Understanding Wilderness and Subsistence in Gates of the Arctic National Park and Preserve, Alaska Chad E. Dear Abstract—Recreationists’ understanding of subsistence in Gates of the Arctic National Park (GAAR) was examined using Structural Developmental Theory. This perspective contends that people universally differentiate three major domains of social issues—moral, conventional, and personal—by how they reason about them. Seventy-five percent of respondents understood subsistence use of GAAR as a moral issue. Further, respondents maintained conflicting moral judgments when justice, welfare, and naturalism values associated with subsistence were juxtaposed with human and nonhuman welfare values associated with wilderness. The high incidence of conflicting judgments suggests the existence of conflict, or cognitive disequilibrium, between values associated with subsistence and values associated with wilderness. Respondents attempted to coordinate their judgments in ways that can be described as overriding, contradictory, and contextual. No respondents had hierarchical integrated understandings of subsistence and wilderness values. Implications of these findings are discussed. Introduction ____________________ Gates of the Arctic National Park & Preserve (GAAR), one of the 10 park units created by the Alaskan National Interest Lands Conservation Act (ANILCA) of 1980, was established with a management mandate to preserve wilderness values that is unparalleled in any other U.S. National Park (USDI/ NPS 1986). This mandate, however, must be understood in the context of the other purposes for which the park was established, allowed uses, and prior existing rights. GAAR and other National Parks created by ANILCA, unlike parks in the lower forty-eight United States, allow for the continuation of “customary and traditional” subsistence uses by rural Alaskan residents of wild, renewable resources for direct personal or family consumption (Willis 1985). Although the allowance of subsistence in GAAR diverges from previous legislative definitions of wilderness, the human presence and subsistence culture in the park was initially thought to complement the wilderness purposes of the park (USDI/NPS 1986). Such a complimentary relationship would successfully harmonize the GAAR management mandates to allow for the continuation of subsistence and to Chad E. Dear received his master’s degree in geography from Western Washington University, Bellingham, WA 98225, U.S.A. He is currently a Fulbright Fellow in Swaziland, Southern Africa. E-mail: chaddear@ hotmail.com In: Watson, Alan; Sproull, Janet, comps. 2003. Science and stewardship to protect and sustain wilderness values: Seventh World Wilderness Congress symposium; 2001 November 2–8; Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Proc. RMRS-P-27. Ogden, UT: U.S. Department of Agriculture, Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Research Station. 90 “provide for the enjoyment” of the wilderness qualities of the park. The actual relationship between the subsistence culture and the wilderness purposes of GAAR, however, has not been adequately studied and is, therefore, not yet well understood. Understanding recreationists’ perspectives regarding subsistence in GAAR is important for two primary reasons: (1) to determine if the aforementioned GAAR management mandates conflict, and (2) to empirically examine the influence interactions with subsistence may have on psychological developmental aspects of recreationists’ understanding of nature, indigenous people, the human role in wilderness, and the human-nature relationship. The purpose of this study, therefore, is to identify, classify and analyze recreationists’ understanding of subsistence in the context of their experience in GAAR. Theoretical Framework ___________ Such an analysis requires an approach that accounts for the complexities inherent in the coordination of the socialpsychological constructions of wilderness and subsistence. In other words, recreationists’ behaviors and reactions to subsistence can not be taken at face value because such behaviors and reactions are underlain by complex patterns of understanding and active efforts to balance personal judgments about the issues the situation in GAAR provokes. Structural Developmental Theory, the perspective employed in this study, explains human understanding through the nature and functions of psychological structures (Ginsburg and Opper 1969). These structures are comprised of the various ideas people develop and maintain regarding social issues. Structures also include patterns of thinking, or schemas, that situate ideas within more or less well-organized wholes. Ideas are organized within these schemas through the process of reasoning about social issues. Ideas and schemas regarding social issues can be differentiated depending on the perceived moral, conventional, or personal nature of issues (Turiel 1998). In other words, if a person thinks morally about an issue, they will employ different ideas and reason about the issue differently than a person who thinks about the same issue as governed by conventional or personal considerations. Specific qualities of individuals’ judgments and supporting justifications are the criteria for determining into which “social domain” the individual conceptualizes an issue. Identification, classification, and analysis of ideas and schemas within moral, conventional, and personal social domains provide a basis for understanding the interrelations between thought, action, and cultural context (Kahn 1999). The premise here is that forms of reasoning make a difference in how different possibilities are weighed and USDA Forest Service Proceedings RMRS-P-27. 2003 Dear calculated by an individual. The link between thought, action, and cultural context is complicated, however, by the psychological fact that people develop and simultaneously maintain multiple and, at times, conflicting judgments regarding the same issue (Turiel and others 1991). Such cases may result in judgment/action inconsistencies. Turiel and others (1991) note that judgment/action inconsistencies require explanations that take judgment coordination into account. Judgment coordination occurs when an individual’s experience simultaneously activates psychological structures supporting conflicting judgments. These structures, which were not brought into relation before, are forced into a psychological relationship. The relationship established through judgment coordination generally involves a compromising of structures. Identifying, classifying, and analyzing ideas and schemas related to an issue within moral, conventional, and personal domains aid in understanding an individual’s or group’s progress through the process of judgment coordination. Finally, hierarchical integration, or structural development, happens when previously held psychological structures are, through interaction with the social and physical world, transformed into more comprehensive and adequate ways of understanding the world and acting upon it. This process involves the development of a new understanding of conflicting values that preserves the integrity of both values, but transforms their relationship from one of conflict to one of integration under some superordinate concept. This process is distinct from judgment coordination in that it does not involve a compromising of judgments, but instead involves an integration of judgments at a hierarchically elevated level. Methods _______________________ Semistructured interviews including hypothetical scenarios describing interactions between recreational and subsistence use of GAAR were posed to GAAR recreationists. Qualitative methods were employed to (1) identify recreationists’ initial and secondary judgments and justifications of the issue; (2) classify respondents’ initial judgments within moral, conventional, or personal social domains; and (3) analyze the coordination efforts between recreationists’ multiple and conflicting judgments and justifications. The respondent group was composed of 24 males and 6 females ranging from 19 to 66 years of age with an average age of 42. Seventy-six percent had received or were working on four-year college degrees. Forty-six percent had attained or were working on graduate or professional degrees. Fortyeight percent of respondents identified their homes as being in urban areas, 30 percent in suburban areas, and 22 percent in rural areas. Thirty-three percent of respondents were from the contiguous United States east of the Rocky Mountains, 26 percent west of the Rocky Mountains, 22 percent were from Alaska, and 18 percent from Europe. Ninetythree percent of respondents identified themselves as being Caucasian. Respondents had been planning their trips to GAAR for an average of 7.5 months. USDA Forest Service Proceedings RMRS-P-27. 2003 Understanding Wilderness and Subsistence in Gates of the Arctic... Findings _______________________ Sixty-one percent of respondents were accepting of subsistence in GAAR and conceptualized the issue as moral. Fourteen percent were not accepting of subsistence in GAAR and also conceptualized the issue as moral. Justifications of both positive and negative moral judgments referred to concepts of justice (rights and fairness), welfare (of humans and/or nature), and naturalism (perceived harmonious and respectful relationship between subsistence user and nature). Twenty-five percent of respondents were accepting of subsistence in GAAR and conceptualized the issue as conventional. A broad finding of this study is that respondents’ psychological structures associated with wilderness generally developed exclusively of psychological structures associated with subsistence, and vice versa. In other words, there was a lack of coordination between wilderness and subsistence structures. These structures may have developed separately because they were never before forced together through experience. While previous experience may have rarely done so, the scenarios presented to respondents in interviews did simultaneously activate recreationists’ psychological structures regarding wilderness and subsistence. Questions in the interview forced respondents to reason about the issue of subsistence use of GAAR Wilderness in a way that connects these previously disconnected structures. Respondents’ attempts to reason about the hypothetical scenario resulted in respondents maintaining multiple and conflicting judgments in the interview. The high percentage of individual respondents who maintained conflicting judgments suggests the existence of conflict, or cognitive disequilibrium, between values associated with subsistence and values associated with wilderness. The psychological discomfort associated with states of disequilibrium led respondents to attempt to resolve or coordinate their conflicting judgments. Respondents were identified as coordinating their judgments in ways that can be described as overriding, contradictory, and contextual. Each major coordination type and subtype, as well as their implications, are described below. In many cases, individuals coordinated their conflicting judgments in multiple ways. Judgment Coordinations Overriding—In overriding coordinations, a moral judgment supporting subsistence trumped conflicting secondary judgments supporting wilderness values. The 37 percent of respondents classified in this category lacked a developed moral understanding of wilderness values. This allowed the well-established moral content and structure of respondents’ judgments supporting subsistence to override emerging, still tentative, and not as widely shared judgments supporting wilderness values. For example, Doug, a 55year-old from New Jersey, stated that an interaction with subsistence in GAAR would make his experience “less of a wilderness experience, but in the kind of situations we are 91 Dear talking about its okay…that is part of the balancing of interests that I’m prepared to accept and that I believe is just.” The moral value Doug associates with subsistence, what he “believes is just,” overrode values he associated with wilderness experience. Doug’s judgment coordination, as well as others in this category, may be explained by the general acknowledgment that social justice moral schemas are much stronger in society than wilderness/biocentric ones. The lack of strength of wilderness values also may have to do with the tendency of these subjects to frame wilderness values as mostly grounded in self-interested references relating to “wilderness experience”—an argument perceived as much weaker than social justice as grounds for moral justification. The implication of this finding is that the subsistence community, as well as land managers, can rely on a significant amount of support for subsistence use of GAAR. Overriding coordinations, however, foreclose any attempts at integration between wilderness and subsistence values. Recreationists’ support of subsistence, therefore, is at the expense of wilderness values. Contradictory—In contradictory coordinations, two contradictory judgments were simultaneously upheld. The 25 percent of respondents classified in this category conceptualized the issue of subsistence in wilderness as being conventional rather than moral. Psychologically structuring the issue as conventional allowed these respondents to maintain contradictory judgments because their final judgment ultimately depended upon the social norms of the area established by custom, laws, and regulations. This group of recreationists does not need to be persuaded to accept subsistence for any reason besides the fact that there is a law allowing subsistence use of GAAR. Contextual—In contextual coordinations, respondents’ judgments depended on the specific context, as specified by them. Four major types were identified: ecological impact, naturalism, race, and frequency of interaction. Each contextual coordination subtype, as well as its implications, is detailed below. Eighty-three percent of respondents’ support for subsistence use of GAAR was contingent upon subsistence users’ impact on what was perceived as the more important moral value of ecological health. If subsistence was determined to degrade the ecological health of the environment, then subsistence would not be acceptable. For example, Eric, a 19year-old from England, claimed that he would support subsistence “as long as the other concerns for the ecology of the area…are being met.” If this category of respondents is added to the 14 percent of respondents who were initially not accepting of subsistence for reasons including ecological impact, then nearly all (97 percent) respondents can be understood to value ecological welfare when set in competition with justice, welfare, and naturalism values associated with subsistence. The implication of this finding is that if recreational and subsistence activities occur in the same geographic area, and psychological conflict is to be kept at a minimum, perceived environmental impact must be kept low. The greater the perceived environmental impact, the greater the difficulty of eventually integrating wilderness and subsistence values. 92 Understanding Wilderness and Subsistence in Gates of the Arctic... Seventy percent of respondents made contextual coordinations referring to naturalism, or a perceived harmonious and respectful relationship with nature. In such cases, respondents supported subsistence; however, this support was contingent upon subsistence users’ ability to exemplify a harmonious and respectful relationship with nature. Recreationists required that subsistence users conduct their activities according to an environmental ethic that, in most cases, was assumed to be part of the subsistence culture. Recreationists’ support for subsistence was withdrawn if this ethic was not perceived. This pattern of support—and withdrawal of support—for subsistence, depending on respondents’ perceptions of local peoples’ relationships with nature, is characteristic of the stereotype of the “ecologically noble savage” (Buege 1996). This requirement of “authenticity,” Buege claims, is oppressive in that it forces Natives to conform to non-Native conceptions of how they should live and relate to nature. Very few respondents overtly acknowledged that the right to self-determination—a prominent justification supporting subsistence in the park—contradicts and could possibly override the naturalistic reasoning pattern described above. Respondents’ support of subsistence was also dependent on race. Thirty-three percent of respondents were less likely or unwilling to judge subsistence acceptable if the subsistence user was a non-Native. For example, Dave, a 47-yearold Hispanic from California, claimed that “if a non-Native person...had the right to [subsist in the park] through...a government permit or something like that then I would be more adverse to that than if it was a person who through family generations acquired that right.” The ANILCA mandate “to provide the opportunity for rural resident people engaged in a subsistence way of life to continue to do so” (emphasis added) is a colorblind mandate (Willis 1985). A significant portion of subsistence activities in and around GAAR is undertaken by non-Native rural residents. The condition of race described by this group of respondents reveals a potential for increased conflict between recreational users and non-Native subsistence users of GAAR. Frequency of interaction was the final major type of contextual coordination identified. Thirty percent of respondents claimed that their support for subsistence would deteriorate with increased encounters with subsistence or signs of subsistence activities. The implication of this finding is that conflict between recreation and subsistence users of GAAR could be avoided by regulating recreational visitation to keep frequency of contact with subsistence low. If high frequency of contact is unavoidable, stronger justifications supporting subsistence may be needed to minimize conflict. A less prominent, but interesting, contextual coordination referred to a distinction between subsistence being acceptable in the park and subsistence being acceptable in respondents’ experience in the park. In other words, subsistence is acceptable, but “Not In My Wilderness Experience” (NIMWE). The enormous size of GAAR, coupled with the fact that recreational and subsistence activities generally occur in separate areas of the park, suggests that a large majority of visitors to GAAR never see subsistence activity or signs of it. These physical realities currently allow for the continuation USDA Forest Service Proceedings RMRS-P-27. 2003 Dear of NIMWE thinking. The finding that NIMWE thinking was apparent in at least 17 percent of respondents suggests that a partitioning of the park through a zoning scheme, where recreationists would know that they have a lesser chance of encountering subsistence, would be accepted and appreciated by this segment of recreationists. The separation of users or perpetuation of NIMWE thinking would not, however, contribute to a more adequate understanding of the unresolved conflicts in recreationists’ understanding of subsistence in GAAR. In other words, this practice would avoid psychological and social conflict rather than attempting to resolve conflict. Toward an Integrated Understanding of Wilderness and Subsistence Values ______________ Respondents generally lacked the ability to coherently reason about or understand subsistence use of GAAR. Through the interview process, many respondents showed signs that they were becoming aware of the inadequacy of their understanding of the issue. This awareness of their inadequacy of understanding, or state of disequilibrium, led in this case to judgment coordination efforts. Such efforts involved certain values being maintained at the expense of other values. Disequilibrium may also instigate the development of new and more adequate psychological structures that preserve the integrity of both values, but transform their relationship from one of conflict to one of integration under some superordinate concept (that is, hierarchical integration). The presence of disequilibrium, and the subsequent potential for hierarchical integration, represents an opportunity to move closer to the goal of reducing conflict between wilderness and subsistence values in GAAR and other parks and protected areas around the world. Left unmanaged, recreationists’ personal experiences, such as actually encountering subsistence in GAAR or other parks, might activate developmental processes or it might not; existing biases and contradictions might prevail. Without presentation of balanced views regarding this issue, such as was presented in interviews, exposure to subsistence in GAAR or other “wild” areas may only reinforce and elaborate existing conflicting understandings of wilderness and subsistence. The opportunity for the National Park Service, or other parties with a vested interest in the continued preservation of the physical and cultural values inherent in GAAR, is to facilitate the developmental process of creating new understandings of wilderness, subsistence, and their relationship in the park. Recognizing that the active mental life of visitors is critical in their experience is a first step in this process. Recreationists are not passive receptors easily molded by pamphlet-style educational efforts. Recreationists’ response to these or any other informational medium is determined USDA Forest Service Proceedings RMRS-P-27. 2003 Understanding Wilderness and Subsistence in Gates of the Arctic... in large part by their existing ideas and schemas regarding an issue. Their existing understanding is not idiosyncratically structured, but generally follows the various patterns revealed in this study. Specifically, recreationists are inclined to identify and reason about conflicting moral ideas involved in interactions between recreational and subsistence use of the park. In this sense, wilderness recreationists are moral philosophers potentially seeking advanced forms of reasoning. Effective efforts to influence recreationists’ judgments and actions regarding subsistence use of GAAR and other parks and protected areas involves understanding and accounting for the deliberate, systematic, and philosophical nature of recreationists’ thought. To the extent that managers can understand, work with, and employ methods that more directly engage peoples’ psychological structures, the more precise and sure they can be about the effects of particular managerial interventions. Understanding recreationists’ thought content and processes relating to subsistence are also relevant beyond immediate management concerns. Such understanding can add clarity to the current contentious discourse regarding the idea of wilderness and its ability to appropriately address issues of past and present indigenous human cultures on wild lands. In this regard, a structural developmental approach to what has been termed by some, “The Great New Wilderness Debate” (Callicott and Nelson 1998), may illustrate the disequilibrium many in the conservation philosophy community are experiencing. Such an approach may also trace our psychological progress toward a more adequate and integrated understanding of wilderness, subsistence, and the human role in nature. References _____________________ Buege, Douglas J. 1996. The ecologically noble savage revisited. Environmental Ethics. 18(1): 71–88. Callicott, J. Baird; Nelson, Michael P. 1998. The great new wilderness debate. Athens: University of Georgia Press. 712 p. Ginsburg, Herbert; Opper, Sylvia. 1969. Piaget’s theory of intellectual development: an introduction. New Jersey: Englewood Cliffs. 272 p. Kahn, Peter H., Jr. 1999. The human relationship with nature: development and culture. Cambridge: The MIT Press. 295 p. Turiel, Elliot. 1998. The development of morality. In: Damon, W., ed. Handbook of child psychology. 5th ed., volume 3. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: 863–932. Turiel, Elliot; Hildebrandt, Carolyn; Wainryb, Cecilia. 1991. Judging social issues: difficulties, inconsistencies, and consistencies. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 56(2): 1–15. U.S. Department of the Interior/National Park Service. 1986. Gates of the Arctic National Park: general management plan, land protection plan, and wilderness suitability review. Washington, DC: USDI/NPS. Willis, Frank. 1985. Doing things right the first time: the National Park Service and the Alaska National Interest Lands Conservation Act of 1980. Washington, DC: USDI/NPS. 93