1 1 2 Comparative behavior of red lionfish (Pterois volitans) on native Pacific vs. invaded Atlantic coral reefs 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Running head: Native vs. invasive lionfish behavior Katherine Cure1,2,*, Cassandra E. Benkwitt3, Tye L. Kindinger3, Emily A. Pickering3, Timothy J. Pusack3, Jennifer L. McIlwain1,4, Mark A. Hixon3 1 The Marine Laboratory, University of Guam, Mangilao, Guam 96923, USA 2 Present address: School of Plant Biology, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia 3 Department of Zoology, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR 97331-2914, USA 4 Department of Environment and Agriculture, Curtin University, Perth, WA 6845, Australia 15 16 *katherine.cure@gmail.com 17 ABSTRACT: Pacific red lionfish (Pterois volitans) have invaded Atlantic reefs and reached 18 much greater population densities than on native reefs. We hypothesized that lionfish on invaded 19 reefs would (1) experience higher kill rates and thus spend less time hunting, given the naïveté of 20 Atlantic prey, (2) consume a greater variety of prey, given the lack of native prey defenses, and 21 (3) display less pronounced crepuscular patterns of hunting, given the ease of capturing Atlantic 22 prey. Comparative behavioral observations were conducted in two native regions (Philippines 23 and Guam) and two invaded regions (Cayman Islands and Bahamas) to assess lionfish time 24 budgets and diurnal activity patterns, and to explore correlations between environmental 25 variables and lionfish behavior. Contrary to our first hypothesis, total time allocated to hunting 26 and kill rates showed no difference between native and invaded reefs, despite considerable 27 regional variation. However, Atlantic prey of lionfish were twice as large as Pacific prey, 28 suggesting that despite similar hunting behavior, invasive lionfish are receiving greater 29 nutritional input. Furthermore, consistent with our second hypothesis, lionfish on invaded reefs 2 30 had broader diets, and also relied less on "blowing" behavior for prey capture, pointing to 31 substantial prey naïveté in the invaded range. Importantly, only in the invaded range did we 32 observe lionfish consuming parrotfishes, the decline of which could have indirect effects on 33 interactions between seaweeds and corals. Finally, lionfish overall tended to exhibit a 34 crepuscular pattern in behavior whereby hunting peaked at sunrise and/or sunset, with no 35 differences attributable to native vs. invasive status. 36 37 KEY WORDS: Native vs. invasive behavior; Diurnal hunting pattern; Crepuscular hunting; 38 Time budget 39 40 INTRODUCTION 41 In the mid 1980´s Pacific red lionfish (Pterois volitans) invaded the Western Atlantic via the 42 aquarium trade (Semmens et al. 2004), and beginning in the early 2000´s extended their range 43 throughout the Caribbean, Gulf of Mexico, and down the South American coastline (Schofield 44 2010). Despite local attempts at removal, invasive lionfish reached local population densities far 45 greater than in their native Pacific (Whitfield et al. 2007, Green & Côté 2008, Kulbicki et al. 46 2012). At invaded locations, lionfish consume a broad diversity of small fishes and crustaceans 47 including juveniles of herbivores (Morris & Akins 2009) and have caused substantial reductions 48 in the recruitment and abundance of reef fishes (Albins & Hixon 2008, Green et al. 2012). 49 Invasive lionfish can also negatively affect native piscivorous predators through both predation 50 on juveniles and competition with adults (Albins 2012). Thus, lionfish may ultimately cause 51 drastic changes in Atlantic coral-reef ecosystems (Albins & Hixon 2011). 3 52 Lionfish are likely protected from predators by their cryptic coloration and numerous 53 venomous spines (Allen & Eschemeyer 1973). Predators rarely target adult lionfish at either 54 native or invaded locations, despite anecdotal evidence of occasional predation (Bernadsky & 55 Goulet 1991, Maljkovic & Van Leeuwen 2008). Because natural controls have not been 56 definitively identified at either the native Pacific or invaded Atlantic, direct removals of lionfish 57 by humans have to date been the only effective way to reduce invasive populations. Because 58 invasive lionfish are widespread geographically and occur far deeper than usual SCUBA depths 59 (Whitfield et al. 2007, Lesser & Slattery 2011), complete eradication is unlikely. 60 Before the invasion, little was known about the ecology and behavior of lionfish, due in part 61 to their rarity in their native range (Kulbicki et al. 2012). Lionfish use two types of hunting 62 methods: slow stalking of prey aided by fanlike pectoral fins that herd prey, and sit-and-wait 63 ambush (Randall 2005). Blowing behavior, by which lionfish produce jets of water directed at 64 prey while approaching them, enhances predatory efficiency by confusing or distracting prey 65 (Albins & Lyons 2012). Lionfish diets are well documented in the invasive range and include a 66 broad variety of coral reef fishes, crustaceans (Morris & Akins 2009, Côté & Maljkovic 2010, 67 Green et al. 2012), and even conspecifics (Valdez-Moreno et al. 2012). These records coincide 68 with diet reported from qualitative descriptions in the native range (Myers 1999). Hunting takes 69 place during crepuscular periods (Myers 1999, Randall 2005) when low light levels impede 70 visually adapted prey (Helfman 1986). During the daytime, native lionfish become inactive in 71 holes and crevices (Fishelson 1997), a pattern common to a variety of other predatory fishes and 72 often influenced by ambient light levels (Belovsky & Slade 1986). Invasive lionfish in the 73 Bahamas also display this crepuscular pattern of hunting behavior (Green et al. 2011). 4 74 Behavioral comparisons between populations in native vs. invaded ranges are highly 75 informative because they may identify factors that foster invasion success (Holway & Suarez 76 1999, Morris & Whitfield 2009, Meyer & Dierking 2011), and may help in developing control 77 measures (Guo 2006). Changes in diel activity of fishes are a common response to differences in 78 biotic and abiotic environmental factors, including predator abundance, prey availability, 79 presence/absence of competitors, habitat structure, depth, and abiotic conditions such as 80 temperature and light levels (Chen et al. 1999, Reebs 2002, Hansen et al. 2004, Andrews et al. 81 2009, Côté & Maljkovic 2010). 82 invasion is often accompanied by release from the natural controls of competition and predation 83 (Mack et al. 2000), behavioral differences in invasive species are therefore likely between native 84 and invaded locations. For invasive predators, such differences can mean access to more 85 abundant or higher quality prey (Meyer & Dierking 2011), enhanced by the substantial 86 advantages novel invasive predators usually have over native naïve prey (Cox & Lima 2006). Because these factors may vary regionally, and because 87 We conducted replicate comparative field observations in two regions in the native Pacific 88 Ocean and two regions in the invaded Atlantic Ocean in an effort to determine whether there are 89 any inter-ocean differences in lionfish behavior. Assuming native Atlantic prey are naïve to 90 invasive lionfish, we hypothesized that lionfish would have higher success at killing prey at 91 invaded regions. If so, we further hypothesized that higher success at killing prey would result in 92 concomitant changes to predatory behavior in the invaded Atlantic, including less time spent 93 hunting as lionfish satiated more rapidly, and hunting being less restricted to the low-light levels 94 of crepuscular times often exploited by native predators (Helfman 1986). We also predicted that, 95 as an efficient and voracious generalist predator, lionfish would consume a greater variety of 96 prey in their invaded Atlantic range compared to their native Pacific range. 5 97 98 MATERIALS AND METHODS 99 Data collection 100 We observed in situ behavior of lionfish in two regions within each ocean: the Philippines 101 and Guam in the native range, and the Cayman Islands and the Bahamas in the invaded range 102 (Fig. 1). Invasive lionfish were first detected in the Bahamas in 2004 and in the Caymans in 2008 103 (Schofield 2009). Sampling was conducted over a 3-year period, mostly during June to 104 September 2009-2011. We selected sites known to have lionfish, including sandy slopes with 105 coral patches, continuous reef walls, and a few artificial habitats (e.g., piers, small wrecks, tire 106 reefs, and old fish traps). Maximum depth at these sites was 25 m. 107 The number of sites sampled depended on lionfish frequency encountered at those sites. In 108 the native range, where the probability of encountering lionfish was low, 20 sites were sampled 109 in the Philippines and 13 on Guam. In the invasive range, 5 sites were sampled in the Bahamas 110 and 4 in the Cayman Islands. 111 Because capturing and tagging lionfish observed in this study was logistically impractical, 112 and because tagging can possibly alter lionfish behavior towards the observer (personal 113 observation), we chose to observe untagged animals. At each site during any given time of day, 114 we ensured that different animals were sampled by swimming over the site unidirectionally and 115 sampling lionfish encountered haphazardly along the way. Each site was sampled only once 116 within a day, and if we returned to that same site on another day, we sampled different parts of 117 the site and different time periods. Therefore, we assumed that our observations were statistically 118 independent and representative, even when we had no way of knowing with certainty whether 119 we resampled the same fish between days. 6 120 In each region, we conducted a series of standardized ten-minute observations taken from 121 sunrise to sunset (between ~0600 and ~1830 h). During each period, lionfish behavior was 122 recorded by trained observers using either SCUBA or snorkel. There were 5 observers in the 123 native range and 8 in the invaded range (1 observer in the Pacific and 2 observers in the Atlantic 124 completed 60% of the total observations). Care was taken to minimize the influence of observer 125 presence on lionfish behavior by keeping a distance of approximately 3 m from each fish. We 126 attempted to sample equally all times of day, sites, habitats and environmental conditions, 127 subject to logistic constraints. 128 Eight lionfish behaviors were quantified, following an initial ethogram constructed from a 129 pilot study in the Bahamas. Behaviors were quantified as either proportion of time (i.e. 130 proportion of each 10 minute observation period) or counts (i.e. number of events per each 10 131 minute observation period). Proportion of time was recorded for each of four activities: inactive, 132 minimal activity, active, and hunting. The first three range from lionfish being stationary, to 133 short distance movements, to long distance movements, respectively, but in all instances pectoral 134 fins are relaxed (i.e. not flared and in position for hunting). Hunting activity was obvious as 135 lionfish focused on particular prey with fully flared pectoral fins (Green et al. 2011). Counts 136 were recorded for aggressive interactions (chasing other lionfish or other fish species), strikes 137 (successful and unsuccessful attacks on potential prey), kills (successful capture of prey, i.e. prey 138 consumed), and blows (water current directed at prey). During each observation period, we 139 identified all prey approached by lionfish to at least the family level as well as the species level 140 where possible, and estimated prey body size as total length (TL) to the nearest cm. 141 For each 10-min observation we also measured five environmental variables known to 142 influence fish behavior. (i) We recorded the microhabitat within which each observation 7 143 occurred, mostly hard coral, rock-boulder/cave and sand/rubble, and less frequently seagrass 144 beds, sponge fields, soft coral fields, and artificial structures. Lionfish usually did not move 145 outside of the identified microhabitat during the observation time. In the few cases where such 146 movement did occur, microhabitat was classified as the area where lionfish spent most of the 147 observation time. During each sample period, we also recorded (ii) cloud cover (clear: 0 to 25%, 148 partly cloudy: 25 to 75%, overcast: >75%), (iii) current (low: diver barely kicking to maintain 149 position, medium: periodic kicking required by diver to maintain position, high: constant kicking 150 by diver required to maintain position), (iv) estimated lionfish size (TL) and (v) depth. 151 Temperature was measured in situ using HOBO® temperature loggers every 30 min in Guam 152 and the Bahamas, while temperature data for the Philippines and the Cayman Islands were 153 recorded 154 (http://coralreefwatch.noaa.gov/satellite/current/products_vs.html). 155 Statistical analyses from NOAA virtual stations 156 Frequency distributions for each of the quantified behaviors were highly skewed and had 157 high proportions of ones and zeros, so data transformation did not result in either normality or 158 homoscedasticity. The two most common behaviors (inactive and hunting), were therefore 159 analyzed with a logistic regression using a generalized linear mixed-effects model (GLMM), in 160 which region (random effect) was nested within ocean (fixed effect). The model was robust to 161 the skewed nature of the data and allowed us to explore the cumulative effects of putative 162 explanatory variables. Cumulative effects of ocean, time period, habitat, cloud cover, current, 163 lionfish size (TL), and depth on lionfish behavior, were assessed. 164 Despite the reduction in detail from the conversion of proportions to binary data, a logistic 165 regression model was chosen in part because a lionfish that is exhibiting inactivity or hunting is 8 166 doing so exclusively (i.e., if a lionfish is inactive, it cannot hunt, and vice versa). The behaviors 167 categorized as “minimal activity” and “active” (both not involving hunting) accounted for less 168 than 10% of the total time budgets for all regions, so these were excluded from further analysis, 169 as well as rates of aggressive behavior toward conspecifics, which were very low in all regions. 170 Data for y were binary variables created for lionfish behavior (0 = inactive and 1 = hunting). 171 An observation was considered as inactive if >50% of the ten minute period was spent inactive, 172 and as hunting if >50% of the ten minutes were spent hunting; 75% of all observation periods 173 were dominated by one behavior or the other (i.e. either hunting or inactive represented >80% of 174 the observation period). All logistic regressions were done in R (R Development Core Team 175 2010) using the package “lme4” (Bates et al. 2011) and following the guidelines of Rossiter & 176 Loza (2010) and Peng et al. (2002). Model fit was assessed by examination of model residuals, 177 predicted outcomes, likelihood ratio tests and chi-square statistics (Quinn & Keough 2002). 178 Validation of the model by comparing predicted probabilities to observed outcomes was also 179 performed (70% of outcomes were correctly predicted by the model). 180 Partitioning of variance to determine the relative importance of each explanatory variable in 181 the model was calculated using the R package “hier.part” (Walsh & MacNally 2008). 182 Hierarchical partitioning is a technique that, rather than seeking a best fit, uses all possible 183 models in a regression hierarchy to distinguish variables that have the highest independent 184 correlations with the response variable; these variables are most likely to influence variation 185 (MacNally 1996). 186 One-way univariate permutational analyses of variance (PERMANOVA, Anderson et al. 187 2008) were used to compare strike and kill rate means at the level of ocean vs. ocean, with region 188 nested within ocean. These were chosen because of their robustness to deviations from normality 9 189 and homoscedasticity, characteristic of our data. PERMANOVAs were run for 9999 190 permutations in PRIMER 6 (PRIMER-E Ltd., 2009) with the following specifications: 191 Euclidean distance, sequential sums of squares, and permutation of residuals under the reduced 192 model (Anderson et al. 2008). 193 194 RESULTS 195 We observed lionfish ranging in size from 5 to 35 cm TL during a total of 192 hours of time 196 budgeting at native reefs (Philippines 37 hr, Guam 28 hr) and invaded reefs (Cayman Islands 73 197 hr, Bahamas 54 hr). Variation in lionfish time budgets was not substantial between oceans, but 198 considerable between regions within each ocean (Fig. 2, Table 1). Lionfish were usually more 199 active in the Philippines (native) and the Cayman Islands (invaded), and more sedentary in Guam 200 (native) and the Bahamas (invaded). In all regions, we never observed predators attacking 201 lionfish. Temperature was similar both between oceans (pooling regional means ±SE: Pacific = 202 28.13 ± 0.02 °C, Atlantic = 28.69 ± 0.01 °C) and between regions within oceans (mean ± SE: 203 Philippines = 30.23 ± 0.04 °C, Guam = 28.12 ± 0.01 °C, Cayman Islands = 29.72 ± 0.06 °C, 204 Bahamas = 28.68 ± 0.02 °C). 205 Hunting behavior and prey consumed 206 Lionfish hunting behavior contradicted a priori expectations of inter-ocean differences. Time 207 spent hunting throughout the day was equal between the Atlantic and the Pacific (Table 1), 208 although maximum hunting time was nonetheless observed in the Pacific (Philippines = 70% 209 hunting) and minimum hunting time in the Atlantic (Bahamas = 17.6% hunting) (Fig. 2). Diurnal 210 patterns of lionfish behavior showed that, irrespective of native vs. invaded range, hunting was 211 greater and inactivity lower during sunrise and/or sunset (Fig. 3, Table 1). A strongly crepuscular 10 212 hunting pattern was most evident for lionfish in Guam and the Bahamas, while lionfish hunting 213 in the Philippines and the Cayman Islands peaked at sunrise (Fig. 3, A & C). 214 Also contradicting expectations, both strike and kill rates were similar between oceans 215 (Strikes: Pseudo-F = 0.051, df = 1,1146, p = 1; Kills: Pseudo-F = 0.081, df = 1,1146, p = 0.834) 216 (Fig. 4A), although there was considerable regional variation. Successful kill rates (% of total 217 strikes that resulted in prey being consumed = [number of kills / number of strikes]*100) were 218 highest in the Bahamas (51.2%), followed by Philippines (50.0%), the Cayman Islands (25.6%), 219 and Guam (22.9%) (Fig. 4A). 220 Despite lionfish time budgets and kill rates varying more between regions than between 221 oceans, there were several clear differences in hunting behavior between the native Pacific and 222 invaded Atlantic ranges. First, mean blowing rates were three times lower in the invaded range 223 vs. the native range (Fig. 4B). Second, mean prey size was nearly double in the invaded vs. the 224 native range (mean SEM: Atlantic = 2.45 0.42 cm, Pacific = 1.50 0.35 cm). Third, 225 observed diets (kill rates) were broader in the invaded range than in the native range (total # of 226 prey taxa killed/1000 min: Atlantic = 1.6, Pacific = 0.9), even though strikes targeted a greater 227 diversity of fishes in the native Pacific (total # of prey taxa targeted/1000 min: Atlantic = 2.5, 228 Pacific = 3.8). Lionfish successfully killed prey in 6 fish families in the invaded range 229 (Apogonidae, Blenniidae, Gobiidae, Labridae, Pomacentridae and Scaridae), vs. only 2 in the 230 native range (Pomacentridae and Trichonotidae) (Table 2). Strikes were mostly towards gobies 231 (Gobiidae), wrasses (Labridae) and parrotfishes (Scaridae) in the invaded range, and towards 232 cardinalfishes (Apogonidae), gobies, and marine catfishes (Plotosidae) in the native Pacific. 233 Successful kills in the Atlantic included ecologically important species such as juvenile 234 parrotfishes, which were not targeted by lionfish in the Pacific. These differences in diet breadth 11 235 occurred despite the fact that there are far more potential prey fish species in the native range 236 compared to the invaded range of lionfish (Roberts et al. 2002). 237 Lionfish size 238 Overall, body size had no statistical effect on lionfish behavior (Table 1). However, the 239 coefficient estimate for size was negative, suggesting that smaller lionfish spent more time 240 hunting (β = -0.014, p = 0.102, Table 1). Furthermore, when individual regions were analyzed 241 separately, smaller lionfish (5 to 15 cm TL) in the Philippines and the Cayman Islands spent 242 more time hunting than larger fish (Spearman Rank Correlations: ῤ = -0.145, p = 0.040 for the 243 Philippines; ῤ = -0.131, p = 0.036 for the Cayman Islands). Lionfish size distributions differed 244 across regions, with larger lionfish found in Guam and the Bahamas (mean ± SD: Philippines = 245 17.69 ± 6.15 cm TL, Guam = 22.08 ± 7.22 cm TL, Cayman Islands = 15.63 ± 5.57 cm TL, 246 Bahamas = 22.66 ± 5.44 cm TL; Kruskal-Wallis H = 219.191, df = 3, 1146, p < 0.001). 247 Environmental effects 248 Between-ocean comparisons revealed that native Pacific and invasive Atlantic lionfish 249 responded similarly to measured environmental factors (Table 1). Hunting activity was greatest 250 when overcast (Table 1), and this response to changes in cloud cover was most evident for 251 lionfish on Guam and the Bahamas (Fig. 5A). Together with the Caymans, these two regions 252 were sampled at shallower depths (mean ± SEM: Philippines = 19.3 ± 0.3 m, Guam = 10.9 ± 0.4 253 m, Cayman Islands = 9.4 ± 0.3 m, Bahamas = 3.1 ± 0.1 m; Kruskal-Wallis H = 925.1, df = 3, 254 1146, p < 0.001), where the effects of cloud cover on ambient light levels were most obvious to 255 the observers. High currents had a consistent effect across all regions, resulting in less time spent 256 hunting and greater inactivity when compared to both low and medium currents (Table 1), 257 especially on Guam (Fig. 5B). Habitat had only slightly significant effects in both inactivity and 12 258 hunting patterns (Table 1, Fig. 5C). However, lionfish in rock-boulder habitats tended to hunt 259 less and be more inactive, in contrast to lionfish over hard coral and sand/ rubble, where hunting 260 peaked, especially in the Atlantic (Fig. 5C). 261 When all variables were examined simultaneously, hierarchical partitioning analyses 262 indicated that time-of-day, followed by depth and habitat, were most important in explaining 263 variation in lionfish behavior (Fig. 6). Although coefficient estimates for depth in the logistic 264 model were not significant, depth accounted for almost 20% of total model deviance (Fig. 6) and 265 coefficient estimates indicated a trend towards greater time spent hunting with increasing depth 266 (ß = 0.028, p = 0.499) (Table 1). 267 268 DISCUSSION 269 Despite initial expectations of interoceanic differences in lionfish behavior related to 270 native vs. invasive status, our field observations from two regions in each of two oceans suggest 271 that overall patterns in lionfish time budgets, daily activity patterns, and success rates at killing 272 prey are similar in the native Pacific and the invaded Atlantic. These results show that if ease of 273 prey capture is higher for invasive lionfish due to naïve prey, this is not reflected in either kill 274 rates, hunting time or crepuscular hunting patterns. 275 Instead, lionfish behavioral patterns are determined more by regional differences in a 276 suite of environmental factors, which act synergistically to affect behavior. The most pronounced 277 of these factors was the low light level associated with crepuscular times, during which time 278 spent hunting was maximal, regardless of ocean of residence. Twilight foraging is a feature 279 common among coral-reef piscivores, and is probably related to the advantages that low light 280 levels confer to predators vs. their prey (Helfman 1986). The largely crepuscular hunting pattern 13 281 found, despite peaks occurring in either sunrise and sunset (Guam and Bahamas) or sunrise alone 282 (Philippines and Caymans), confirms earlier descriptions of lionfish in both their native range 283 (Fishelson 1975, Myers 1999) and invaded range (Green et al. 2011). 284 Between-ocean differences 285 While lionfish time budgets and kill rates did not vary appreciably between oceans, there 286 were nonetheless differences in diet breadth, prey size and use of blowing behavior between the 287 native and invaded ranges. Although coral-reef fishes are far more diverse in the Pacific than in 288 the Atlantic (Roberts et al. 2002), we observed invasive lionfish successfully consuming a 289 broader diversity of fishes in the Atlantic than native lionfish in the Pacific. Diet was broader in 290 the Atlantic despite the fact that lionfish strikes were directed at a greater diversity of prey in the 291 Pacific. Atlantic prey species composition was similar to that previously observed in Bahamian 292 lionfish (Albins & Hixon 2008, Morris & Akins 2009). We observed only invasive lionfish 293 consuming parrotfishes (Scaridae). Parrotfishes are ecologically important herbivores that help to 294 keep seaweeds from overgrowing corals (Mumby 2006). Declines in such herbivores in the 295 invaded range could have severe indirect effects on Atlantic coral reefs (Albins and Hixon 2011). 296 There are at least three possible (not mutually exclusive) explanations for increased diet 297 breadth in invasive relative to native lionfish. The first is based on optimal foraging theory, 298 which predicts that diet breadth is determined by the encounter rate of preferred prey (Stephens 299 & Krebs 1986). It is possible that crustaceans and juvenile pomacentrids are preferred prey in the 300 Pacific and are present in sufficiently high densities that diet breadth is reduced in native Pacific 301 lionfish. The higher availability of fish recruits during the dates we sampled in the Philippines 302 (Abesamis & Russ 2010) compared to other regions is consistent with this hypothesis, yet the 14 303 fact that lionfish targeted a greater diversity of prey in the Pacific but successfully killed a lower 304 diversity of prey, tends to falsify this hypothesis. 305 A second explanation is that prey are naïve to lionfish as a novel predator in the newly 306 invaded Atlantic, allowing generalist lionfish to successfully capture a broader diversity of prey, 307 even when kill rates and hunting times were equal between native and invaded locations. This 308 explanation goes in line with our initial hypothesis that invasive lionfish would consume greater 309 variety of prey given the lack of native prey defenses. As observed for invasive lionfish (Albins 310 & Hixon 2008, Green et al. 2011), native prey may exhibit weak or nonexistent responses to 311 newly introduced predators (Cox & Lima 2006, Smith et al. 2008). Lionfish are new to the 312 Atlantic and do not resemble any native Atlantic predators. Therefore, prey types that normally 313 may not be available to native Pacific lionfish are present in invasive lionfish diet. Our findings 314 of broader targeted species in the native Pacific Ocean, yet greater success at killing more 315 diverse prey in the Atlantic, lends credence to this hypothesis. The prey naïveté explanation is 316 also supported by the larger prey size accessed by invasive lionfish. It is possible that, because 317 prey in the Pacific are more likely to recognize lionfish as predators, only the smallest and/or 318 least mobile species and individuals are vulnerable to predation. Still further evidence of prey 319 naïveté in the invaded range comes from differences in the use of blowing behavior by lionfish. 320 Lionfish employed blowing while stalking prey three times more often in the native Pacific range 321 compared to the invaded Atlantic range. Blowing behavior may confuse prey and facilitate head- 322 first capture as prey face upcurrent (Albins & Lyons 2012). Greater use of this hunting technique 323 may be required in the Pacific because prey recognize lionfish and are more wary. In contrast, 324 because lionfish are new to the Atlantic, they need not employ such secondary hunting methods 325 as frequently to capture naïve Atlantic prey. Given that native prey can adapt to invasive 15 326 predators by rapid evolution of behavioral responses to predator presence (Schlaepfer et al. 2005, 327 Freeman & Byers 2006), we suggest that future research focus on whether invasive lionfish and 328 native prey alter their attack and evasion behavior, respectively, through time. 329 A third explanation for increased diet breadth in invasive relative to native lionfish is the 330 competitive and/or predatory release that often accompanies invasions (Mack et al. 2000). For 331 example, the introduced grouper Cephalopholis argus fed on larger prey as a response to lower 332 competitor densities in non-native vs. native reefs (Meyer & Dierking 2011). A similar pattern 333 could affect lionfish hunting, given the larger prey sizes consumed at invaded reefs and the 334 higher diversity of ecologically similar reef fishes (i.e., potential competitors) in the native 335 Pacific relative to the invaded Atlantic (Roberts et al. 2002). Additionally, Albins (2012) has 336 demonstrated that invasive lionfish compete effectively with native grouper. Regarding release 337 from predation, although venomous spines appear to be an effective prey defense for larger 338 lionfish (Allen & Eschmeyer 1973), we hypothesize that new lionfish recruits (which have 339 flexible spines with less venom) may be the target of co-evolved, specialized, smaller predatory 340 fishes in the Pacific that do not occur in the Atlantic, thereby providing a source of biotic control 341 that is absent in the Atlantic. 342 Conclusions 343 Generally, lionfish are crepuscular predators in both their native Pacific and invaded 344 Atlantic ranges. Invasive lionfish seem to have maintained their native behaviors that generally 345 vary with environmental conditions, and therefore display no major inter-ocean differences in 346 overall activity patterns. Lionfish nonetheless exhibit substantial behavioral and ecological 347 differences between Pacific and Atlantic locations. First, invasive lionfish spend far less time 348 using blowing behavior, perhaps indicating prey naïveté in the Atlantic. Second, invasive 16 349 lionfish have far broader diets (measured by kill rates) despite the fact that (a) native lionfish 350 hunted greater variety of prey, and (b) the Pacific Ocean supports a far greater species diversity 351 of potential prey reef fishes. Importantly, only invasive lionfish were observed consuming 352 ecologically important parrotfishes. Third, Atlantic prey of lionfish are larger, even though prey 353 consumption rates are comparable to native Pacific lionfish, therefore implying that invasive 354 lionfish ingest a greater daily ration in terms of prey biomass than do native lionfish. Overall, it 355 is clear that red lionfish display a substantial capacity for behavioral adaptation to local 356 environmental conditions, likely contributing to their enormous success as an invasive species. 357 358 Acknowledgements: We are immensely grateful to Jason Miller, Dioscoro Inocencio, and Gabe 359 Scheer for help with field work, Kurt Ingeman for reviewing the manuscript, and Mark Albins 360 for general advice. Staffs of the Perry Institute for Marine Science (Bahamas) and the Central 361 Caribbean Marine Institute (Caymans) provided logistic support. Rene Abesamis and Renclar 362 Jadloc assisted with logistics in the Philippines. Statistical advice from Andrew Halford and 363 Alexander Kerr was invaluable for dealing with a complex dataset. This research was funded by 364 U.S. National Science Foundation grants 08-51162 and 12-33027 to MAH, and a UOG 365 Micronesian Area Research Center (MARC) scholarship to KC. 366 367 LITERATURE CITED 368 369 370 371 372 373 Abesamis RA, Russ GR (2010) Patterns of recruitment of coral reef fishes in a monsoonal environment. Coral Reefs 29:911-921 Albins MA, Hixon MA (2008). Invasive Indo-Pacific lionfish (Pterois volitans) reduce recruitment of Atlantic coral-reef fishes. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 367:233-238 17 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 Albins MA, Hixon MA (2011) Worst case scenario: potential long-term effects of invasive predatory lionfish (Pterois volitans) on Atlantic and Caribbean coral-reef communities. Environ Biol Fish DOI: 10.1007/s10641-011-9795-1 415 416 417 418 Fishelson L (1997) Experiments and observations on food consumption, growth and starvation in Dendrochirus brachypterus and Pterois volitans (Pteroinae, Scorpaenidae). Environ Biol Fish 50: 391-403 Albins MA (2012) Effects of invasive Pacific red lionfish Pterois volitans versus a native predator on Bahamian coral-reef fish communities. Biol Inv DOI:10.1007/s10530-012-0266-1 Albins MA, Lyons PJ (2012) Invasive red lionfish Pterois volitans blow directed jets of water at prey fish. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 448:1-5 Allen GR, Eschmeyer WN (1973) Turkeyfishes at Eniwetok. Pac Discov 26:3-11 Andrews KS, Williams GD, Farrer D, Tolimieri N, Harvey CJ, Bargmann G, Levin PS (2009) Diel activity patterns of sixgill sharks, Hexanchus griseus: the ups and downs of an apex predator. Anim Behav 78: 525-536 Anderson MJ, Gorley RN, Clarke RK (2008) PERMANOVA+ for PRIMER: Guide to software and statistical methods. Primer-E Ltd., Plymouth/UK Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B (2011) Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. Package ´lme4´. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Springer, New York Belovsky GE, Slade JB (1986) Time budgets of grassland herbivores: body size similarities. Oecologia 70: 53-62 Bernadsky G, Goulet D (1991) A natural predator of the lionfish, Pterois miles. Copeia 1991(1): 230-231 Chen WM, Purser J, Blyth P (1999) Diel feeding rhythms of greenback flounder Rhombosolea tapirina (Günther 1862): the role of light-dark cycles and food deprivation. Aquac Res 30: 529537 Côté IM, Maljkovic A (2010) Predation rates of Indo-Pacific lionfish on Bahamian coral reefs. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 404: 219-225 Cox JG, Lima SL (2006) Naïveté and an aquatic-terrestrial dichotomy in the effects of introduced predators. TREE 21:674-680 Fishelson L (1975) Ethology and reproduction of pteroid fishes found in the Gulf of Aqaba (Red Sea), especially Dendrochirus brachypterus (Cuvier) (Pteroidae, Teleostei). PSZNI: Mar Ecol 39:635–656 18 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 Freeman AS, Byers JE (2006) Divergent induced responses to an invasive predator in marine mussel populations. Science 313: 831-833 Green SJ, Côté IM (2008) Record densities of Indo-Pacific lionfish on Bahamian coral reefs. Coral Reefs 28: 107 Green SJ, Akins JL, Côté IM (2011) Foraging behavior and prey consumption in the Indo-Pacific lionfish on Bahamian coral reefs. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 433:159-167 Green SJ, Akins JL, Maljković A, Côté IM (2012) Invasive lionfish drive Atlantic coral reef fish declines. PLOS One 7(3): e32596. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0032596 Guo Q (2006) Intercontinental biotic invasions: What can we learn from native populations and habitats? Bio Invasions 8:1451-1459 Hansen EA, David BO, Closs GP (2004) Diel patterns of feeding and prey selection in giant kokopu (Galaxias argenteus). N Z J of Mar Freshw Res 38:341-345 Helfman GS (1986) Fish behaviour by day, night and twilight. In: Pitcher TJ (ed) Behavior of Teleost Fishes. Pp. 479-512. Chapman & Hall, London. Holway DA, Suarez AV (1999) Animal behavior: an essential component of invasion biology. TREE 14(8): 328-330 Kulbicki M, Beets J, Chabanet P, Cure K, Darling E, Floeter SR, Galzin R, Green A, HarmelinVivien M, Hixon M, Letourneur Y, Lison de Loma T, McClanahan T, McIlwain J, MouTham G, Myers R, O´Leary JK, Planes S, Vigliola L, Wantiez L (2012) Distributions of Indo-Pacific lionfishes Pterois spp. In their native ranges: implications for the Atlantic invasion. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 446: 189-205 Lesser MP, Slattery M (2011) Phase shift to algal dominated communities at mesophotic depths associated with lionfish (Pterois volitans) invasion on a Bahamian coral reef. Bio Inv 13: 18551868 Mack RN, Simberloff D, Lonsdale WM, Evans H, Clout M, Bazzaz FA (2000) Biotic invasions: Causes, epidimiology, global consequences, and control. Ecol App 10: 689-710 MacNally, R (1996) Hierarchical partitioning as an interpretative tool in multivariate inference. Aus Ecol 21(3): 224-228 Maljkovic A, Van Leeuwen T (2008) Predation on the invasive red lionfish, Pterois volitans (Pisces: Scorpaenidae), by native groupers in the Bahamas. Coral Reefs 27: 501 Meyer AL and Dierking J (2011) Elevated size and body condition and altered feeding ecology of the grouper Cephalopholis argus in non-native habitats. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 439: 203- 212 19 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 Morris JA, Akins JL (2009) Feeding ecology of invasive lionfish (Pterois volitans) in the Bahamian archipelago. Environ Biol Fish 86:389-398 Morris JA, Whitfield PE (2009) Biology, ecology, control and management of the invasive IndoPacific lionfish: An updated integrated assessment. Tech Mem NOS NCCOS 99. National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration, Washington DC Mumby, P J (2006) The impact of exploiting grazers (Scaridae) on the dynamics of Caribbean coral reefs. Ecol App 16:747-769 Myers R (1999) Micronesian reef fishes: a field guide for divers and aquarists. Coral Graphics, Barrigada/Guam Peng CJ, Lee KL, Ingersoll GM (2002) An introduction to logistic regression analysis and reporting. J. Educ Res 96(1): 3-14 Quinn GP, Keough MJ (2002) Experimental Design and Data Analysis for Biologists. Cambridge University Press, New York R Development Core Team (2008) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria http://www.R-project.org Randall J (2005) Reef and shore fishes of the South Pacific: New Caledonia to Tahiti and the Pitcairn Islands. University of Hawaii Press, Honolulu Reebs SG (2002) Plasticity of diel and circadian activity rhythms in fishes. Rev Fish Biol Fish 12: 349-371 Roberts CM, McClean CJ, Veron JEN, Hawkins JP, Allen GR, McAllister DE, Mittermeier CG, Schueler FW, Spalding M, Wells F, Vynne C, Werner TB (2002). Marine biodiversity hotspots and conservation priorities for tropical reefs. Science 295:1280-1284 Rossiter DG, Loza A (2010) Technical note: analyzing land cover change with logistic regression in R. University of Twente, Faculty of Geo-Information Science & Earth Observation, Enschede, NL. http://www.itc.nl/~rossiter/teach/R/R_lcc.pdf Schofield P (2009) Geographic extent and chronology of the invasion of non-native lionfish (Pterois volitans [Linnaeus 1758] and P. miles [Bennet 1828]) in the Western North Atlantic and Caribbean Sea. Aquat Invasions 4(3): 473-479 Schofield P (2010) Update on geographic spread of invasive lionfishes (Pterois volitans [Linnaeus 1758] and P. miles [Bennet 1828]) in the Western North Atlantic Ocean, Caribbean Sea and Gulf of Mexico. Aquat Invasions 5(1): S117-S122 Schlaepfer MA, Sherman PW, Blossey B, Runge, MC (2005) Introduced species as evolutionary traps. Ecol Lett 8: 241-246 20 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 Semmens BX, Buhle ER, Salomon AK, Pattengill-Semmens CV (2004) A hotspot of non-native marine fishes: evidence for the aquarium trade as an invasion pathway. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 266: 239-244 Smith GR, Boyd A, Dayer CB, Winter KE (2008) Behavioral responses of American toad and bullfrog tadpoles to the presence of cues from the invasive fish, Gambusia affinis. Biol Inv 10(5): 743-748 Stephens DW, Krebs JR (1986) Foraging theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey Valdez-Moreno M, Quintal-Lizama C, Gómez-Lozano R, García-Rivas MC (2012) Monitoring an alien invasión: DNA barcoding and the identification of lionfish and their prey on coral reefs of the Mexican Caribbean. Plos One 7(6): e36636 Walsh C, Mac Nally R (2008) hier.part: Hierarchical partitioning, R package version 1.0-3. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Springer, New York Whitfield PE, Hare JA, David AW, Harter SL, Muñoz RC, Addison CM (2007) Abundance estimates of the Indo-Pacific lionfish Pterois volitans/miles complex in the Western North Atlantic. Biol Invasions 9: 53-64. 21 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 Table 1. Results of logistic regressions of lionfish time budgets between the native Pacific Ocean and the invaded Atlantic Ocean, using a general linear mixed-effects model (GLMM). Shown are coefficient estimates for each explanatory variable (ß) together with standard errors (SE), significance (p) and odd ratios (eß). Also presented are the independent effects of each explanatory variable (%) on the dependent variable (lionfish behavior), calculated by hierarchical partitioning. P values in bold italics are significant. Likelihood ratio and chi-square test statistics indicate logistic regression model fit. Reference levels for this regression were set as Atlantic for ocean, sunrise for time period, hard coral for habitat, clear for weather, and high for current. Coefficient Intercept Ocean Pacific Time Period Morning Midday Afternoon Sunset Habitat other rock‐boulder sand‐rubble Cloud cover overcast partly cloudy Current low medium Size Depth Likelihood ratio test statistic Pearson Chi‐square 543 Estimate (ß) ‐0.003 0.438 ‐2.557 ‐2.446 ‐1.748 ‐0.719 0.434 ‐0.479 0.451 0.687 ‐0.158 1.210 1.125 ‐0.014 0.028 SE 0.773 0.891 0.244 0.279 0.216 0.271 0.259 0.239 0.271 0.227 0.184 0.326 0.346 0.013 0.024 p eß 0.997 0.623 1.549 <0.001 0.078 <0.001 0.087 <0.001 0.174 0.008 0.487 0.094 1.543 0.045 0.619 0.096 1.569 0.002 1.988 0.389 0.854 <0.001 3.355 0.001 3.079 0.102 0.258 0.499 0.241 420.736, p<0.001 1092.019, p=0.727 Independent effects (%) 3.754 35.626 13.715 7.567 9.570 10.055 19.712 22 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 Table 2. Reef-fish and crustacean prey targeted by lionfish (based on strikes, but not necessarily kills) while hunting in their native Pacific (Philippines, Guam) and the invaded Atlantic (Cayman Islands, Bahamas). Shown are species (or taxa when species identification was not possible) targeted with an indication of whether each prey type was accessible to lionfish in the Atlantic, Pacific or both, and with their respective species specific successful kill rates. The unidentified category includes both fishes and crustaceans. PACIFIC (Pac.) ATLANTIC (Atl.) Philippines Guam Caymans Bahamas Access In % Success % Success % Success % Success Mysidacea Both ‐ ‐ 50 ‐ Osteichthyes Acanthuridae Acanthurus nigricans Pac. ‐ 0 ‐ ‐ Apogonidae Apogon townsendi Atl. ‐ ‐ 100 ‐ Apogon sp. Pac. 0 0 ‐ ‐ Cheilodipterus sp. Pac. 0 ‐ ‐ ‐ Subphylum/ Superclass Family Species/taxa Crustacea Blenniidae Malacoctenus triangulatus Atl. ‐ ‐ 100 ‐ Pac. ‐ 0 ‐ ‐ Gobiidae Meiacanthus atrodorsalis Coryphopterus glaucofraenum ‐ ‐ 100 75 Unidentified goby Both 0 0 100 ‐ Grammatidae Gramma loreto Atl. ‐ ‐ 0 ‐ Labridae Halichoeres bivittatus Atl. ‐ ‐ ‐ 50 Halichoeres garnoti Atl. ‐ ‐ 100 0 Labroides dimidiatus Pac. 0 ‐ ‐ ‐ Thalassoma bifasciatum Atl. ‐ ‐ ‐ 33 Plotosidae Plotosus lineatus Pac. 0 ‐ ‐ ‐ Pomacentridae Chromis recruits Pac. 50 ‐ ‐ ‐ Pomacentrus coelestis Pac. ‐ 0 ‐ ‐ Stegastes partitus Atl. ‐ ‐ ‐ 100 Scaridae Scarus iserti Atl. ‐ ‐ ‐ 75 Sparisoma aurofrenatum Atl. ‐ ‐ ‐ 100 Unidentified parrotfish Atl. ‐ ‐ 0 ‐ Serranidae Serranus tigrinus Atl. ‐ ‐ 0 ‐ Tetraodontidae Canthigaster rostrata Atl. ‐ ‐ 0 0 Trichonotidae Trichonotus elegans Pac. 100 ‐ ‐ ‐ Both 36.2 29.2 20.9 22.7 Unidentified Atl. 23 551 552 553 554 Fig. 1. Regions samp pled: the Phiilippines and d Guam in thhe Pacific Ocean, and thhe Cayman ntic Ocean. Islands and Bahamass in the Atlan 24 Mean proportion of time (+SE) 1.00 Inactive MinAct Active Hunting 0.90 0.80 0.70 0.60 0.50 0.40 0.30 0.20 0.10 0.00 Philippines n=221 PACIFIC 555 556 557 558 Guam n=167 Cayman Islands n=439 Bahamas n=324 ATLANTIC Fig. 2. Time budget summaries for lionfish in each of four regions, two per ocean. Shown are mean proportions (± SEM, n = number of 10-minute samples) of dawn-to-dusk time in each of the four activities recorded (inactive, minimal activity [MinAct], active and hunting). 25 559 Philippines C Inactive Hunting Mean proportion of time (+SE) 1 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 1 Mean proportion of time (+SE) A Sunrise* Morning n= 66 n= 26 n= 70 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 Sunrise* Morning n= 94 n= 33 D Guam 1 Mean proportion of time (+SE) 0.8 Midday Afternoon Sunset Inactive 0.9 Hunting 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 1 Mean proportion of time (+SE) n= 26 n= 88 Midday Afternoon Sunset n= 85 n= 48 n= 124 Bahamas Inactive Hunting 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 0 Sunrise* Morning n= 40 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 Inactive Hunting 0.9 0 0 B Cayman Islands n= 42 Midday Afternoon n= 11 PACIFIC n= 36 Sunset n= 38 Sunrise* Morning n= 72 n= 71 Midday Afternoon n= 24 n= 137 Sunset n= 20 ATLANTIC Fig. 3. Time budgets of lionfish across time of day for inactivity and hunting behavior at each of the four regions: (A) Philippines , (B) Guam, (C) Cayman Islands, and (D) Bahamas. Shown are mean proportions (± SEM) of time spent in each behavior, standardized to regional sunrise and sunset times: Sunrise = sunrise to 2 hr after sunrise, Morning = 2 to 5 hr after sunrise, Midday = ca. 3-hour period midway between sunrise and sunset, Afternoon = 2 to 5 hr before sunset, Sunset = 2 hr before sunset to sunset. n = # of 10-min samples. Asterisk (*) next to sunrise represents significant differences in activity levels for this time period (p < 0.01; see GLMM results Table 1). 26 569 0.50 Kills Strikes Mean rate (#/10 mins +SE) 0.45 A 0.40 0.35 0.30 0.25 0.20 0.15 0.10 0.05 0.00 Philippines Guam Cayman Islands Little Cayman Bahamas 0.8 Mean blow rate (#blows/10 min +SE) 0.7 a a 0.6 B 0.5 0.4 0.3 b 0.2 b 0.1 0 Philippines Guam PACIFIC 570 571 572 573 574 575 Cayman Islands Little Cayman Bahamas ATLANTIC Fig. 4. Comparison of: (A) mean ( SEM) strike rates (successful and unsuccessful) and kill rates (successful strikes, i.e. prey consumed), and (B) mean ( SEM) blow rate of lionfish in the Pacific (left) and Atlantic (right). For (B) letters denote significant differences in blow rates between Pacific and Atlantic Oceans (Pseudo-F = 13.234, df = 1, 1113, p = 0.001). 27 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 Fig 5. Mean M proporttion of time (± SEM) liionfish spennt inactive oor hunting inn relation too: (A) cloud cov ver (C = cleaar, PC = parrtly cloudy, O = overcastt), (B) waterr current leveel (L = low,, M = medium, H = high)), and (C) habitat h (HC C = hard cooral, RBC = rock-bouldder/cave, SR RB = sand/rubb ble) in the native n Pacificc (left) and invaded i Atlaantic (right). Asterisks nnext to letterrs (H, O and RBC) R represent significaant differencces in overaall activity levels at this level for each environm mental factorr (p< 0.05; seee GLMM reesults Table 1). 28 40.00 35.00 % Independent effects 30.00 25.00 20.00 15.00 10.00 5.00 0.00 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 Time* Period Depth Habitat * Size Current* Cloud * cover Ocean Fig. 6. The percent contribution of each explanatory environmental variable to the total variation in lionfish behavior detected by the hierarchical partitioning analyses of the logistic regression model (see Table S1 in the Supplement). Asterisks next to variables indicate which variables were significant in the logistic model.