This file was created by scanning the printed publication. Errors identified by the software have been corrected; however, some errors may remain. Effect of Seed Size on Removal by Rodents1 William G. Standley2 Seeding is commonly used for restoring depleted vegetation. Many seeding projects fail because rodents eat the seeds (Bramble and Sharp 1949, Spencer 1954, Nelson et al. 1970). A variety of techniques for reducing the impact of rodents have been tested, but few have been successful. Most often resource managers poison rodent populations before seeding, but this method is largely unsuccessful because of rapid immigration of new individuals (Sullivan 1979, Sullivan and Sullivan 1984).New methods of biological management could be developed that use information gained from diet and behavior studies to reduce destruction of seeds by rodents. Many studies show that certain rodents prefer particular species or sizes of seeds (Reynolds and Haskell1949, Reynolds 1950, Abbott 1962, Gashwiler 1967, Smith 1970, Lockard and Lockard 1971, Srnigel and Rosenzweig 1974, Everett et al. 1978, Price 1983).Thus, whenever alternative plant species are available that both meet the resource manager's objectives and have seeds not preferentially foraged by local seed'Paper presented at symposium, Management of Amphibians, Reptiles, and Small Mammals in North America. (Flagstaff, AZ,July 19-2 1, 1988.) 2WilliamG. Standley, formerly a graduate student, University of Arizona, Arizona Cooperative Fish and Wildlife Research Unit, is currently Animal Ecologist, EG&G Energy Measurements, lnc., c/o NPR- 1, P. 0. Box 127, Tupman, CA, 93276. Abstract.-Plots located in southeastern Ariiona were seeded with small and large grass seeds. After 3 days, virtually all large seeds were removed by rodents, while small seeds were still present 36 days after planting. Thus, managers may increase seed survival in this area, without removing rodents, b y seeding with small seeds rather than large seeds. eating rodents, seeding could be successful even with rodents present. In southwestern deserts of North America, where range managers are attempting to restore rangelands depleted by overgrazing (Cox et al. 19821, kangaroo rats (Dipodornys sp.) and pocket mice (Perognathus sp. and Chaetognathus sp.) are some of the primary seed eaters (Brown et al. 1979b). As early as 1950, Reynolds suggested that the influence of Merriam's kangaroo rats (Dipodornys rnerriami) on seeding success depends on the size of seeds used. Brown et al. (1979b), Inouye et al. (1980), and Price (1983) all found that heteromyids preyed selectively on large seeds. In this study, I investigated the prediction that fewer small seeds than large seeds would be removed by rodents in a seeded area in southeastern Arizona. Study Area and Methods The study area was on the USDA Forest Service Santa Rita Experimental Range located 45 km south of Tucson, Pima County AZ, which is thoroughly described by Martin and Reynolds (1973). The vegetation was typical Sonoran desert-scrub, dominated by mesquite (Prosopis juliflora), burroweed (Haplopappus tenuisectus) and cholla (Opuntia spp.). Annual precipitation averages 36 to 43 cm and is bimodal, with peaks in winter and summer. Plots were seeded fol- lowing all recommended procedures (Jordan 19811, using large and small seeds, both separately and together. Seeded plots were located within a slightly sloped I-hectare area with a Comoro soil type, at an elevation of 1300 m. I compared the number of seeds surviving on 4 experimental plots to the number of seeds surviving on a control plot which was protected from rodents. The study area was prepared by removing large shrubs by hand and plowing small plants with a disk. The control plot was protected from rodents with a rodent-proof fence similar to that used by Brown et al. (1979a). All rodents within the exclosure were removed by trapping before seeding. Each of the 5-15 x 17 m plots was seeded with 3 evenly placed pairs of 15 m rows, one pair for each of 3 treatments which were: (1)small seeds planted at a rate of 175/m (0.15 g/m), (2) large seeds planted at a rate of 100/m (4.7 g/m), and (3) 5 small and large seeds planted together at 88/m and 50/m (0.07g/m and 2.35 g/m), respectively. Seeding rates were chosen according to recommended rates for similar sized seeds (Jordan 1981). The treatment assigned to each pair of rows was randomly selected. The small seeds were blue panicgrass (Panicurn antidotale) which weighed an average of 0.85 mg each. The large seeds were barley (Hordeurn vulgare) which weighed an average of 47.0 mg each. All seeds were planted with a cone seeder at a depth of 1 to 2 cm on 21 June 1984, just before expected summer rains. Because blue panicgrass seeds are very small and difficult to recover from the soil, they were dyed with water soluble green food coloring before planting. Barley seeds were also dyed to avoid a possible bias. The species of rodents on the experimental plots were monitored by placing 100 live traps at 10 m intervals on and around the plots on the 5th and 6th nights after planting. Traps were baited with a mixture of both sizes of seeds and checked at midnight and sunrise. The number of seeds surviving on plots was monitored by collecting soil samples from the rows immediately after planting and at 3,9,18, and 36 days after planting. One random sample was taken from each quarter of every row each time. Samples were not taken from the outer meter of any row because the cone seeder applied seeds at a more variable rate at the beginning and end of each row. Soil samples, 2.5 to 3.5 cm deep and 15 x 25 cm in area, were taken lengthwise along each row with the aid of a two-sided, fixed-area sampler and a trowel. The samples were placed in paper bags, and oven-dried at 50 C for 24 hours. Seeds were recovered by shaking soil samples through a series of Tyler sieves (#5,#10,#14,#18,#20,and #25) for 3 minutes. The number of seeds remaining were counted by examining the contents of each sieve, both dry and immersed in a salt water solution, through a 10X viewing scope. The average number of seeds recovered in the soil samples taken from the 4 experimental plots divided by the total number found in the control plot times 100 was used as a seed survival index (SSI). This dimensionless index permits comparison of the removal of different sized seeds by rodents even though they were planted at different rates. It also standardizes for the experi- mental error contributed by the difficulty of recovering small seeds. The granivorous arthropods and birds present on the study area had equal access to control and experimental plots, so should not have biased SSIs. Results Eleven of 17 individual rodents captured on or around the plots were heteromyids: 9 were Merriam's kangaroo rats, and 2 were bannertail Seeds sown separately m 3 9 18 SMALL SEEDS LARGE SEEDS 36 DAYS AFTER PLANTING Seeds sown together SMALL SEEOS 0 3 9 18 36 DAYS AFTER PLANTING Figure 1 .-Seed survival index (average number of seeds recovered in 4 experimental pbts divided by total number of seeds recovered in the control plot times 100) for small and large seeds. (A) Seeds sown separately and (B) Seeds sown together. kangaroo rats (D.spectabilis). Two white-throated woodrats (Neotoma albigula), 2 southern grasshopper mice (Onychomys torridus), 1 deer mouse (Peromyscus rnaniculatus) and 1 cotton rat (Sigmodon hispidus) were also captured. Whether large and small seeds were planted separately or together, the SSIs were higher for small seeds than for large seeds starting with 3 days after planting (fig. 1). After 36 days, the large seeds planted either separately or with small seeds were virtually gone from experimental plots (SSI = 0.4 and 2.1, respectively). The SSI for small seeds planted separately was 76.5 after 36 days, while the small seeds planted with large seeds had an SSI of 43.6. The SSIs of large seeds planted separately decreased at a faster rate than the SSIs of large seeds sown with small seeds. The SSIs of small seeds planted separately decreased at a slower rate than the SSIs of small seeds sown with large seeds, however. Complete data are presented in Standley (1985). Discussion I do not present inferential statistics to test for significant differences between large and small seed survival because the experimental plots were actually sub-plots rather than true replicates (Hurlbert 1984). For this study site, however, striking differences between the SSIs of large and small seeds whether planted separately or together are certainly evidence that smaller seeds have a much higher survival rate than large seeds due to differential predation by rodents. The higher rate of removal of large seeds planted separately compared to large seeds planted with small seeds may have occurred because the lower density of large seeds in the mixed rows made them less attractive to rodents. The relatively higher rate of removal of small seeds planted with large seeds, compared to small seeds planted separately, likely occurred because large seeds attracted rodents to the rows, where the rodents then ate both sizes of seeds. Sullivan and Sullivan (1982) observed the opposite effect when seeding lodgepole pine (Pinus contorta). Lodgepole seed consumption by rodents was reduced by planting the relatively small lodgepole seeds with sunflower seeds, which were larger and more preferred by granivorous rodents present. The opposing results may be due to differences in method of seeding (Sullivan and Sullivan broadcast their seeds) or the size of plots (Sullivan and Sullivan's plots were larger). Another possibility is that the main granivorous rodents in their study area, deer mice, are more selective than the heteromyids present in this study. Nine days after planting there was a lower SSI for small seeds planted separately (fig. la) than on 18 or 36 days, which can only be attributed to variability in seeding rate and sampling error. It is possible that not all seeds removed by rodents, small or large, were destroyed. Reynolds and Glendening (1949) found that the seed caching behavior of Merriam's kangaroo rats actually increased spread of some plant species. Factors other than seed size affect selection by rodents for particular seed species, such as percent soluble carbohydrates (Kelrick and MacMahon 1985, Kelrick et al. 1986; but also see Jenkins 1988), moisture content (Frank 1988a), and moldiness (Frank 1988b). For most seeds, however, resource managers have only the information on size available. This study only compared the effect of size by using grass seeds of similar composition that differ most in their linear dimensions. The results of this study support other studies which showed that heteromyid rodents selected large seeds and reduced standing stocks of large seeds in the soil to a greater extent than small seeds (Brown et al. 1979a, Inouye et al. 1980, Price 1983).Therefore, when site conditions and management needs allow a choice of which species to seed, resource managers should consider the size of seeds when planning seeding in areas inhabited by heteromyids. Acknowledgments Appreciation is extended to Drs. J. H. Brown, H. L. Morton, and N. S. Smith for guidance, and to S. Collins, Dr. J. Cox, S. Horton, M. Podborny, B. Travis, and D. Youkey for field assistance. The research was funded by the Arid Land Ecosystems Improvement Unit of the USDA, Agricultural Research Service. Literature Cited Abbott, Herschel G. 1962. Tree seed preferences of mice and voles in the northeast. Journal of Forestry 60:97-99. Bramble, William C. and Ward M. Sharp. 1949. Rodents as a factor in direct seeding on spoil banks in central Pennsylvania. Journal of Forestry 47:477-478. Brown, James, H., Diane W. Davidson, and 0.J. Reichman. 1979a. An experimental study of competition between seed-eating desert rodents and ants. American Zoologist 19:ll29-1143. Brown, James, H., 0.J. Reichman, and Diane W. Davidson. 1979b. Granivory in desert ecosystems. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 10:201-227. Cox, Jerry, R., Howard L. Morton, Thomas. N. Johnson, Jr., Gilbert L. Jordan, S. Clark Martin, and Louis C. Fierro. 1982. Vegetation restoration in the Chihuahuan and Sonoran deserts of North America. United States Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service, Western Series Number 28, August 1982. Everett, Richard L., Richard 0. Meeuwig, and Richard Stevens. 1978. Deer mouse preference for seed of commonly planted species, indigenous weed seed, and sacrifice foods. Journal of Range Management 31:70-73. Frank, Craig L. 1988a. The influence of moisture content on seed selection by kangaroo rats. Journal of Mammalogy 69:353-357. Frank, Craig L. 1988b. The effects of moldiness level on seed selection by Dipodomys spectabilis. Journal of Mammalogy 69:358-362. Gashwiler, Jay S. 1967. Conifer seed survival in a western Oregon clearcut. Ecology 48:431-438. Hurlbert, Stuart H. 1984. Pseudoreplication and the design of ecological field experiments. Ecological Monographs 54:187-2ll. Inouye, Richard S., Gregory S. Byers, and James H. Brown. 1980. Effects of predation and competition on survivorship, fecundity, and community structure of desert annuals. Ecology 6l:l344-l35l. Jenkins, Stephen H. 1988. Comments on relationships between native seed preferences of shrub-steppe granivores and seed nutritional characteristics. Oecologia 75:481482. Jordan, Gilbert L. 1981. Range seeding and brush management on Arizona rangelands. Cooperative Extension Service, Agricultural Experiment Station, University of Arizona, College of Agriculture, T81121. Kelrick, Michael Ira, and James A. MacMahon. 1985. Nutritional and physical attributes of seeds of some common sagebrush-steppe plants: Some implications for ecological theory and management. J. Range Manage. 38:65-69. Kelrick, M. I., J. A. MacMahon, R. R. Parmenter, and D. V. Sisson. 1986. Native seed preferences of shrubsteppe rodents, birds and ants: the relationships of seed attributes and seed use. Oecologia 68:327337. Lockard, Robert, B., and Joan S. Lockard. 1971. Seed preference and buried seed retrieval of Dipodomys deserti. Journal of Mammalogy 52:219-221. Martin, S. Clark and Hudson G. Reynolds. 1973. The Santa Rita Experimental Range: your facility for research on semidesert ecosystems. Journal of the Arizona Academy of Science 8:56-67. Nelson, Jack R., A. M. Wilson, and Carl J. Goebel. 1970. Factors influencing broadcast seeding in bunchgrass range. Journal of Range Management 23:163-170. Price, Mary V. 1983. Laboratory studies of seed size and seed species selection by heteromyid rodents. Oecologia 60:259-263. Reynolds, Hudson G. 1950. Relation of Merriam kangaroo rats to range vegetation in southern Arizona. Ecology 31:456-463. Reynolds, H. G. and G. E. Glendening. 1949. Merriam kangaroo rat as a factor in mesquite propagation on southern Arizona range lands. Journal of Range Management 2:193-197. Reynolds, Hudson, G. and Horace S. Haskell. 1949. Life history notes on Price and Bailey pocket mice of southern Arizona. Journal of Mammalogy 3O:l5O-l56. Smigel, Barbara W., and Michael L. Rosenzweig. 1974. Seed selection in Dipodomys merriami and Perognathus penicillatus. Ecology 55:329-339. Smith, Christopher C. 1970. The coevolution of pine squirrels (Tamiasciurus) and conifers. Ecological Monographs 40349-371. Spencer, Donald A. 1954. Rodents and direct seeding. Journal of Forestry 52:824-826. Standley, William G. 1985. The effect of seed size on reseeding in the presence of heteromyids. M.S. thesis, University of Arizona, Tucson. 15 p. Sullivan, Thomas P. 1979. Repopulation of clear-cut habitat and conifer seed predation by deer mice. Journal of Wildlife Management 432361-871. Sullivan, Thomas, P. and Druscilla S. Sullivan. 1982. The use of alternative foods to reduce lodgepole pine seed predation by small mammals. Journal of Applied Ecology 19:33-45. Sullivan, Thomas P. and Druscilla S. Sullivan. 1984. Influence of range seeding on rodent populations in the interior of British Columbia. Journal of Range Management 37:163-165.