Mothers versus others: Caregiving experience influences perception of infant vocalizations Rachel Albert, Lily Molik, Haley Roenneburg, Jena VanderLogt, Emily Lindberg, & Rebecca Pletka University of Wisconsin-Stevens Point Background Caregivers’ responses to infant babbling have long-term effects on infant language learning. • Caregivers consistently provide feedback to infants’ prelinguistic vocalizations (Goldstein & West, 1999). • Maternal responsiveness to vocalizations in the first two years predicts later language development (TamisLeMonda, Bornstein, & Baumwell, 2001). What factors influence caregivers’ responding? • Studies on adults’ perceptions of cries have found experience related differences between parent and nonparents (e.g. Green, Jones, & Gustafson, 1987). • Mothers rate the qualities of babbles and respond differently to infant vocalizations than non-mothers (Albert, Schwade, & Goldstein, under review; See Figure 2 for replication). • Caregiving experience appears to be one factor that influences when and how caregivers respond to infant vocalizations. What is the effect of caregiving experience on nonmothers’ responsiveness to babbling? • Experienced non-mothers (i.e. daycare workers) and inexperienced non-mothers rated and responded to infant vocalizations via a playback paradigm to assess the influence of caregiving experience on responding. Method Summary of Findings Participants: 30 female participants (mean age=20.63 years, range=18-25) were classified as inexperienced (n=15) or experienced caregivers (n=15). Experienced caregivers were currently employed at a daycare working with children under age 2 (mean=12.5 hrs/wk, range = 3-40 hrs/wk). Procedure: Participants provided an in-the-moment vocal response to 80 audio-visual examples of infant vocalizations (Figure 1). They also rated each vocalization on a 7-point Likert ‘speechiness’ scale with ‘1’ being least speech-like and ‘7’ being most speech-like. Stimuli: Each stimulus consisted of a 7 second video of an unfamiliar 9-month-old infant coupled with a prelinguistic vocalization. Infants were in the act of playing and vocalizing. The stimulus clips were created by recombining audio and visual images of infant behavior on two dimensions: Infraphonology (Table 1) and Directedness (Figure 1). Data Analysis: The mean speechiness rating for each infraphonological syllable type was calculated for each participant. Participants’ vocal responses were coded into one of five categories (Table 2). Ah Ah Ba Object Directed Vocalizations • Caregiving experience does not appear to significantly influence non-mothers perceptions of infant vocalizations. Ba Undirected Vocalizations Figure 1. Sample images of stimulus clips. Paired clips allowed for assessing the effects of the four syllable types, and the effects of changing the direction of the vocalization (object directed, undirected). Syllabus Type Description Example Quasi-resonant vowel (QRV) Vowel produced with a closed vocal tract, resulting in nasalized vocalizations [grunt] Fully-resonant vowel (FRV) Vowel produced with an open vocal tract [ah] Marginal Syllable (MS) Slow sequences of consonant–vowel articulation [bwaa] Canonical Syllable (CS) Fast consonant-vowel transitions with a FRV [ba] Table 1. Prelinguistic vocalizations were categorized by their acoustic properties using an infraphonological coding system (Oller, 2000). Speech Ratings Comparison of Inexperienced and Experienced Non-Mothers • Eliminates the personal interaction dynamics (i.e. idiosyncratic history) a caregiver has with a specific infant. The playback paradigm elicits natural responses. • Mothers show nearly identical patterns of responses whether responding to their own infant in a live play session or to videos of unfamiliar infants in the playback paradigm (Albert, Schwade, & Goldstein, under review). 2. Are the perceptual differences in speech ratings between mothers and non-mothers explained by age more than caregiving experience? 4. Do similar perceptual differences exist between fathers and non-fathers? Figure 2. Mean speech maturity rating for each infraphonological type (±1 SE) by parity group. * p <.05. Note: Mother data (green) from Albert, Schwade, & Goldstein (under review). Figure 3. Mean speech maturity rating for each infraphonological type (±1 SE) by experience group. Influences of Caregiving Experience on Non-Mothers’ Responses to Infant Vocalizations Benefits of using a playback paradigm: • All participants are exposed to the same set of highly controlled stimuli. • Currently testing a group of full-time daycare employees in infant classrooms. 3. Does childbirth reorganize women’s perceptions of infant vocalizations? What is a playback paradigm? • Playback paradigms are commonly used in non-human animal research to assess how specific visual or auditory behaviors of one animal influence the behavior of another. Future directions • Currently comparing an age-matched group of nonmothers and mothers. Background on Methodology: Playback Paradigm • A method used to expose participants to prerecorded stimuli and measure participants’ responses to each stimulus. Playback paradigm 1. Can we better define caregiving experience to determine how much exposure to infant vocalizations equates to experience? Results Comparison of Mothers and Non-Mothers • Caregiving experience does predict some differences in how non-mothers respond to infant vocalizations. For example, inexperienced non-mothers ask more questions than experienced non-mothers when responding to vowels. Response Type Definition Example Question Questions about the infant’s state or current actions What do you have? Description Statements that describe the infant, infant’s action, or objects near infant That’s a ball. Affirmation Words that hold a place in the conversation without providing new information Uh-huh, I know Play Prompt Imitation + Albert, R.R., Schwade, J.A., & Goldstein, M.H. (under review). Understanding parenting from the infant’s perspective: New methods reveal new influences on responsiveness to infant behavior. Goldstein, M.H., & West, M.J. (1999). Consistent responses of human mothers to prelinguistic infants: The effect of prelinguistic repertoire size. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 52-57. Green, J.A., Jones, L.E., & Gustafson, G.E. (1987). Perception of cries by parents and nonparents: Relation to cry acoustics. Developmental Psychology, 23, 370-382. Oller, D. K. (2000). The emergence of the speech capacity. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Prompts that guide an infant to perform an Roll me the ball! action Duplications of the infant’s vocalization References Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Bornstein, M.H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748-767. Baby: [ba]; Mom: [ba] Note: adapted from Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2001 Table 2. Coding system used to categorize caregiver speech. Figure 4. Mean proportion of vowel vocalizations that received each response type (+1 SE) by experience group. + p =.055. This research was supported by a UWSP UPDC Grant to RRA and UWSP URSCA travel grants to all presenters. Contact information: Rachel Albert University of WisconsinStevens Point ralbert@uwsp.edu