo r g e n e ra l p o w e r-la w d istrib u tio n s in n a tu re . J. Anim. Eco!. 77, 1212 — De J a g e r M, W eissing FJ, F lerm an PM J, N olet BA, R ietkerk M, M ooij W M , F lerm an PM J. 2 0 0 8 v an d e n K oppel J. 2011 Lévy w a lk s evo lv e th ro u g h E x p erim en tal e v id e n c e fo r sp a tia l self-o rg a n iz a tio n in tera c tio n b e tw e e n m o v e m e n t a n d e n v iro n m e n ta l a n d its e m e rg e n t effe c ts in m u ssel b ed eco sy stem s. com p lex ity . Science 332,1 5 5 1 - 1 5 5 3 . (d o i:1 0 .1 1 2 6 / Science 322, 7 3 9 - 7 4 2 . (d o i:1 0 .1 1 2 6 /sc ie n c e . MGE, R aposo EP, S ta n ley FIE. 1 9 9 9 O p tim izing th e scien ce.1 2 0 1 1 8 7 ) 1 16 39 52 ) success o f ra n d o m s ea rc h e s. Nature 401, 9 1 1 - 9 1 4 . J a n se n VAA, M a sh an o v a A, Petrovskii S. 2 01 2 24. C o m m en t o n 'Lévy w alk s evo lv e th ro u g h in teractio n b e tw e e n m o v e m e n t a n d e n v iro n m e n ta l c o m p lex ity '. Science 335, 9 1 8 . (d o i:1 0 .1 1 2 6 /sc ie n c e .1 2 1 5 7 4 7 ) Turchin P. 1 99 8 Quantitative analysis o f movement. S u n d e rla n d , MA: S in a u er A ssociates. 25. 1 22 2. (d o i:1 0 .1 1 1 1 /j .l 3 6 5 -2 6 5 6 .2 0 0 8 .0 1 428.x) 3 0. V isw a n a th a n GM , B uldyrev SV, Havlin S, d a Luz (d o i:1 0 .1 0 3 8 /4 4 8 3 1 ) 3 1. Frank SA. 2 0 0 9 T he c o m m o n p a tte rn s o f n atu re . E d w ard s AM et al. 2 0 0 7 R evisiting Lévy flig h t search J. Evol. Biol. 22, 1 5 6 3 - 1 5 8 5 . (d o i:1 0 .1 1 1 1 /j.1 4 2 0 - p a tte rn s o f w a n d e rin g a lb a tro sse s, b u m b le b e e s a n d 9 1 0 1 .2 0 0 9 .0 1 7 7 5 .x ) Proc. /?. Soc. B 281: 20132605 19. n atu re 0 6 1 9 9 ) 2 6. J. 2 00 5 A nim al search s tra te g ie s: a q u a n tita tiv e 1 0 9 8 /tsif.2 0 1 1.0 79 7) 18. d e e r. Nature 449, 1 0 4 4 - 1 0 4 8 . (d o i:1 0 .1 0 3 8 / H u nt HL, S ch eiblin g RE. 2001 Patch d y n am ic s o f C. R. Acad. Sei. [Paris) 146, 5 3 0 - 5 3 3 . 17. De J a g e r M, W eissing FJ, Flerm an PM J, N o let BA, v an d e K oppel J. 2 0 1 2 R esp o n se to p artic le s s u s p e n d e d in sta tio n a ry liq uids req u ired by 16. 20. rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org 15. B a rtu m e u s F, C atalan J, Fulco UL, Lyra ML, V isw a n a th a n GM. 2 0 0 2 O p tim izing th e e n c o u n te r