ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2006, 71, 481–490 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.011 REVIEWS Sexual cannibalism and mate choice JOHN PRENTER, CALUM MacNEIL & ROBERT W. ELWOOD School of Biology & Biochemistry, Queen’s University Belfast (Received 17 September 2004; initial acceptance 29 October 2004; final acceptance 7 May 2005; published online 13 February 2006; MS. number: RV-53R) Sexual cannibalism, where a female kills and consumes a courting male, represents an extreme form of sexual conflict and has been proposed as a mechanism of mate choice. We evaluate the evidence for mate choice through premating sexual cannibalism via mate rejection, other indirect mechanisms of mate ‘choice’ and choice in postmating sexual cannibalism. We highlight a paucity of investigations, particularly of field studies, and note gaps in our knowledge. There is empirical support for the size-dependent sexual cannibalism predicted by mate choice through premating sexual cannibalism. This may represent mate choice operating on absolute male size but it could be a by-product of female foraging behaviour and greater vulnerability of relatively smaller males. Thus, indirect mate choice is as plausible an explanation of sizedependent sexual cannibalism as is direct mate choice based on discrimination of male traits. Direct female choice, mediated through premating sexual cannibalism, has yet to be demonstrated. We suggest a framework for distinguishing direct and indirect choice and note an absence of information on which to test it. There is evidence for sequential mate choice in postmating sexual cannibalism, but the nature or basis of the female’s discriminatory behaviour remains unclear. Costs and long-term fitness benefits of the putative mate choice have been largely ignored. Reversed sexual cannibalism, in which the male eats the female, presumably occurs when the gain from food is high and potential gain from mating low and probably has little to do with mate choice. Ó 2005 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Mr. Blackwall has sometimes seen two or more males on the same web with a single female, but their courtship is too tedious and prolonged an affair to be easily observed. The male is extremely cautious in making his advances, as the female carries her coyness to a dangerous pitch. De Geer saw a male that ‘‘in the midst of his preparatory caresses was seized by her in a web and then devoured’’, a sight as he adds filled him with horror and ‘‘indignation’’. C. Darwin (1871). Sexual cannibalism, where females kill and consume potential or actual mates before, during or after mating, is common in spiders and occurs in some other invertebrates (Elgar 1992; Johns & Maxwell 1997; Schneider & Elgar 2002; Elgar & Schneider 2005; Huber 2005). It may represent the pinnacle of sexual conflict (reviewed in Chapman & Partridge 1996; Brown et al. 1997; Schneider & Lubin 1998; Elgar & Schneider 2005; Schneider et al. 2006; but see Andrade 1998). Males use various strategies to avoid being cannibalized (reviewed in Robinson 1982; Elgar 1992; Moya-Laraño et al. 2004), including waiting Correspondence: J. Prenter, School of Biology & Biochemistry, Queen’s University Belfast, Medical Biology Centre, 97 Lisburn Road, Belfast BT9 7BL, U.K. (email: j.prenter@qub.ac.uk). C. MacNeil is now at the Government Laboratory, Ballakermeen Road, Douglas, Isle of Man IM1 4BR, U.K. 0003–3472/05/$30.00/0 for the female to catch a fly before attempting to mate (e.g. Austin & Anderson 1978; Prenter et al. 1994a, b; Schneider et al. 2001; Fromhage & Schneider 2005) and mating with vulnerable, newly moulted females (e.g. Elgar 1992). The caution with which males court females and the tenaciousness of their postmating escape attempts suggest that sexual cannibalism is a strong selection force, even if the incidence is low (Morse 2004) and, from the male’s point of view, maladaptive (Foellmer & Fairbairn 2004); however, it has long been viewed as a potential mechanism for female mate choice (Darwin 1871; Elgar & Nash 1988; Eberhard 1996). We review the evidence for sexual cannibalism as a mate choice mechanism in premating sexual cannibalism (PRE-SC) and postcopulatory or postmating sexual cannibalism (POST-SC). We also briefly examine reversed sexual cannibalism, where courting males kill females (e.g. Dick 1995; Tsai & Dai 2003; Schütz & Taborsky 2005), and the possibility of male 481 Ó 2005 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 482 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 71, 3 mate choice in sexual cannibalism. While there is a paucity of studies that address the central issue of mate choice through sexual cannibalism, there is a need to examine support for the various hypotheses critically and also highlight gaps in knowledge that may serve as a strategic focus for further study. Most of our discussion will relate to spiders because of a bias in the literature. DIRECT AND INDIRECT MATE CHOICE Direct mate choice by sexual cannibalism is an outcome of a specific discrimination by females, often by size, since size is strongly correlated with many fitness characteristics (e.g. Blanckenhorn 2000), and may be inferred from decreased aggression towards certain males and increased aggression directed at others. Hebets (2003) showed that adult female wolf spiders, Schizocosa uetzi, preferred to mate with males of a familiar phenotype (subadult exposure). Unfamiliar males were more likely to be cannibalized by females. However, we have excluded this study from our survey of PRE-SC (see below, Table 1) because of the unnatural nature of the manipulation of male phenotypes undertaken. Whenever variation in a given male trait, for example size, is associated with variation in female behaviour, for example sexual cannibalism, and in male reproductive success, selection on the particular trait is expected. Differential sexual cannibalism, however, may not always involve direct female choice. We are thus faced with the dichotomy of ideas that represent direct and indirect mate choice (e.g. Darwin 1871; Kirkpatrick 1987; Andersson 1994; Wiley & Poston 1996). Indirect mate choice embraces all other behaviour or attributes that restrict the mates of females but without discrimination or active choice on the part of the female. MATE REJECTION Established criteria guide researchers investigating direct mate choice (e.g. Halliday 1983; Searcy & Andersson 1986; Andersson 1994; Eberhard 2000). Eberhard (1996) suggested that (1) females should respond differently to some conspecifics (i.e. discriminate between them), (2) the discrimination should occur under natural conditions, (3) discrimination by the female between different males Table 1. Summary of evidence for size-related precopulatory sexual cannibalism (PRE-SC) and mate choice Criteria for mate choice Species Differential sexual cannibalism and nature Other female discrimination 1 2 3 4 5 Source Y N Y(?) Y ? Elgar & Nash 1988 Y N Y(?) Y ? Arnqvist 1992; Arnqvist & Henriksson 1997 N N N N ? Johnson 2001, 2005b Y Y Y(?) Y ? Rubenstein 1987 Y N ? Y(?) ? Persons & Uetz 2005 N N N N Y N ? ? ? ? ? N ? ? ? Uhl & Vollrath 1998 Uhl & Vollrath 1998 Elgar & Fahey 1996 ? Y ? ? ? Araneus diadematus Smaller males cannibalized Dolomedes fimbriatus Larger males avoided PRE-SC Dolomedes triton ND Metellina segmentata Schizocosa ocreata Smaller males cannibalized* Smaller males more likely to be attacked and cannibalized Nephila edulis Nephila clavipes Nephila plumipes ND ND ND Micrathena gracilis Gasteracantha minax Gasteracantha cancriformis Misumenia vatia N/A Males with higher pseudocopulation rates less likely to suffer PRE-SC ND; larger males better at gaining copulations (once mounted) ND; larger males escape attack more often but not translated into differential reproductive success Only larger males mate multiply Attack smaller males (with smaller leg tufts), probability of PRE-SC varied with relative size of pair; males with larger leg tufts more likely to mate N/A N/A Females responded to larger males and ignored smaller males but no data on attack behaviour N/A ND N/A N N ? N ? Bukowski & Christenson 2000y Elgar & Bathgate 1996 N/A N/A ? Y ? ? ? Bukowski et al. 2001y ND ND N ? ? ? ? Morse 2004z See text for explanation of criteria for mate choice (based on Eberhard 1996). Y ¼ yes, criteria met; N ¼ no, criteria not met; ? ¼ uncertain/no available data; Y(?) ¼ probable; ND ¼ none detected; N/A ¼ not applicable/data not available. *Cannibalism by females of adult males during male–male fights. yStudy focused on male behaviour and sperm competition. zDespite a lack of empirical evidence, Morse (2004) suggested that a size bias in cannibalized males is likely in M. vatia. REVIEW should result in differential reproductive success for males, (4) the female bias should be associated with specific male morphological or behavioural trait(s), and (5) the variation among males in characteristics used by females to discriminate should be heritable. Evidence that, in spiders, females respond differently to certain conspecific males and that discrimination is based on some measurable morphological or behavioural trait is available for only four species. Elgar (1992) regarded reported sexual cannibalism of smaller males in Metellina segmentata (Rubenstein 1987) as anecdotal, presumably because of the lack of formal statistical analysis. However, using data from the original paper (Figure 9 in Rubenstein 1987), we confirmed that smaller males were cannibalized at a significantly greater frequency than larger males (number cannibalized: small 11/25; large 0/15; Fisher’s exact probability test: P ¼ 0.003). In Araneus diadematus, females were more likely to cannibalize small males (Elgar & Nash 1988) and those that engaged in lower premating pseudocopulation rates (mock copulation without sperm transfer). However, pseudocopulation rate was not correlated with male size and without this association it is difficult to see how selecting for high rates would lead to small males being cannibalized preferentially. The basis of the differential cannibalism in these two species, therefore, remains unclear and we cannot determine direct or indirect mate choice. Female Schizocosa ocreata preferred to mate with males with enlarged secondary sexual characters (leg tufts) but sexual cannibalism was not related to this feature (Persons & Uetz 2005). Female fishing spiders, Dolomedes fimbriatus, did not differ in their attack behaviour towards males (Arnqvist 1992; Andrade 1998); the attack success of females, however, was negatively related to male size because larger males escaped or avoided capture. This seems to be a clear case of indirect mate choice. Premating sexual cannibalism has been noted in various other species but without evidence of either direct or indirect mate choice (Table 1). Returning to the remainder of Eberhard’s (1996) criteria, confirmation of PRE-SC resulting in differential mating success under natural conditions is restricted, reflecting a continued (Elgar 1992) paucity of field studies on sexual cannibalism (Table 1). Obtaining evidence that fulfils the criterion of female discrimination resulting in differential reproductive success for males is more troublesome. In so far as PRE-SC limits the lifetime reproductive success of cannibalized males the criterion is fulfilled; however, the actual reproductive success of surviving and mated males is often not available. Furthermore, information on the heritability and long-term fitness benefits (e.g. Elgar & Nash 1988) of the outcomes of female mate choices (preferences) is also absent. The mate rejection, female mate choice hypothesis for PRE-SC remains to be rigorously or specifically tested experimentally; most of the evidence for or against it comes from wider studies of courtship, mating or sexual cannibalism. ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE SIZE If an apparent discrimination of smaller males is simply a by-product of the relative physical attributes of males and females (Fox 1975; Polis 1981), then the idea of sexual selection resulting from female preferences must be rejected. Indirect mate choice may then be the explanation for the size-dependent nature of PRE-SC. Both mechanisms would result in selection on male size since larger males survive the selection event better than smaller males. The two models, however, make very different assumptions about the ability of females to discriminate between males (Johnson 2001), and the behaviour of males and females is crucial in distinguishing between them. Females attacking courting males and males attempting to escape are two distinct facets of PRE-SC. Information about the aggressive tendencies of females towards courting males is essential and will be a major factor in unravelling the causes of sexual cannibalism and the role of mate choice. If females attack all males, for example as in D. fimbriatus (Arnqvist 1992; Arnqvist & Henriksson 1997), then size-dependent sexual cannibalism is probably the by-product of indirect mate choice. That female attack success, although not attack behaviour, varies with male size in D. fimbriatus suggests that larger males are better able to avoid/escape the female’s attacks. We should ask whether females are consistent in the size class of males rejected (although variation in choice criteria might be expected under variation in environmental circumstances, for example Elgar & Nash 1988; Newman & Elgar 1991) or whether the rejection criteria vary with the difference in size between the interacting pair. If sexual cannibalism varies with the relative size of the interacting males and females, then we propose that direct mate choice may be ruled out as an explanation of PRE-SC. Thus, the outcome of courtship interactions (cannibalism or mating) would be determined by the relative vulnerability of the male to the predatory advances of the larger female. However, if females are consistent in their choice of males for cannibalistic attack and mate with the others (at least under similar environmental conditions), and the absolute size of males determines the outcome of courtship interactions, then direct mate choice is a more likely explanation. The distinction between the effects of absolute size and relative size may, therefore, be crucial in the determination of direct or indirect mate choice via PRE-SC. The data necessary to distinguish between these hypothesis, however, are rarely available. When sexual size dimorphism (ratio of female to male size) was low in D. triton, males were more likely to escape attacks from females and go on to mate successfully (Johnson 2005b). Relative size of the pair (along with female body condition) determined the probability of successful PRE-SC in S. ocreata (Persons & Uetz 2005), although PRE-SC did not represent a mechanism of mate choice. The paucity of information about the effects of relative size on PRE-SC is in stark contrast to the plethora of studies on aggressive behaviour and assessment spawned by game theory, where relative size and its effects are commonly investigated (Taylor & Elwood 2003). Relatively small females might be incapable of making and imposing a choice because they do not possess the ability to reject all unpreferred males. Males may be able to repel attacks and engage in a war of attrition, ultimately resulting in the female abandoning her attacks. As 483 484 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 71, 3 a consequence, a female attempting to select males actively might show cannibalism based on relative size, even though the choice is based on absolute size. Effectively, small females may not be capable of rejecting many males, but larger females may have a greater opportunity to make a choice through sexual cannibalism. Even in this case, our prediction about different female behaviour, with regard to absolute and relative size of males and females, should still hold. A more formal theoretical approach might be needed, however, to consider the outcome of the hypothesized war of attrition. Examination of female motivation to attack and capture males, rather than actual ability to do so (which may be constrained by various factors including size-related male escape and female capture abilities) might prove a profitable approach to the investigation of the tendency of females to cannibalize different courting males. An interruption technique has been successfully used to probe the motivation of invertebrates in aggressive encounters (Elwood et al. 1998) and could be adapted to the problem of sexual cannibalism. Owing to the lack of information about the effects of relative or absolute size of the interacting pair on the occurrence of sexual cannibalism, the theory that PRE-SC forms a mechanism of direct mate choice must be treated with caution. Mate choice in the classical/direct sense, where females actively select males on the basis of some morphological or behavioural characteristic, remains to be demonstrated clearly with regard to PRE-SC. There can be little doubt, however, that indirect mate choice may arise via sexual cannibalism on males that are small relative to females, resulting in size-dependent patterns in PRE-SC, as a consequence of female foraging strategies, and that both scenarios will produce selection for increased size in males to avoid sexual cannibalism. POSTCOPULATORY SEXUAL CANNIBALISM Males are generally expected to be under stronger selection to control fertilization than are females, because their potential loss from failing to fertilize any eggs is greater than that for females restricted to mating with males of inferior quality (Parker 1984; Stockley 1997; Simmons & Siva-Jothy 1998). However, the polygamous nature of many mating systems (but see Fromhage et al. 2005), and the resulting possibility of sperm competition, offers an opportunity for females to manipulate and control paternity. POST-SC offers a similar potential for females to influence copulation duration, sperm transfer and paternity. Eberhard (1996) proposed a modification to the mate rejection hypothesis for cannibalism occurring during or after copulation, that the termination of copulation by initiating sexual cannibalism allows females to control fertilization of their eggs. Here, sexual cannibalism forms a postcopulatory mate choice mechanism, by which females can influence which males father their offspring. The specific conditions that must be met before cryptic choice can be determined are controversial (Birkhead 1998, 2000; Birkhead & Møller 1998; Eberhard 2000; Pitnick & Brown 2000). Paramount among the prerequisites, in terms of POST-SC, are the requirements that (1) variation in paternity is attributable to a male characteristic (e.g. size or specific behaviour) and (2) females are able to control this variation in paternity by varying the onset of cannibalism (Andrade 1998). In simple terms, preferred males may be allowed to copulate for longer before cannibalism is initiated or if cannibalism occurs and these males will transfer more sperm and should, therefore, sire more offspring. Crucially, this argument depends on the mechanism of sperm transfer used and hinges on a positive relation between copulation duration and sperm transferred. This positive relation is known for some species (e.g. Schneider et al., 2006) but not for others (Bukowski et al. 2001; Snow & Andrade 2004; see below). Satisfying the criterion that establishes female control of paternity, that some variation in paternity is attributable to the female, is often problematic, because it is difficult to separate the contributions of the two sexes to variation in paternity. Females often favour the same traits (e.g. large size) that provide males with advantages over their rivals. Certainly, if the female is to exert control of paternity by sexual cannibalism, cannibalism, copulation duration and paternity should be related (Schneider & Elgar 2001). In the past decade or so there have been significant advances in the study of POST-SC, especially from the standpoint of male adaptive (suicidal or sacrificial) behaviour in spiders (e.g. Forster 1992; Andrade 1996, 1998; Andrade et al. 2005). There has been little investigation of mate choice via POST-SC; however, studies on mating behaviour and sperm competition in sexually cannibalistic spiders (see Elgar 1998 for a review of sperm competition in arachnids) provide data that can be evaluated in this light (Table 2). Information is lacking, however, that would allow examination of female mate choice in mantids other than that there is potential for cryptic choice (Lawrence 1992). Where POST-SC has been studied, there is a lack of consistent evidence that differential POST-SC occurs. Where differential female behaviour has been identified, this may not translate into differential POST-SC or increased paternity for cannibalized males. POST-SC can either prolong or shorten the duration of copulation for cannibalized males compared to survivors (Table 2) and copulation duration may be an important determinant of fertilization success for males. While POST-SC was not discriminatory on the basis of any male trait or behaviour measured in the orb-weaving spiders Argiope aurantia (in which males invariably spontaneously expire after their second palpal intromission), Argiope bruennichi (Foellmer & Fairbairn 2003, 2004; Fromhage et al. 2003; but see Schneider et al. 2006) and Nephila fenestrata (Fromhage & Schneider 2005), it still resulted in increased copulation duration. Female mate choice may, therefore, be an unlikely explanation for POST-SC in these species. We must be cautious, however, in regarding increased copulation duration as a benefit of POST-SC. In species where males attempt to make two pedipal intromissions with a female (e.g. A. aurantia: Foellmer & Fairbairn 2003, 2004; A. bruennichi: Schneider et al. 2006), POST-SC during or after the first insertion may drastically shorten REVIEW Table 2. Summary of evidence for cryptic female choice through postcopulatory sexual cannibalism (POST-SC) Criteria for mate choice Species Argiope keyserlingi Differential sexual cannibalism and comment Females delayed onset of POST-SC on smaller second mates*; no evidence that small males escape POST-SC better Argiope ND; POST-SC in up to bruennichi 80% of matings Argiope Larger males with longer aurantia legs more likely to be attacked during first palpal insertion (but not translated into differential POST-SC)y Nephila Smaller males cannibalized plumipes less than larger males when mating with virgin (but not mated) females (but not translated to increased fertilization success) Nephila ND (but females attacked edulis larger males more often in first matings, while NS trend for cannibalized males to weigh less); mating advantage for smaller malesx Nephila ND, no correlation between fenestrata male mass, size or age with occurrence of cannibalism Latrodectus ND, no size, mass or hasselti condition differences detected between cannibalized and noncannibalized males, but males cannibalized on their second palpal insertion obtain longer copulations and higher paternity Latrodectus Small males cannibalized revivensis more when attempting second insemination into second spermatheca Schizocosa Smaller males (poorer condition ocreata with relatively small leg tufts) more likely to be cannibalized by larger (older) females Paternity attributable to male character? Female ability to control variation in male paternity? Effect of sexual cannibalism on copulation duration Source Y Y [ by delaying POST-SC* Elgar et al. 2000 N ? [ with POST-SC ? ? [ with POST-SC(?) (palpal insertion duration [ with cannibalism) Fromhage et al. 2003; Schneider et al. 2006 Foellmer & Fairbairn 2003, 2004 ? (But males cannibalized by nonvirgin females increased paternity compared to surviving males) ? Y(?)z [ with POST-SC Elgar & Fahey 1996; Schneider & Elgar 2001; Schneider et al. 2001 ? None Uhl & Vollrath 1998; Schneider et al. 2000; Elgar et al. 2003 N ? Fromhage & Schneider 2005 ? But increased paternity for cannibalized males Y(?) May control copulation duration with onset of POST-SC [ with POST-SC (but with no effect on paternity) [ with POST-SC ? ? ? B. Berendonck, personal communication ? ? ? Persons & Uetz 2005 Forster 1992; Andrade 1996, 2003; Andrade & Banta 2001, 2002; Snow & Andrade 2004 [ ¼ increased. See Table 1 for other abbreviations. *Applies only to second mates where two males were cannibalized (not to all males cannibalized). ySubject to statistical provisos with regard to multiple tests (effect not significant when corrected for multiple tests) males spontaneously die at the second palpal insertion in this species (Foellmer & Fairbairn 2004). zCannibalized males (mating with nonvirgin females) achieved longer copulations and increased paternity while not transferring more sperm; therefore females may make some form of sperm choice under these circumstances. xAdvantage in fertilization success for smaller males unrelated to POST-SC (small males copulated for longer and fertilized more eggs than large males). copulation duration (and also leave an unfilled spermatheca available for rival males). Furthermore, even in species that insert only one pedipalp, escaping affords the opportunity for further mating, which would considerably increase a male’s fitness over a cannibalized male. Elgar et al. (2000) succeeded in demonstrating female control of paternity separate from the confounding effects of intrasexual selection in the St Andrew’s cross spider Argiope keyserlingi. In this species, females terminate copulation by detaching males from their genital openings, 485 486 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 71, 3 wrapping and then killing them. Females copulating with relatively smaller males delayed sexual cannibalism, allowing prolonged copulations that resulted in more eggs being fertilized (Table 2). The apparent lack of evidence of an ‘inherent’ advantage to small males of escaping cannibalism and, thereby, prolonging copulation duration, supports direct mate choice explanations of sexual cannibalism in A. keyserlingi, according to Elgar et al. (2000). Additional evidence of a mating advantage for smaller males is also present in the cannibalistic spider Nephila edulis (Schneider et al. 2000). Why females of these species should favour smaller, rather than larger males, however, is unclear. Sexual selection may favour small size under conditions of scramble competition, when mate searching entails climbing (Moya-Laraño et al. 2002) or if these males mature earlier and have a higher probability of locating a female than later maturing males (Andersson 1994; see also Vollrath & Parker 1992). Although whether or not and even why sexual cannibalism by females influences this is unclear (Table 2). POST-SC in Latrodectus revivensis has been proposed as a female strategy to counter monopolization of both (paired) sperm storage organs by males (B. Berendonck, personal communication). In this desert widow spider, females ensure they obtain sufficient quantities of sperm to fertilize their eggs by allowing males to inseminate one spermatheca. Thereafter, females practise size-dependent sexual cannibalism on small males, denying them the opportunity to copulate more than once. Thus, females can practise sequential choice via POST-SC, allowing only larger or better-quality males to fill the second spermatheca, or even replace the first ‘inferior’ male’s sperm from the already filled sperm storage organ. Specific details of female discriminatory behaviour, other than the gross effects of cannibalism, however, remain unclear. In N. plumipes, females cannibalize large males (Schneider & Elgar 2001). Although they did not deliver significantly more sperm to the female’s spermathecae, males cannibalized by nonvirgin females secured a higher proportion of paternity than males that survived mating (Schneider & Elgar 2001). We interpret these results as being suggestive of sequential (cryptic) mate choice, although the basis of any discrimination and choice by the female is unclear. However, Schneider & Elgar (2002) suggested that sexual cannibalism in N. plumipes is a consequence of female life history strategies and attributed it to an increased foraging voracity of females that matured at a smaller size and in poorer condition. Mate choice may represent only a contributing factor, therefore, to the evolution and maintenance of POST-SC in N. plumipes. Returning to the criteria for female choice, there is evidence of POST-SC leading to increased paternity for cannibalized males (Table 2). In only one case (Elgar et al. 2000), however, has variation in POST-SC and paternity been associated with or attributable to an identifiable male trait. Whether females can control the variation in paternity that differential POST-SC produces, by varying the onset of sexual cannibalism, is debatable. In three cases (A. keyserlingi, L. hasselti and N. plumipes), females may be able to manipulate or otherwise influence the paternity of their broods through postcopulatory control of sperm uptake or transfer. Nevertheless, females of some sexually cannibalistic spiders do appear to practise cryptic female choice but the exact mechanism of cryptic choice is debatable, requires further investigation and may not be a simple reflection of the increased copulation time that POST-SC seems to provide some males (Table 2). Only further detailed examination of the mechanism of transfer of sperm and other extragametic substances in the ejaculate by males (e.g. Snow & Andrade 2004), and their usage by females, will determine whether females are using sperm from ‘preferred’ males differentially and whether sexual cannibalism can influence this process. Certainly, in the redback spider L. hasselti, there would appear to be a linkage between sperm uptake and POST-SC. However, while no clear mechanism for female control of paternity has been shown, possible male influences (e.g. sperm manipulation) cannot be ruled out. Snow & Andrade (2004) attributed reduced postmating receptivity in cannibalistic L. hasselti females to manipulation by males through substances transferred in the ejaculate. Examination of possible copulatory courtship (Eberhard 1996) by males may enable traits/activities to be identified that distinguish between cannibalized and surviving males and can be related to differences in paternity. Courtship duration itself may constitute a signal used by female L. hasselti to discriminate against males. Andrade & Banta (2001) experimentally manipulated courtship duration (short or long) and found that males in the short courtship group had lower mating success and experienced more female rejection behaviour. They argued that this represented female discrimination, via a form of endurance test. In addition, as indicated earlier, POST-SC in this species increases male paternity (Andrade 1996) and this occurred more often with males in the long courtship group. MATE CHOICE AND SEXUAL CANNIBALISM Sexual cannibalism is thought to drive the evolution of various morphological and behavioural traits, particularly in spiders (reviewed in Robinson 1982; Elgar 1992) and mantids (e.g. Maxwell 1998, 1999). The power of sexual cannibalism as a selective agent derives from the drastic and instantaneous effect it can have on male survival and prospective mating opportunities (Persons & Uetz 2005) and is manifest in the cautious nature of courtship in cannibalistic species (Morse 2004). Sexual cannibalism may be a strong and important selective force even when it occurs infrequently (Morse 2004). There may be more than one cause for the evolution and maintenance of sexual cannibalism for any species (e.g. Johnson 2001; Morse 2004; Elgar & Schneider 2005); various hypotheses are not mutually exclusive and may affect life history stages differently (Johnson 2001). Thus, mate choice, either overt or cryptic, clearly may not be the only explanation for sexual cannibalism. Mistaken identity (e.g. Robinson 1982; Gould 1984; Elgar 1992; Arnqvist & Henriksson 1997), hunger levels (Newman & Elgar 1991; Andrade 1998; Samu et al. 1999; Herberstein et al. 2002) and complicit cannibalism (male sacrifice) to REVIEW ensure paternity (Andrade 1996) contribute to the evolution and maintenance of sexual cannibalism. Even though male L. hasselti derived a paternity advantage (Andrade 1996) from sexual cannibalism, their fate was still determined by the females’ nutritional state (Andrade 1998). However, feeding history was not important in determining the likelihood of PRE-SC in fishing spiders (Johnson 2005a) or the jumping spider Phidippus johnsoni (Jackson 1980). In praying mantids, hungrier females were more likely to cannibalize males than better-fed females (Liske & Davis 1984, 1987; Birkhead et al. 1988; Kynaston et al. 1994). Previous juvenile experience may also affect PRE-SC in adult females. Johnson (2005a) showed that in D. triton, juveniles’ experience of cohabitation with adult males (an indicator of the availability of males/future mating opportunities) increased the likelihood of PRE-SC (see also Hebets 2003). Notwithstanding the possibility that cannibalism may act in mate choice, it seems unlikely that mate choice has been the most important driving force behind the evolution of sexual cannibalism. There are numerous opportunities for females to influence the paternity of their young without resorting to killing and consuming males. However, even if the initial function of sexual cannibalism was to obtain food it might then have become a mechanism of mate choice; this may have added to the benefits of cannibalism (see Persons & Uetz 2005) and may remain an important influence on its maintenance in cannibalistic species, imposing directional selection on male morphology and behaviour, particularly in polygamous mating systems. Indeed, this may be why we find more evidence for indirect than direct mate choice though sexual cannibalism. REVERSED SEXUAL CANNIBALISM Reversed sexual cannibalism may occur particularly in species in which males are larger than females. Male amphipods (Gammarus pulex and G. duebeni celticus) can cannibalize smaller females, especially at the moult when they are most vulnerable (Dick et al. 1993; Dick 1995). Similarly, female brachyurid paddle crabs, Ovalipes catharus, are also vulnerable at their moult when their shell is soft and may be subject to cannibalism during or after mating (Haddon 1995). Male Ichthyoxenus fushanensis (an isopod body cavity parasite of fish) may cannibalize females during or after mating (Tsai & Dai 2003). In the case of Gammarus species, sexual cannibalism would seem to be opportunistic in nature and not have any mate choice implications. Not all females produce eggs at each moult, however, and males may preferentially cannibalize females without eggs, but this has not been tested. Cannibalism of the female may be similarly opportunistic in O. catharus; however, males seem to avoid cannibalism of females they have recently inseminated. Resource limitation may explain reversed sexual cannibalism in I. fushanensis (Tsai & Dai 2003), but requires further empirical investigation. There is anecdotal evidence of a male eating a female in the mantid Tarachodes afzeli (Edmunds 1975). Unusually for a spider, males of the water spider Argyroneta aquatica are larger than females, prefer to approach larger females and may cannibalize females smaller than themselves (Schütz & Taborsky 2005). Females prefer to mate with larger males, but are more wary of them, fleeing from them more often than from males closer to their own size. The vulnerability to sexual cannibalism of the victim appears to be key in A. aquatica and would suggest that an indirect mechanism, rather than direct mate choice, produces the size-dependent reversed sexual cannibalism observed. With the male-biased size dimorphism present and the relative size of the female determining her vulnerability, more smaller females will tend to be cannibalized. Presumably in all cases of reversed sexual cannibalism there is little prospect of the female producing offspring for the male or the need for food exceeds the fitness gain from mating. Since males tend towards promiscuity to maximize their reproductive output, it is difficult to envisage mate choice through sexual cannibalism for males, unless the sex roles are reversed or the sex ratio is extremely female biased, offering males the opportunity to be choosy. Costly courtship and mating may promote male mate choice (Bonduriansky 2001), but there are no clear examples of male mate choice via reversed sexual cannibalism. CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS There are clear gaps in our knowledge with regard to mate choice and sexual cannibalism that need to be filled. Research on mantids has concentrated on male behaviour and avoidance of sexual cannibalism and sheds little light on mate choice (e.g. Maxwell 1998, 1999), although the escape behaviour and copulation success of attacked males argues against mate choice (Liske & Davis 1987; Maxwell 1999). Sexual cannibalism also occurs in scorpions but it is not as prevalent as previously presumed and appears to play no role in mate choice in this group (Polis & Sissom 1990; Peretti et al. 1999; Benton 2001). While there is no similar doubt over the prevalence of sexual cannibalism in spiders, its function remains the subject of debate. The occurrence of mate choice through sexual cannibalism in arachnids is supported by some empirical evidence, manifested by size dependency in PRE-SC (Rubenstein 1987; Elgar & Nash 1988; Arnqvist 1992) and by cryptic choice in POST-SC (Elgar et al. 2000). There is little evidence, however, for direct mate choice by female spiders or scorpions (Peretti et al. 1999) in PRE-SC. Rather, sexual cannibalism may be economic in nature, and may reflect opportunism and voracity in female predatory behaviour, the effects of relative size and the reduced ability of smaller males to fend off larger females. However, whichever mechanism is in operation, the same directional selection on male size will still be exercised. We have presented a framework by which research may be advanced and direct and indirect choice distinguished that requires empirical and theoretical examination. There remains a paucity of evidence and rigorous investigation of the mate rejection hypothesis and PRESC. This represents a poor state of affairs in such an 487 488 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 71, 3 important predatory animal group as arachnids, particularly when the historical notoriety of sexual cannibalism in spiders is taken into account (Darwin 1871). Furthermore, greater emphasis must be placed on field rather than laboratory study, and on determining the longterm fitness benefits of the apparent mate choices of females, since a direct benefit of cannibalism is known only for a few species (e.g. Elgar 1992; Fromhage et al. 2003; Elgar & Schneider 2005). Further investigation of environmental (e.g. feeding rate, male encounter rate, effects of population density) and especially genetic (e.g. population differences) influences on female behaviour towards courting males is also necessary. More detailed examinations of females’ behaviour towards males, particularly their motivation to attack different males, and investigations of the effects of relative size and the ability of males to withstand the advances of females, are required if we are to understand sexual cannibalism in spiders better and to assess the role of mate choice. The direct causes of size dependency in sexual cannibalism are unclear and the examination of the costs and benefits of this behaviour has been inadequate. Future investigations should focus on ameliorating these inadequacies, for example, by comparing size at emergence, foraging ability, growth rate and ability of the offspring of preferred males to avoid cannibalism with those of the offspring of males that would ordinarily be cannibalized. Genetic benefits from mate choice may be context dependent and, therefore, selection pressures on female mate choice will vary across environments. Qvarnström (2001) proposed that a profitable approach to the evolution of mate preferences would be to examine them in terms of optimality, as in other reproductive decisions. The optimal mate choice might be dynamic, rather than constant, and be determined by a variety of factors. Fluctuations in internal factors (e.g. reproductive status of females and their sperm store) and external factors (e.g. food supply and male encounter rate) should all influence the female’s decision to mate or cannibalize a male. Evidence suggests female choice by POST-SC in several spiders, with cannibalized males achieving longer copulations and potentially greater paternity than noncannibalized males (but see above). It has been assumed that longer copulation allows increased quantities of sperm to be transferred to the female, with a resultant increase in paternity. However, this has been called into question (Bukowski et al. 2001; Snow & Andrade 2004). While cryptic choice would seem likely, the mechanism by which it is achieved is unclear. This highlights the need for detailed investigation of the mechanics of sperm transfer and of mechanisms by which females may control paternity of their offspring. In general, research should be focused on examining the various criteria for demonstrating mate choice in both PRE-SC and POST-SC. In the latter, it is important to examine both variation in paternity with POSTSC and the mechanism by which it may be achieved. Where POST-SC leads to variation in male paternity, but no obvious mechanism for female control presents itself, further detailed examination of male and female copulatory behaviour is advocated. Little is known about reversed sexual cannibalism, although it appears to be associated with opportunism and vulnerability in the victim and its occurrence may be unrelated to mate choice. There is a growing consensus that sexual cannibalism may have multiple explanations and could have developed in response to a variety of lifestyle and phylogenetic constraints (e.g. Morse 2004; Elgar & Schneider 2005). Mate choice may be just one of these explanations. We caution that alternative explanations of the evolution of sexual cannibalism, especially economic cannibalism and life history implications, be considered before explanations based on mate choice, especially direct mate choice, are adopted. Acknowledgments Mark Elgar, Jutta Schneider, Michael Taborsky, Chad Johnson, Maydianne Andrade, Bernhard Huber and Tina Berendonck kindly provided information on unpublished research. We are grateful to Leigh Simmons, Mark Elgar, Tina Berendonck, Mark Briffa, Gillian Riddell and Liz Pothanikat for their comments on the manuscript. J.P. acknowledges support from the UK Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (S20306) and a Research Fellowship from the Institute of Irish Studies (QUB). C.M. thanks the UK Natural Environment Research Council (GR3/12871) and the Isle of Man Government for support. References Andersson, M. 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press. Andrade, M. C. B. 1996. Sexual selection and male sacrifice in the Australian redback spider. Science, 271, 70–72. Andrade, M. C. B. 1998. Female hunger can explain variation in cannibalistic behaviour despite male sacrifice in redback spiders. Behavioral Ecology, 9, 33–42. Andrade, M. C. B. 2003. Risky mate search and male self-sacrifice in redback spiders. Behavioral Ecology, 14, 531–538. Andrade, M. C. B. & Banta, E. M. 2001. Female preference for prolonged courtship in redback spiders. Advances in Ethology, 36, 115. Andrade, M. C. B. & Banta, E. M. 2002. Value of remating and functional sterility in redback spiders. Animal Behaviour, 63, 857–870. Andrade, M. C. B., Gu, L. & Stoltz, G. A. 2005. Novel male trait prolongs survival in suicidal mating. Biology Letters, 1, 276–279. Arnqvist, G. 1992. Courtship behaviour and sexual cannibalism in the semi-aquatic fishing spider, Dolomedes fibriatus (Clerck) (Araneae: Pisauridae). Journal of Arachnology, 20, 222–226. Arnqvist, G. & Henriksson, S. 1997. Sexual cannibalism in the fishing spider and a model for the evolution of sexual cannibalism based on genetic constraints. Evolutionary Ecology, 11, 255–273. Austin, A. D. & Anderson, D. T. 1978. Reproduction and development of the spider Nephila edulis (Koch) (Araneidae: Araneae). Australian Journal of Zoology, 26, 501–518. Benton, T. 2001. Reproductive ecology. In: Scorpion Biology and Research (Ed. by P. Brownell & G. Polis), pp. 278–301. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Birkhead, T. R. 1998. Cryptic female choice: criteria for establishing female sperm choice. Evolution, 52, 1212–1218. Birkhead, T. R. 2000. Defining and demonstrating postcopulatory female choice: again. Evolution, 54, 1057–1060. REVIEW Birkhead, T. R. & Møller, A. P. 1998. Sperm competition, sexual selection and different routes to fitness. In: Sperm Competition and Sexual Selection (Ed. by T. R. Birkhead & A. P. Møller), pp. 757– 777. London: Academic Press. Birkhead, T. R., Lee, K. E. & Young, P. 1988. Sexual cannibalism in the praying mantis Hierodula membranacea. Behaviour, 106, 112–118. Blanckenhorn, W. U. 2000. The evolution of body size: what keeps organisms small?. Quarterly Review of Biology, 75, 385–407. Bonduriansky, R. 2001. The evolution of male mate choice in insects: a synthesis of ideas and evidence. Biological Reviews, 76, 305–339. Brown, W. D., Crespi, B. J. & Choe, J. C. 1997. Sexual conflict and the evolution of mating systems. In: The Evolution of Mating Systems in Insects and Arachnids (Ed. by J. C. Choe & B. J. Crespi), pp. 352–377. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bukowski, T. C. & Christenson, T. E. 2000. Determinants of mating frequency in the spiny orbweaving spider, Micrathena gracilis (Araneae: Araneidae). Journal of Insect Behavior, 13, 331–352. Bukowski, T. C., Linn, C. D. & Christenson, T. E. 2001. Copulation and sperm release in Gasteracantha cancriformis (Araneae: Araneidae): differential male behaviour based on female mating history. Animal Behaviour, 62, 887–895. Chapman, T. & Partridge, L. 1996. Sexual conflict as fuel for evolution. Nature, 381, 189–190. Darwin, C. 1871. Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man. London: Murray. Dick, J. T. A. 1995. The cannibalistic behaviour of 2 Gammarus species (Crustacea, Amphipoda). Journal of Zoology, 236, 697–706. Dick, J. T. A., Montgomery, W. I. & Elwood, R. W. 1993. Replacement of the indigenous amphipod Gammarus duebeni celticus by the introduced Gammarus pulex: differential cannibalism and mutual predation. Journal of Animal Ecology, 62, 79–88. Eberhard, W. G. 1996. Female Control: Sexual Selection by Cryptic Female Choice. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press. Eberhard, W. G. 2000. Criteria for demonstrating postcopulatory female choice. Evolution, 54, 1047–1050. Edmunds, M. 1975. Courtship, mating and possible sex pheromones in three species of Mantodea. Entomologist’s Monthly Magazine, 111, 53–57. Elgar, M. A. 1992. Sexual cannibalism in spiders and other invertebrates. In: Cannibalism: Ecology and Evolution among Diverse Taxa (Ed. by M. A. Elgar & B. J. Crespi), pp. 128–155. Oxford: Oxford Scientific. Elgar, M. A. 1998. Sperm competition and sexual selection in spiders and other arachnids. In: Sperm Competition and Sexual Selection (Ed. by T. R. Birkhead & A. P. Møller), pp. 307–339. London: Academic Press. Elgar, M. A. & Bathgate, R. 1996. Female receptivity and male mate-guarding in the jewel spider, Gasteracantha minax Thorell (Araneae). Journal of Insect Behavior, 9, 729–738. Elgar, M. A. & Fahey, B. F. 1996. Sexual cannibalism, competition and size dimorphism in the orb-weaving spiders Nephila plumipes Latreille (Araneae: Araneoidea). Behavioral Ecology, 7, 195–198. Elgar, M. A. & Nash, D. R. 1988. Sexual cannibalism in the garden spider Araneus diadematus. Animal Behaviour, 36, 1511–1517. Elgar, M. A. & Schneider, J. M. 2005. The evolutionary significance of sexual cannibalism. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 34, 135– 163. Elgar, M. A., Schneider, J. M. & Herberstein, M. E. 2000. Female control of paternity in the sexually cannibalistic spider Argiope keyserlingi. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 267, 2439–2443. Elgar, M. A., Bruce, M. & de Crespigny, F. E. C. 2003. Male mate choice and patterns of paternity in the polyandrous, sexually cannibalistic orb-web spider, Nephila plumipes. Australian Journal of Zoology, 51, 357–365. Elwood, R. W., Wood, K. E., Gallagher, M. B. & Dick, J. T. A. 1998. Probing motivational state during agonistic encounters in animals. Nature, 393, 66–68. Foellmer, M. W. & Fairbairn, D. J. 2003. Spontaneous male death during copulation in an orb-weaving spider. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 270, S183–S185. Foellmer, M. W. & Fairbairn, D. J. 2004. Males under attack: sexual cannibalism and its consequences for male morphology and behaviour in an orb-weaving spider. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 6, 163–181. Forster, L. M. 1992. The stereotyped behaviour of sexual cannibalism in Latrodectus hasselti Thorell (Araneae: Theridiidae), the Australian redback spider. Australian Journal of Zoology, 40, 1–11. Fox, L. R. 1975. Cannibalism in natural populations. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 6, 87–106. Fromhage, L. & Schneider, J. M. 2005. Safer sex with feeding females: sexual conflict in a cannibalistic spider. Behavioral Ecology, 16, 377–382. Fromhage, L., Uhl, G. & Schneider, J. M. 2003. Fitness consequences of sexual cannibalism in female Argiope bruennichi. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 55, 60–64. Fromhage, L., Elgar, M. A. & Schneider, J. M. 2005. Faithful without care: the evolution of monogyny. Evolution, 59, 1400–1405. Gould, S. J. 1984. Only his wings remained. Natural History, 93, 10–18. Haddon, M. 1995. Avoidance of post-coital cannibalism in the brachyurid paddle crab Ovalipes catharus. Oecologia, 104, 256–258. Halliday, T. R. 1983. The study of mate choice. In: Mate Choice (Ed. by P. Bateson), pp. 3–32. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hebets, E. A. 2003. Subadult experience influences adult mate choice in an arthropod: exposed female spiders prefer males of a familiar phenotype. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 100, 13390–13395. Herberstein, M. E., Schneider, J. M. & Elgar, M. A. 2002. Costs of courtship and mating in a sexually cannibalistic orb-web spider: female mating strategies and their consequences for males. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 51, 440–446. Huber, B. A. 2005. Sexual selection research on spiders: progress and biases. Biological Reviews, 80, 363–385. Jackson, R. R. 1980. Cannibalism as a factor in the mating strategy of the jumping spider Phidippus johnsoni (Araneae, Salticidae). Bulletin of the British Arachnological Society, 5, 129–133. Johns, P. M. & Maxwell, M. R. 1997. Sexual cannibalism: who benefits? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 12, 127–128. Johnson, J. C. 2001. Sexual cannibalism in fishing spiders (Dolomedes): an evaluation of two explanations for female aggression towards potential mates. Animal Behaviour, 61, 905–914. Johnson, J. C. 2005a. Cohabitation of juvenile females with mature males promotes sexual cannibalism in fishing spiders. Behavioral Ecology, 16, 269–273. Johnson, J. C. 2005b. The role of body size in mating interactions of the sexually cannibalistic fishing spider Dolomedes triton. Ethology, 111, 51–62. Kirkpatrick, M. 1987. The evolutionary forces acting on female mating preferences in polygynous animals. In: Sexual Selection: Testing the Alternatives (Ed. by J. W. Bradbury & M. B. Andersson), pp. 67–82. Chichester: J. Wiley. Kynaston, S. E., McErlaine-Ward, P. & Mill, P. J. 1994. Courtship, mating behaviour and sexual cannibalism in the praying mantis, Sphodromantis lineola. Animal Behaviour, 47, 739–741. Lawrence, S. E. 1992. Sexual cannibalism in the praying mantid, Mantis religiosa: a field study. Animal Behaviour, 43, 569–583. 489 490 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 71, 3 Liske, E. & Davis, W. J. 1984. Sexual behaviour of the Chinese praying mantis. Animal Behaviour, 32, 916–944. Liske, E. & Davis, W. J. 1987. Courtship and mating behaviour of the Chinese praying mantis, Tenodera aridifolia sinensis. Animal Behaviour, 35, 1525–1537. Maxwell, M. R. 1998. Lifetime mating opportunities and male mating behaviour in sexually cannibalistic praying mantids. Animal Behaviour, 55, 1011–1028. Maxwell, M. R. 1999. The risk of cannibalism and male mating behavior in the Mediterranean praying mantid, Iris oratoria. Behaviour, 136, 205–219. Morse, D. H. 2004. A test of sexual cannibalism models, using a sitand-wait predator. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 81, 427–437. Moya-Laraño, J., Halaj, J. & Wise, D. W. 2002. Climbing to reach females: Romeo should be small. Evolution, 56, 420–425. Moya-Laraño, J., Pascual, J. & Wise, D. W. 2004. Approach strategy by which male Mediterranean tarantulas adjust to the cannibalistic behaviour of females. Ethology, 110, 717–724. Newman, J. A. & Elgar, M. A. 1991. Sexual cannibalism in orbweaving spiders: an economic model. American Naturalist, 138, 1372–1395. Parker, G. A. 1984. Sperm competition and the evolution of animal mating strategies. In: Sperm Competition and the Evolution of Animal Mating Systems (Ed. by R. L. Smith), pp. 2–60. London: Academic Press. Peretti, A. V., Acosta, L. E. & Benton, T. G. 1999. Sexual cannibalism in scorpions: fact or fiction. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 68, 485–496. Persons, M. H. & Uetz, G. W. 2005. Sexual cannibalism and mate choice decisions in wolf spiders: influence of male size and secondary sexual characters. Animal Behaviour, 69, 83–94. Pitnick, S. & Brown, W. D. 2000. Criteria for demonstrating female sperm choice. Evolution, 54, 1052–1056. Polis, G. A. 1981. The evolution and dynamics of intraspecific predation. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 12, 225–251. Polis, G. A. & Sissom, W. D. 1990. Life history. In: The Biology of Scorpions (Ed. by G. A. Polis), pp. 161–263. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Prenter, J., Elwood, R. W. & Montgomery, W. I. 1994a. Male exploitation of female predatory behaviour: cannibalism reduction in male autumn spiders, Metellina segmentata. Animal Behaviour, 47, 235–236. Prenter, J., Elwood, R. W. & Colgan, S. 1994b. The influence of prey size and female reproductive state on the courtship of the autumn spider, Metellina segmentata: a field experiment. Animal Behaviour, 47, 449–456. Qvarnström, A. 2001. Context-dependent genetic benefits from mate choice. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 16, 5–7. Robinson, M. H. 1982. Courtship and mating behaviour in spiders. Annual Review of Entomology, 27, 1–20. Rubenstein, D. I. 1987. Alternative reproductive tactics in the spider Meta segmentata. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 20, 229–237. Samu, F., Toft, S. & Balázs, K. 1999. Factors influencing cannibalism in the wolf spider Pardosa agrestis (Araneae, Lycosidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 45, 349–354. Schneider, J. M. & Elgar, M. A. 2001. Sexual cannibalism and sperm competition in the golden orb-web spider Nephila plumipes (Araneoidea): female and male perspectives. Behavioral Ecology, 12, 547–552. Schneider, J. M. & Elgar, M. A. 2002. Sexual cannibalism in Nephila plumipes as a consequence of female life history strategies. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 15, 84–91. Schneider, J. M. & Lubin, Y. 1998. Intersexual conflict in spiders. Oikos, 83, 496–506. Schneider, J. M., Herberstein, M. E., de Crespigny, F. E. C., Ramamurthy, S. & Elgar, M. A. 2000. Sperm competition and small size advantage for males of the golden orb-web spider Nephila edulis. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 13, 939–946. Schneider, J. M., Thomas, M. L. & Elgar, M. A. 2001. Ectomised conductors in the golden orb-web spider, Nephila plumipes (Araneoidea): a male adaptation to sexual conflict. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 49, 410–415. Schneider, J. M., Gilberg, S., Fromhage, L. & Uhl, G. 2006. Sexual conflict over copulation duration in a cannibalistic spider. Animal Behaviour. Schütz, D. & Taborsky, M. 2005. Mate choice and sexual conflict in the size dimorphic water spider Argyroneta aquatica (Araneae: Argyronetidae). Journal of Arachnology, 33, 767–775. Searcy, W. A. & Andersson, M. 1986. Sexual selection and the evolution of song. Annual Reviews of Ecology and Systematics, 17, 507–533. Simmons, L. W. & Siva-Jothy, M. T. 1998. Sperm competition in insects: mechanisms and the potential for selection. In: Sperm Competition and Sexual Selection (Ed. by T. R. Birkhead & A. P. Møller), pp. 341–432. London: Academic Press. Snow, L. S. E. & Andrade, M. C. B. 2004. Pattern of sperm transfer in redback spiders: implications for sperm competition and male sacrifice. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 785–792. Stockley, P. 1997. Sexual conflict resulting from adaptations to sperm competition. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 12, 154–159. Taylor, P. W. & Elwood, R. W. 2003. The mismeasure of animal contests. Animal Behaviour, 65, 1195–1202. Tsai, M. L. & Dai, C. F. 2003. Cannibalism within mating pairs of the parasitic isopod, Ichthyoxenus fushanensis. Journal of Crustacean Biology, 23, 662–668. Uhl, G. & Vollrath, F. 1998. Little evidence of size-selective cannibalism in two species of Nephila (Araneae). Zoology, 101, 101– 106. Vollrath, F. & Parker, G. A. 1992. Sexual dimorphism and distorted sex ratios in spiders. Nature, 360, 156–159. Wiley, R. H. & Poston, J. 1996. Indirect mate choice, competition for mates, and coevolution of the sexes. Evolution, 50, 1371–1381.