This file was created by scanning the printed publication. Errors identified by the software have been corrected; however, some errors may remain. Microhabitat as a Template for the Organization of a Desert Rodent Community1 Michael A. Bowers2and Christine A. Flanagan3 It is generally believed that species have different fitnesses in different habitats, that most communities are comprised of sufficient habitat variation over which fitness differentials can be expressed, and that species select habitats that maximize their fitness (e.g., Levins 1962, Schoener 1971).The manner and degree to which species respond to the habitat template involves elements of selection in its purest form (i.e., choice), relegation, and correlation. At the community level rarely do species occupy habitats in an ideal or cost-free fashion. By occupying space or using resources in a habitat specific manner organisms alter habitat suitability and thereby change the basis over which habitats are selected (Fretwell and Lucas 1970).Species that use limited resources in an efficient manner or are behaviorally dominant can monopolize the choicest habitats and relegate, directly or indirectly, subordinate or competitively inferior species to secondary 'Paper presented at symposium, Management of Amphibians, Reptiles, and Small Mammals in North America. (Flagstaff,AZ,July 19-21, 1988.) 2MichaelA. Bowers is Assistant Professor in the Department of Environmental Sciences and Research Coordinator at the Blandy Experimental Farm, University of Virginia, Clark Hall, Charloffesville, VA 22903. 3ChristineA. Hanagan is Assistant Curator of the Orland E. WhiteArboretum, University of Virginia, P.0.Box 1 75, Boyce, VA 22620. Abstract.-We used 20 0.25-hafenced plots to experimentally study microhabitat use by 1 1 desert rodent species in southeastern Arizona. Removal of the largest granivore , Dipodomys spectabifis, produced the most pervasive shifts in the use of microhabitats while adding food or removing ants produced few responses. These results support the idea that this community is organized around competitive interactions involving aggression, preemption, and relegation. habitats (Colwell and Fuentes 1973, Bowers et al. 1987). If the capture success rates of predatory species varies among habitats this can also affect the absolute and relative fitness of prey species and their distribu tion among habitats (Kotler 1984, Bowers 1988). Marked patterns of habitat occupancy and segregation are often cited as evidence that ecological communities are structured. The general pattern is that some (if not most) species in a community utilize habitats differently from random and differently than if each species occurred by itself. Observational and manipulative experiments have shown that dynamical proper ties of populations (including patterns of growth, demographics, and interaction) often become expressed as spatial phenomena, thereby establishing a connection between habitat occupancy and population dynamics (see Connor and Bowers 1987). Many communities are comprised of an array of microhabitats which represent discrete, exploitable resources which occur with sufficient variability so as to be partitionable among species. The availability and distribution of microhabitats have been shown to limit the growth and density of many populations and, thereby provide an ecologically relevant and readily identifiable context over which species interactions and population growth can be studied (Price 1978, Rosenzweig 19811. Desert rodents have long provided ecologists with a model system for examining the role of microhabitat in structuring communities. The basic pattern throughout the major North American deserts is that locally co-occurring species characteristically forage in microhabitats that are structurally distinctive with respect to perennial vegetation and soil type (Rosenzweig and Winakur 1969, Price 1978; for reviews see Brown et al. 1979, Munger et al. 1983, Price and Brown 1983). Three mechanisms, alone or in combination, apparently account for the general pattern. First, because of differences in body size, mode of 1ocomotion and behavior, rodents differ in their abilities to exploit particular distributions of food (i.e., seed) resources that are created by structural features of the microhabitat (Bowers 1982, Harris 1984, Price 1983, Reichman 1981).Second, rodents may differ in their ability to escape visually oriented predators so that the most susceptible rodents are limited to the safest microhabitats (i.e., under vegetative cover) while more vagile rodents show more unrestricted use of alternate microsites (Kotler 1984). Third, the ability of some species to aggressively defend areas from other rodents may be high in some habitats and low in others resulting in habitat dependent segregation involving domination/ relegation (Hutto '1978, Frye 1983, Bowers et al. 1987). Desert rodent populations are remarkable in their ability to respond to short-term changes in the abundance and distribution of food resources; primarily seeds. Some of the more marked responses involve changes in use of microhabitats. For example, enriching microhabitats with supplemental seeds increases the use of these by desert rodents (Harris 1984, Kotler 1984, Price and Waser 1985).Such shifts are particularly noteworthy for microhabitats where the risk of being preyed upon is high, and suggests that both energetic profits and predatory risk play a role in determining which microsites are used (Hay and Fuller 1981, Price and Waser 1985, Bowers 1988). Food availability also can change the manner in which some rodent species interact: from competitive exploitative interactions under low levels of food to aggressive interference interactions under high levels of food (e.g., Congdon 1974). In complex communities microhabitat use originates with preferences of individual species for certain microhabitats, but these basic responses may become altered, directly or indirectly, by interactions with other species. Moreover, at the community level it is not clear how changes in the resource base are manifest in patterns of spatial usage. Some important questions are: Does interspecific competition become more or less important with increasing food availability? Does the mode of competition change? How does food availability change the relative roles of preference and relegation in determining habitat occupancy? Thus, detailing the interplay between population and community-level responses to changes in resource availability should reveal much about the processes influencing microhabita t use and, thereby, the factors responsible for the organization of these communities. In this paper we describe patterns of microhabitat use of 11Chihuahuan Desert rodents over a span of more than eight years. We experimentally manipulated both species composition and food supply and measured resulting shifts in microhabitat use. By detailing shifts in microhabitat use in response to our manipulations we were able to identify the most important interactions among species, estimate their relative strengths, and say something about the mode of interaction promoting the shifts. Our results suggest that the organization of this community revolves more around differences in the ability of species to occupy and defend certain key microhabi ta ts than changes in food availability. Study Site and Methods The present paper details changes in microhabitat use in response to longterm experimental manipulation of rodent composition and food supply. Our study site was located at an elevation of 1330 m in a relatively homogeneous desert shrub habitat on the Cave Creek Bajada 6.5 km east and 2 km north of Portal, in Arizona, USA. Manipulations were performed in twenty 0.25-ha plots. Each plot was fenced with 0.64-cm mesh hardware cloth, extending 0.7-m above and buried 0.2-m below ground. In addition to an unmanipulated fenced control (see below), the remaining treatments consisted of two general classes: treatments where one or more rodent species were removed, potentially changing both food availability and the potential for direct behavioral interactions; and food alteration treatments where supplemental millet seeds were added at a rate of 96 kg per year or seed-eating ants were removed. Experimental treatments were assigned to plots at random. Fourteen rodent species of which 11 were commonly captured, inhabited the study site, all except those mentioned above had equal access to all plots (fig. 1). Because of problems in consistently identifymg the two Onychomys species (as either 0,torridus or 0. leucogaster) we group these together under the designation, Onychomys spp. Sixteen equally-spaced gates in each plot allowed the selective exclusion of rodent species above a threshold body size while allowing all other species access. Access gates varied in size among the treatments. Large gates (3.7 x 5.7-cm) allowed all rodent species free access to control (2 plots), ant removal (4 plots), and the seed addition plots (8plots; see below); medium-sized gates (2.6 x 3.0-cm) were used to exclude only the largest granivore, Dipsdornys spectabilis (2 plots); and small gates (1.9 x 1.9-cm) were used to exclude all Dipodomys species (4 plots). The seed addition treatments included six plots where supplemental seeds were applied in 12 monthly applications (hereafter referred to as "constant seed additions"); two plots received the total allotment of seeds in three applications during the fall (September-November; referred to as "pulsed seed additions"). Seeds were uniformly scattered by hand over each plot. It was estimated from productivity measurements at the site that the addition of 96 kg of seeds per year should have approximately doubled the total biomass of seeds produced annually (our estimate of seed production was ca. 400 kg/ha/yr). The constant seed additions included two plots where whole millet (Panicurn rniliaceam) was added (mean seed mass = 6 mg); two plots where cracked millet was added (mean mass = 1 mg); and two plots where an equal mixture of whole and cracked millet was added. The pulsed seed treatment was designed to represent a doubling of the seed production of summer annual plants, a particularly important food source for the rodents in this community (Davidson et al. 1985). Brown and Munger (1985) found no differences in responses of rodents to addition of seeds of different size, so the four constant seed addition treatments will be lumped together here (6 plots). Rodents were censused monthly during the week of the new moon (moonlight has been shown to effect the microhabitats used by desert rodents; Bowers 1988) using live traps placed in each plot in 7 x 7 grids with 6.5-rn between trap stations. Traps were baited with millet and opened for one night per month with plot gates closed so that only plot residents would be captured. For more details concerning the experimental design, see Bowers et al. 1987, Brown and Munger (19851, and Brown et al. (1986). Following the lead of many previous studies on desert rodent communities we used the percent cover of perennial plants to characterize the microhabitat at each of the 980 trap stations. Percent cover within a 2-m radius of each trap station was measured by ocular estimation using reference disks of known percent coverage. Cover was measured in 1978, 1981, and 1983. There was no significant changes in perennial cover over this five year period (Mann-Whitney U-test; P > 0.051, so we used data from 1983 to characterize microhabitats. Table 1 summarizes vegetation cover data over the entire study site. Fence installation was completed in June, 1977; premanipulative trapping was conducted from July-Seg tember, 1977; and the manipulations were initiated in October, 1977. We restrict our analyses to include postmanipulation data compiled from October 1977 to December 1984 and to only those 20 plots to which rodents had access. Analyses were designed to answer two questions: first, what are the patterns of species associations occurring at the community level; and sec- Bioodamys spaclrbIIIs Dlpodomys m8rrhnl Ant r e m o v a l ( 4 plots) 539 787 Seed-pulse ( 2 plots) 278 390 Seed-constant (6 plots) 821 1005 Dipodomysremoval ( 4 plots) 0 0 8 . spectabilk- 0 465 Dipodomys ordil Porognathus Perognrthus Prnlcillatus fhvuS Peramyscus m#nlculatus Control ( 2 plots) removal ( 2 plots) Figure 1 .-Rodent species on study site (including their body sizes) along with their capture frequencies in each of the experimental treatments. Included are the number of plots in each treatment. ond, what role does microhabitat play in the distribution of individual species. In this study patterns of association (including the association of rodent species with each other and with structural microhabitats) are analyzed at the level of individual trap stations (980 total). Hence, we Poromyscus dontomys magslotis oremlcus Weetama elbi~ulo Onychomyr SPP. were interested in measuring responses of rodents to microhabitat variation occurring at a scale of a meter or two. However, we acknowledge that habitats may also be selected at larger spatial scales (Morris 1987). For example, rodents may also select areas on the basis of rnicrohabitat composites (e.g., at the level of the home range) which might be best examined by considering structural microhabitats over trap station aggregates. However, there is reason to believe that even if selection does occur at these larger scales it is still oriented towards excluding or including certain key microhabitats. Hence, we were confident our analyses would detect patterns at both scales. Indices of species association were calculated by using the frequency that species were captured at the same trap station using trap data for the eight year period. This involved several steps: (i) tabulating the proportion of trap stations where each species was captured over the eight year study; (ii) tallying the number of trap stations where each pair of species co-occurred; and (iii) comparing the observed frequency of co-cap tures to that expected if species captures were distributed independently and randomly among trap stations. The expected frequency of species co-capture was calculated by multiplying together the proportion of stations capturing species individually to generate a probability of joint occurrence. A modified chi-square statistic, including the sign of association, was then used as an index of association: i.e., a measure of the difference between the observed and expected values. The null hypothesis was that there would be an equal number of positive and negative associations with less than 5% of the association values being statistically significant at a P = 0.05. The analysis described above can also be used to examine the association of all species in the community at individual trap stations. Specifi- cally, instead of asking how frequently species pairs associate we can use the maximum likelihood estimation technique to estimate how many trap stations should have captured O,1,2, . . n species (where n is the number of species in the community) over the eight year period. As in the above analysis, this uses the proportion of stations capturing each species, multiplies these together in all possible combinations that might produce co-captures of from 0 to n species, and sums these probabilities for each number of possible co-cap tures to give an expected distribution over the population of trap stations. The null expectation here is that species captures are independently and randomly distributed among trap stations. Analyses were also performed to examine the individualistic responses of species to variation in microhabitat and, particularly, how these change when manipulations are applied at the level of the entire community. We used percent cover by perennial plants at trap stations as a general descriptor of microhabitat type. Our goal was not to use a series of variables to explain the largest amount of variation in microhabitats where species were captured but rather we were interested in identifying a major resource axis over which both species distributions and community-level responses could be analyzed. Past work justified using cover as such a variable (Brown et al. 1979, Munger et al. 1983, Price and Brown 1983). Our scheme of categorizing microhabitats was simple: trap stations were grouped into those with greater-than-median and those with less-than-median cover. This was performed separately for stations in each of the six treatments. Hence, each microhabitat category was represented by an equal number of trap stations in each treatment type. The null hypothesis for analyzing the trap data was if rodents use microhabitats randomly, and without regard to vegetative cover, they should be trapped in equal frequencies at stations in the two microhabitat categories. Avoidance or preference for microhabitats would be indicated by a disproportionate number of captures in one or the other category. We were also interested in examining (1) the microhabitat affinities of species in the different treatments, and (2) shifts in types of microhabitats used by the same species over the different seasons of the year and over the six experimental treatments. In the first case we used the Fisher Exact Probability procedure in a twotailed test of the null hypothesis that captures in the two microhabitats did not differ from a 1:l ratio (Siege1 1956); in the second we subjected the proportion of species' captures in the two microhabitats to a 2-way ANBVA where season and treatment represented treatment factors. Results Results are based on 8,019 captures of the 11 most common rodent species. Figure 1 lists the frequency of capture for each species in the six treatments summed over the eight year study period. Community-Wide Patterns of Microhabitat Use What are the patterns of species association at the level of the entire community? In answering this we considered the frequency that species were captured at the same trap station. We performed two tests. We first calculated species associations for all possible pairings of the 11species occurring in plots with intact rodent assemblages (i.e., those 14 plots with large gates) resulting in a total of 45 values of species association. Plotting all association values show that most species in this community are captured at the same station much less frequently than predicted by chance (fig. 2; the null hypothesis is that there would be an equal number of positive and negative associations and that only 5% of these would be statistically significant at P < 0.05). The deviation from what is expected is particularly striking considering that 27 of the association values exceeded the cutoff value for significance (3.84 for p ~ 0 . 0 5and d.f.=l) and all of these were in the direction of negative species associations; there was not a single significant positive association. This suggests a high level of organization revolves around the spatial segregation of species. Among those factors that could be responsible for this marked segregation are unique habitat preferences of species. These could work alone or in conjunction with habitat segregation that is mediated through interactions with other rodent species. The design of our experiment allows a further examination of the role of species interactions in producing the pattern. Specifically,our experiment includes treatments with an intact rodent assemblage (14 plots; 686 stations) as well as treatments where either D. spectabilis (2 plots; 98 stations) or all Dipodomys (4 plots; 196 stations) were selectively removed and excluded. Because previous studies have shown Dipodomys (and especially D. spectabilis) to be behaviorally dominant over many of the species they co-occur with (Blaustein and Riser 1974, Frye 1983, Bowers et al. 1987) there is reason to think that by their removal the patterns of association of the remaining species may change. To evaluate this possibility we restricted the analyses to include just those eight non-Dipodomys species that occurred in all three treatments (number of painvise association values for this group = 21). The degree to which these species were associated with each other at trap stations in each of the three treatments was calculated as before, and then compared across the three treatments (fig. 3). The results show that removing either all Dipodomys or just D. spectabilis significantly a1ters the degree to which the remaining species are spatially segregated (X2 = 17.33, df = 2; P < 0.000). While the trend is clearly towards more positive and fewer negative associations when competitors are removed, most of the species are still negatively associated with each other. The previous analysis can be extended from the two-species case to one considering the association of all 11 species. Specifically, instead of asking how frequently species pairs associate we can use the maximum likelihood estimation technique to estimate how many trap stations should have captured O,1,2 . . . 11 species over the eight year period. Comparing the actual number of species captured per station with that expected (fig. 4) shows that the observed distribution is shifted to the left of that expected (significantly different at P < 0.05 using KolmogorovSmirnov one sample test), that there are significant differences in the mode of species co-captured per station (expected=4;observed=3), and 16 14 12 Species Associations 10 8 6 4 2 n1 -40 -30 -20 -10 0 10 Chi Square 20 30 40 Figure 2.-Estimates of species associationsfor plots with intact rodent assemblages (i.e., those with large gates). Association values represent modified chi-squares (with the sign of association) and where calculated according to whether species were captured at the same trap station more or less frequently than expected by chance. See text for more detail. Intact Dipodomys Removal 0 0. s. Removal - I positive negative Associations Figure 3.-Histogram of the number of positive and negative species associations for nonDipodomys species broken into three treatment categories: (i) treatments with intact rodent assemblages; (ii) D. specfabilis removal plots; and (iii) Dijwdomys removal plots. that there are large differences in the proportion of stations capturing two species (ca. 5% for the expected compared to 23% for the observed). The main result is that trap stations captured fewer species than expected if species captures were random, which further evidence that species in this community are spatially segregated. Use of Space by Individual Species: The Role of Cover In this section we are interested in the individualistic responses of species to microhabitat variation and, particularly, how these change when manipulations are applied at the level of the entire community. There was marked variability both within and between species in the usage of microhabitats (table 2 and figs. 5 and 6). On control plots Pero- myscus eremicus, Neo toma albigula, Reithrodontomys megalotis, and Dipodomys merriami (in all treatments but the D. spectabilis removals) all showed positive associations for trap stations with greater-than-median cover. Treatment Expected # species per S t a t i o n Figure 4.-Histogram of expected and observed number of species captured at individual trap stations. Figure 5.-Distribution of captures in greater-than and less-than median cover for the five hetsromyidspecies listed according to treatment and seuson. Capture data is graphed relative to what the null hypothesis predicts (i.e., an equal number of captures in both microhabitat types; the zero line). Preference for higher-than-median sites is represented by positive values; less-than-mediancover by negafive values. Bars within treatment categories indicate season: from left to right Spring (March-May), Summer (June-August), Fall (September-November),Winter (December-February). Treatment designation is as follows: "-DS", Dipodomysspectabilk removal; "C",control; "SC",constant seed addition; '-Aw, ant removal; "-Dm,D i m omys removal; "SP", pulsed seed additions. Those species associated with more open microhabi tats included the large kangaroo rat, Dipodomys spectabilis, and the smallest species, Perognathus ffavus.The remaining species used the two microhabitats more indiscriminantly with the exception that Peromyscus maniculatus was captured more frequently in high-cover microsites in the D. spectabilis removal treatment. Figures 5 and 6 and table 2 show our experiments were of the kind and were of sufficient intensity to promote community-wide changes in the use of microhabitats by all species; only the Onychomys showed significant seasonal shifts in microhabitat use (captured more frequently in higher-cover areas during the fall than in the other seasons). Using the control treatment as a reference point showed that the majority of species shifted their use of microhabitats on plots where D. spectabilis was experimentally removed. These shifts, involving eight of the nine species present, included an increase in the use of microsites with less-than-median cover by D. merriami, P. pencillatus, P. ffavus,and N. albigula, and an increase in the use of high-cover sites by P. maniculatus, P.eremicus, R. megalotis, and D. ordii. The remaining manipulations registered fewer and less dramatic shifts: i.e., increased use of open microhabitats by P. pencillatus and P. maniculatus on constant seed addition plots; and shifts towards highercover microsites by R. megalotis and P. pencillatus in ant removal and Dipodomys removal treatments, respectively. The role of microhabitat in the organization of this community can be further evaluated by comparing the distribution of trap captures for all species with what is available at trap stations (fig. 7). The objective was to determine whether certain types of microhabitats are used by the rodent community more frequently than others. This analysis shows that the distribution of captures in control, D. spectabilis removal, and Dipodomys removal plots all differ significantly from that expected if the use of microhabitats was random with respect to vegetative cover (KolmogorovSrnirnov two sample test; P < 0.05). However, there are characteristic ways these differ from expected. On control plots there were fewer than expected rodent captures in traps having < 5% cover; on D. spectabilis removal plots there were a greaterthan-expected number of captures for this same cover category; and on Dipodomys removal plots most rodents were captured at trap stations with > 10 % cover. Discussion Our results identify species interactions as the principal factor producing structure in this community. It is significant that, by adding supplemental seeds or removing ants, we were able to change microhabitats used by only a few of the species but removing a large, potentially dominant competitor produced many shifts. This suggests that the primary mode of interaction, as it effects the patterns of microhabitat use in this community, involves the direct responses of rodent species to each other rather than interactions mediated through the exploitation of food resources, or the individualistic responses of rodents to particular microhabita t types. The results point to the importance of one dominant species, D. spectabilis, whose presence in the community plays a disproportionate role in determining which microhabitats are utilized by the other species, and thus the organization of the community as a whole. Whenever it is present, regardless of how much food is available, it appears to relegate the majority of other rodent species to higher-than-median cover habitats, thereby reducing the density of potential competitors in the open habitats it prefers. A notable exception is Perognathus flavus which was captured in open sites along with D. spectabilis. Because of its small size (ca. 7 g) and low population density, P. flavus may have only a negligible impact on the food resources that can be harvested by D. spectablilis and, therefore, may not compete directly with or be subjected to its aggressive behavior. The importance of such size-ratio thresholds in allowing species to coexist has been discussed (Ebwers and Brown 1982). Defending open areas from other rodents may be a mechanism by which D. spectabilis is able to preempt food resources for its exclusive use. Supporting evidence for this comes from other research at our study site where it was found that Treatment Figure 6.-Distribution of captures in the two microhabitat categories for the six Cricetid rodents listed by treatment and season. See legend to figure 5 for more details. experimental seeds placed in open microhabitats remained largely unharvested when D. spectabilis was present but quickly disappeared in plots where it was removed (see Bowers et al. 1987). Our results also infer something about the mechanism by which D. 1 spectabilis affects the use of space by other rodent species in the community. Competition can be mediated through two processes: (i) exploitative interactions where species interact through a shared resource base; or (ii) contest interactions involving aggressive dominance and relegation CONTROL rc-----i ----, Lo,,, REMOVAL PERCENT COVER Figure 7.-Distribution of trap captures (broken line; all species combined) and available trap sites (sold line) relative to vegetative cover on (i) control; (it) 0. spectaMlis removal; and (iii) Dipodomys removal plots. to suboptimal areas and resources. For exploitation alone to account for the patterns of microhabitat use, D. spectabilis, through its foraging, would have to significantly alter the distribution of food (seed) resources among the microhabitats in ways that are ecologically significant for the other species. This is unlikely for several reasons. First, many of the seeds utilized by the smaller species appear to be too small to be econornically harvestable by D. spectatnlis (see Bowers et al. 1987).Second, many of the species showing significant microhabitat shifts were non-granivores (i.e., Neotoma), and hence, should be relatively insensitive to changes in the resource base attributable to the foraging of D. spectabilis. Third, adding seeds should have made food more available to all species and reduced the degree to which D. spectabilis was able to alter the distribution of food resources, so that shifts by the other species would have been expected in response to this treatment. Moreover, significant changes in the distribution of food resources were more likely to have been caused by D. merriami that occurs at higher densities than D. spectabilis. Our results show that adding supplemental seeds or removing D.merriami produced fewer shifts than removing just D. spectabilis. As an alternative to exploitation, competitors of large body size may directly restrict the foraging activities of smaller species through interference. Under an interference mode of competition adding seeds may not alter the intensity or outcome of the interaction. Because most significant shifts in microhabitat use occurred in the D. spectabilis removal treatmentcoupled with the fact that adding seeds had little effect on the patterns of microhabitat-leads us to the conclusion that aggressive interference by D. spectabilis is the mechanism most consistent with our results. Our study also indicates that the majority of shifts in microhabitat use originate with the D. spectabilis-D. merriami interaction and, at the community level, this one interaction affects the microhabitat utilization of the majority of rodent species through a complex network of direct and indirect interactions. Perhaps the most striking shift (not in the magnitude of response but in the number of individuals involved) was the increased use of open areas by the numerically dominant D. merriami when D. spectabilis, which had formally used these sites was removed. Mast other shifts by the smaller r e dents, including the increased use of open microhabi tats by Perognathus flavus, Peromyscus maniculatus and Reithrodontomys megalotis when all Dipodomys were removed, suggest that these species responded directly to D. merriami and only indirectly to D.spectabilis. Hence, there appears to be a hierarchy of interactions. The primary one is between the behavioral ID. spectabilis) and numerical ID. merriami) dominants and it is this interaction around which the community is organized. Other studies have noted the potential for interference between desert rodents (Blaustein and Riser 1974, Hutto 1978, Rebar and Conley 19831, especially between D. spectabilis and D. merriami (Frye 19831, and our study shows how this one interaction can resound throughout the community to affect many other species. A primary motivation for our study-and most studies focusing on the role of habitat-is that microhabitats represent a limited and exploitable resource and the manner in which they are used directly impinges on population growth and density. Many of the experimentally induced microhabitat shifts we have reported were accompanied by changes in local species density (Brown and Munger 1985, Brown et al. 1986) that support the contention that D.spectabilis controls the dynamics of this community through a combination of direct and indirect effects. For example, increasing food levels by adding seeds resulted in an in- crease of D ,spectabilis and a decrease in D. merriami densities. Removal of D. spectabilis resulted in positive density compensation of D. merriami but no changes in densities of the smaller seed-eaters; removal of all Dipodomys, however, resulted in large density increases in several of the smaller rodents. Taken together, the microhabitat and density responses to our manipulations indicate that interference competition for certain foraging sites not only determines the spatial organization of this community but that it is directly involved in the regulation of rodent densities. There are several aspects that warrant further comment. First, our results show that when D. spectabilis is present open sites are underutilized by the community as a whole; when D. spectabilis is removed the remaining Dipodomys shift to use these open sites; but when all Dipodomys are removed the remaining species are unable to fully utilize the vacated microhabitats (fig. 7). Hence, there appears to be a limit to how far the community can compensate for the absence of certain species. Among the possible explanations for this might be that assemblages of desert rodents have been associating together for a sufficient time to have lost the flexibility to respond to situations where one or more of the species are absent (Schroder and Rosenzweig 1975).Another is that quadrupedal species may have a limited ability to avoid predators in open microhabitats and this limits the degree to which they can compensate when the bipeds are removed. In either case the relaxation of one factor (in this case the removal of dominant competitors) appears to be accompanied by the increased importance of others. Second, the effects of interference competition by D. spectabilis appear to be effective in excluding interspecifics primarily in open areas although this dominant does occur in greater-than-median cover habitats. It may be that aggression is of lim- ited value in bushy microsites where subdominant species may readily find refugia. As a result, I>. spectabilis may be involved in two kinds of interactions with each of its competitors; exploitatively for seeds in bushy sites and through interference in open microhabitats. As a result, the highly asymmetrical interactions between the dominant/subordinates in open sites may become more nearly symmetrical in bushy sites where premiums are on foraging efficiency. Third, the existence of strong, aggressive interactions among species increases the potential for indirect and high-order interactions that involve species that overlap very little in resource utilization. For example, the large herbivore, Neotoma albigula was as likely to shift its microhabitat use as the granivorous species. However, it is interesting to note that although the non-granivores shifted microhabitat use when granivorous species were removed, significant density changes were limited to just other granivores (Brown and Munger 1985). Hence, while interference may play a role in determining use of microhabitats by rodents in several foraging guilds, its effects appear to be most significant for ecologically similar species. The goal of experimental programs is to hold most variables constant while manipulating others, and then to measure for shifts in response variables. In this paper we have used patterns of microhabitat use in control plots as a reference point for interpreting our experimental results. The assumption in doing this is that the degree to which the community responds to a particular manipulation provides an estimate of its importance in producing the basic pattern. In our particular case we wanted to know how the baseline patterns of microhabitat use (i.e., those in control plots) change when supplemental food is added or species are removed. While some of our patterns are easy to interpret, others are very complex and appear to in- volve a hierarchy of responses that operate over different scales in time and space. The existence of such a dynamic and diverse set of responses shows the limitations of most twospecies models of interspecific interactions upon which past theories of community organization have largely been based; they also call into question the value of studies seeking to understand the mechanistic processes that determine community composition through comparative, nonexperimental methods. Implications for Management While the spatial association of small mammals with particular microhabitats has been rigorously and repeatedly documented, and the patterns suggest almost a universal role of microhabi ta t in "structuring" small mammal communities, the processes responsible for producing these associations are poorly understood (Price and Brown 1983, Bowers 1986).To successfully manage/manipulate such communities there is a clear need to better understand the processes that determine which microhabitats are used and which are not. Towards this end we identify two particularly relevant areas for our discussion: (1)the scales in time and space over which microhabitat use occurs; and (2) the roles of correla tion, and selection/relegation in the occupancy of microhabitats. Vagile organisms, e.g. small mammals, can potentially respond to features of the habitat at several different scales. At the macro-end of the habitat spectrum animals choose areas in which to establish home ranges. Microhabitat selection, in contrast, usually involves the use/ disuse of small areas within the home range. There are also temporal differences in schedules of usage: macrohabitat selection occurs over a much longer timescale (weeksmonths) while microhabitat use occurs more immediately (seconds- minutes). While it was assumed for years that macrohabitat selection occurred through the selection of composite microhabitats, recent work on small mammals suggests that the two may be largely separate (Morris 1987). Most factors that are demonstrably important to the structure of small mammal communities, i.e., primary productivity, plant species and foliage height diversity, vegetation cover, substrate type, competitor diversity and abundance, and predatory pressure, vary more between macrohabitats than among microhabitats within particular locales. For example, primary productivity and plant cover are determined by plant species composition and general conditions for growth that vary over large environmental gradients at the macrohabitat scale. These large scale gradients influence patterns of microhabitat use by determining which rodent species are present, their densities, the distribution and abundance of food resources, and the types of microhabitats that are available for selection. As a consequence, the composition, densities and demographical behavior of small mammal populations and communities may more closely reflect habitat variability at the macro-rather than the micro-scale. On the other hand, microhabitat usage is a phenomena involving choices of individuals. Microhabitats that, by definition, vary over scales smaller than individual home ranges, have significance for the survivorship or reproduction of foraging individuals, but may have little relevance when integrated over the population as a whole. Most experimental studies examining the role of microhabitat in structuring small mammal communities tend to confound micro- and macrohabitat effects. Typically, manipulations (e.g., food addition, species removal, tailoring of vegetation) are applied at the level of the rnacrohabitat with microhabitat usage by individuals measured as a response variable. The research reported here suffers from such a confounding. Other field experiments that examine the allocation of foraging time among patches restrict manipulations to the level of microhabitats (Kotler 1984, Price and Waser 19851, and are not confused by responses of entire populations. Clearly, the time has come to utilize the information we now have to design comprehensive studies that distinguish between micro- and macrohabitat selection: i.e, studies that manipulate certain microhabitats on a scale over which populations might respond. Correctly gauging the scale over which species respond to the environmental mosaic is critical to the successful management of that species. Programs aimed at managing species by manipulating microhabitats may or may not be successful depending on the scale at which the manipulation is applied. If the goal is to manage populations then rnacrohabitat may be the correct context for the program. This is not to suggest that microhabitat is an inappropria te context for management programs. What it does suggest is that management oriented programs should be directed towards populations rather than the behavior of individuals. In many cases this may involve changing the focus from the micro to macro level. Our second point for discussion involves habitat correlation versus selection/relegation. Habitat usage is determined by the habitats available, the tolerances/preferences of organisms for these habitats, and the among-habitat variability in fitness. Clearly, there must be some variability in the structure of the habitat in order for selection to occur. Habitats that are relatively homogeneous at the smaller scales may not exhibit habitat associations even by highly selective species. Conversely, showing that a habitat has a significant degree of microhabitat variability does not imply that organisms have the ability or inclination to respond to that variability. In order to apply the patterns of microhabitat use from one site to predict what is occurring at another requires an understanding of the biological factors underlying microhabitat use. Achieving this has proved difficult because of several problems. First, it is clear from a growing body of experimental work (including the present study) that habitat association does not necessarily imply habitat selection. Because microhabitats are rarely discrete, usually grade from one type to another, and involve a suite of factors that either characterize or are correlated with specific microhabitats, it is rare that habitat occupancy can be tied to a single factor. As a result it is difficult to conclude that an animal is selecting a habitat per se, some feature of that habitat, or some factor that is only correlated with that microhabitat. As a complicating factor habitat selection probably reflects integrated responses of organisms to maximize fitness relative to several largely independent processes. For example, animals might select microhabitats so as to minimize predatory risk, or food encounter rates, or to jointly maximize food intake while minimizing predatory risk (Bowers 1987). Second, the present results and those of others (Price 1978, MfCloskey 1978, Wondolleck 1978, Bowers et al. 1987) show that microhabitat provides a template over which species interactions and competitive hierarchies become expressed. The pattern is one of selection/ relegation-the competitive dominant selecting its preferred microhabitat and through exploitative or interference competition relegating other species to less preferred sites. The more ecologically similar two species-and hence, the greater the intensity of competition between them-the greater the potential role of interspecific competition in determining microhabitat usage. Competitive interactions represent dynamical processes impinging on microhabitat association and usage. Seasonal or year-to-year fluxes in resource availability or changes in the distribution of resources among microhabitats can a1ter the economical basis underlying competitive interactions, and thereby promote shifts in microhabitat usage. For example, Congdon (1974) found during periods of low resource availability that the large D.deserti and the smaller, D. rnerriarni, coexisted in the same microhabitats but that the former became aggressive and excluded the latter from these sites when food levels increased. Similarly, Frye (1983) found that D. spectabilis excluded D. rnerriarni from areas around its burrows just in the fall when seeds from summer annuals were abundant. Competitively based selection/ relegation has the effect of increasing usage of secondary habitats while decreasing usage of the most preferred ones. The result is that competition promotes the segregation of species among microhabitats and the degree to which the community is spatially organized. Thus it is no accident that the most striking patterns of microhabitat use and segregation are in communities that are highly competitive (Connor and Bowers 1987). As the present study has demonstrated even one strong interaction involving just two species (in this case the behavioral and numerical dominants) can affect microhabitat usage by all species in the community through direct and indirect pathways of interaction. Care must be taken when examining the spatial organization of communities where competition might be occurring. Efforts to understand microhabitat utilization through reconstitution studies that measure individual species preferences for microhabitats, then combines these in a general model of microhabitat associa tion, will miss higher-order competitive effects that may be the main determinants of microhabitat use. Further, since competition can be indeterminate, work over complex pathways, and operate over widely varying scales in time and space it is doubtful that any one model can be used to predict microhabitat use over all communities. As a first step towards using microhabitat utilization as a tool for management programs we need to know which communities are interactive (i.e., structured around selection/relegation schemes), which are non-interactive, and something about ecological attributes of each. It may be that in some communities microhabitat is the correct context for management programs while in other communities the focus should be on species interactions. Species removal experiments such as the one described here provide a straightforward test of these models. What we are suggesting here is that microhabitat use be viewed as a manifestation of process and that these processes provide the basis for management. We feel that the most important question is not which habitats are being used by a particular species but why it is using that microhabitat and not others. Recent work has shown that the pathways by which species interact at the level of ecological communities can be very complex and that similar patterns of microhabitat usage need not share a common sequence of causation (see papers in Diamond and Case 1986). Without knowing something about which processes are locally important it is risky to extrapolate findings from one site in managing another. For example, Bowers (1986) found in rarefaction studies of the same three species rodent community that microhabitat use at one site was affected by interspecific competition but not at two others. Such results underscore the fact that microhabitat use involves multidimensional responses of organisms to their environment. Understanding the basics of such relationships should be the goal of community ecologists and managers alike. Acknowledgments We thank J.H. Brown and D.B. Thompson for help in the field and for discussion. J.H. Brown and R.T. M'Closkey provided critical reviews of the manuscript. Literature Cited Blaustein, Andrew R., and Arthur C. Riser. 1974. Dominance relationships of the dark kangaroo mouse (Microdipodops megacephalus) and the little pocket mouse (Perognathus longimembris) in captivity. Great Basin Naturalist 34:312-314. Bowers, Michael A. 1982. Foraging behavior of heteromyid rodents: field evidence of resource partitioning. Journal of Mammalogy 63:361-367. Bowers, Michael A. 1986. Geographic comparison of microhabitats used by three heteromyid in response to rarefaction. Journal of Mammalogy 67:46-52. Bowers, Michael A. 1987. Seed removal experiments on desert rodents: the microhabitat by moonlight effect. Journal of Mammalogy 69:201-204. Bowers, Michael A., and James H. Brown. 1982. Coexistence of desert rodents: Chance or community structure? Ecology 63:391-400. Brown, James H., Diane W. Davidson, James C. Munger, and Richard S. Inouye. 1986. Experimental community ecology: the desert granivore system. p. 41-61. In (J. Diamond and T.J. Case, eds.) Community ecology, Harper and Row, New York. Brown, James H., and James C. Munger. 1985. Experimental manipulation of a desert rodent community: food addition and species removal. Ecology 66:1545-1563. Brown, James H., O.J. Reichman, and Diane W. Davidson. 1979. Granivory in desert ecosystems. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 10:201-227. Bowers, Michael A., Daniel B. Thompson, and James H. Brown. 1987. Spatial organization of a desert rodent community: food addition and species removal. Oecologia 72:77-82. Colwell, Robert K., and Eduardo R. Fuentes. 1975. Experimental studies of the niche. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 6:281310. Congdon, Justin. 1974. Effect of habitat quality on distributions of three sympatric species of desert rodents. Journal of Mammalogy 55:659-662. Connor, Edward F., and Michael A. Bowers. 1987. The spatial consequences of interspecific competition. Annales Zoologici Fennici 24:213-226. Davidson, Diane W., Douglas A. Sampson, and Richard S. Inouye. 1985. Granivory in the Chihuahuan Desert: interactions within and between trophic levels. Ecology 66:486-502. Diamond, Jared, and T.J. Case. 1986. Community Ecology. Harper and Row, New York, New York. 663 p. Fretwell, Steven D., and H.L. Lucas, Jr. 1970. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. I. Theoretical development. Acta Biotheoretica 19:16-36. Frye, Robert J. 1983. Experimental field evidence of interspecific aggression between two species of kangaroo rats (Dipodomys). Oecologia 59:74-78. Harris, John H. 1984. An experimental analysis of desert rodent foraging ecology. Ecology 65:1579-1584. Hay, Mark E., and P.J. Fuller. 1981. Seed escape from heteromyid rodents: the importance of microhabita t and seed preference. Ecology 62:1395-1399. Hutto, Richard L. 1978. A mechanism for resource allocation among sympatric heteromyid rodent species. Oecologia 33:ll5-126. Kotler, Burt P. 1984. Risk of predation and the structure of desert rodent communities. Ecology 65689-701. Lemen, Cliff A., and Patricia W. Freeman. 1986. Interference competition in a heteromyid community in the Great Basin of Nevada, USA. Okios 463390-396. Levins, Richard. 1962. Theory of fitness in a heterogeneous environment. I. The fitness set and adaptive function. American Naturalist 96:361-373. M'Closkey, Robert T. 31978. Niche separation and assembly in four species of Sonoran Desert rodents. American Naturalist 112:683-694. Morris, Douglas W. 1987. Ecological scale and habitat use. Ecology 68:362-369. Munger, James C., Michael A. BOWers, and W. Thomas Jones. 1983. Desert rodent populations: factors affecting abundance, distribution, and genetic structure. Great Basin Naturalist Memiors 7:91-116. Price, Mary V. 1978. The role of microhabita t in structuring desert rodent communities. Ecology 59:910-921. Price, Mary V. 1983. Ecological consequences of body size: a model for patch choice in desert rodents. Oecologia 59:384-392. Price, Mary V., and James H. Brown. 1983. Patterns of morphology and resource use in North American desert rodent communities. Great Basin Natur. Memiors 7:ll7-IM. Price, Mary V., and Nickolas M. Waser. 1985. Microhabitat use by heteromyid rodents: effects of artificial seed patches. Ecology 66211219. Rebar, Cindy, and Walt Conley. 1983. Interactions in microhabitat use between Dipodomys ordii and Onychomys leucopster. Ecology 642394-899. Reichman, O.J. 1981. Factors influencing foraging in desert rodents. p. 195-213.In (A.C. Kamil and T.D. Sargent, eds.)Foraging behavior: ecological, ethological and psychological approaches. Garland STPM, New York. Rosenzweig, Michael L. 1982. A theory of habitat selection. Ecology 62~327-335. Rosenzweig, Michael L., and Jerald Winakur. 1969. Population ecology of desert rodent communities: habitats and environmental complexity. Ecology 50:558-572. Schoener, Thomas W. 1971. The theory of foraging strategies. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 2369-484. Schroder, Gene, and Michael L. Rosenzweig. 1975. Perturbation analysis of competition and overlap in habitat utilization between Dipodomys ordii and Dipodomys merriami. Oecologia 19:9-28. Siegel, Sidney 1956. Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. McGraw-Hill, New York. Wondolleck, John T. 1978. Foragearea separation and overlap in heteromyid rodents. Journal of Mammalogy 59:5lO-518.