Psychology Teacher Network is published by the Education Directorate of the American Psychological Association. Subscriptions are free to High School Teacher Affiliates of the APA and APA Members and $15 a year for all others. Address editorial correspondence to Psychology Teacher Network, Education Directorate, APA, 750 First St., N.E., Washington, DC 20002-4242. Address inquiries regarding membership or affiliation to the Membership Office, APA, at the same address. Senior Editor ...........................................................Julie Goldstein TOPSS Editor..................................................................Mary Spilis Community College Editor ...............................Martha Ellis, Ph.D. APA’s Pre-College and Undergraduate Officer.......................................Peter Petrossian Psychology Teacher Network Education Directorate American Psychological Association 750 First Street, NE Washington, DC 20002-4242 Nonprofit US Postage Paid Washington, DC Permit No. 6348 PTN For Teachers of Introductory Psychology APA EDUCATION DIRECTORATE November-December 1997•Volume 7 •Issue 5 PSYCHOLOGY TEACHER NETWORK Undergrads Enjoy APA Summer Science Institute By Maggie Nelson, APA Science Directorate Martin Seligman, PhD. of the University of Pennsylvania, answers students’ questions after his presentation on learned optimism. Left to right are Matthew Presley, Jason Oraker, Sapna Cheryan, Sarah Karlen, Sarah Austin-Willis, and Andrew Shannon. PHOTO A FPO Inside: Briefing . . . . . . . . . . . . . What Works: Vision Distorting Goggles . . . . Nature vs. Nurture . . . . . TOPSS Annual Essay Contest . . . . . . . . . . . . . . New TOPSS Members . . Activity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Dear Doctor . . . . . . . . . . Review . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Announcments . . . . . . . 2 5 6 8 9 10 12 13 14 In June, 32 exceptional undergraduate students convened at The Johns Hopkins University in Baltimore, MD for the third APA Summer Science Institute (SSI). Hailing from the United States, Canada, and Puerto Rico, students came to SSI for 8 days of intensive study in psychological science. The APA Summer Science Institute is an 8-day intensive program designed to engage students in the science of psychology, to model and illustrate the intellectual processes of scientific inquiry, and to give students hands-on experience in cutting-edge psychological research through lectures, discussions, and laboratory visits. Ludy T. Benjamin, Jr., PhD, Professor of Psychology at Texas A&M University, led the Institute, as he did in 1996. Distinguished researchers and teachers came to SSI from univer- sities around the country to present and discuss their work in the areas of sensation and taste, clinical research, social psychology, developmental psychology, and behavioral toxicology. The faculty included Linda Bartoshuk, PhD, Yale University; Charlotte Brown, PhD, University of Pittsburgh; Robert Cialdini, PhD, Arizona State University; Carol Dweck, PhD, Columbia University; Jack Nation, PhD, Texas A&M University; and Martin Seligman, PhD, University of Pennsylvania. In addition, faculty from Johns Hopkins led small groups of students in a 3-day lab experience. Students attending SSI were Spencer Allen, Hendrix College; Sarah Austrin-Willis, Wake Forest University; Daniel Barry, Vanderbilt University; Jennifer Bryan, Acadia University; Emma Buchtel, Yale University; Sapna Cheryan, Northwestern University; Mattie Gabston, Scripps College; Sharmin Ghaznavi, Massachusetts Institute of Technology; Eric Hamako, Stanford University; Colleen Head, College of William and Mary; Paul Johnson, University of Arizona; Erik Johnston, University of Denver; Sarah Karlen, University of Iowa; Jebby Lacey, Westminster College; Heather Littleton, Clemson University; See Undergrads, page 7 BRIEFING The New Psychology of Men By Ronald F. Levant, Ed.D., Nova Southeastern University Dr. Levant is the cofounder and first President of the Society for the Psychological Study of Men and Masculinity, Division 51 of APA. He is the co-author of Masculinity Reconstructed (Plume, 1996) and the coeditor of A New Psychology of Men (Basic Books, 1995) and of Men and Sex: New Psychological Perspectives (John Wiley & Sons, 1997). This article is adapted from an article that appeared in Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 2 Why Study the Psychology of Men? Those not familiar with this new work sometimes ask: "Why do we need a psychology of men? Isn't all psychology the psychology of men?" The answer is: Yes, males have been the focal point of most psychological research, but in studies which viewed males as representative of humanity as a whole. Feminist scholars challenged this traditional viewpoint by arguing for a gender-specific approach, and in the past quarter century, have rewritten the canon on the psychology of women. In the same spirit, men's studies scholars over the past fifteen years have begun to examine masculinity not as a normative referent, but rather as a complex and problematic construct. In so doing, they have provided a framework for a psychological approach to men and masculinity that questions traditional norms of the male role, such as the emphasis on competition, status, toughness, and emotional stoicism, and views certain male problems (such as aggression and violence, devaluation of women, fear and hatred of homosexuals, detached fathering, and neglect of health needs) as unfortunate but predictable results of the male role socialization process. They have also provided a framework for creating positive new definitions of masculinity that support the optimal development of men, women, and children. This new psychology of men is both overdue and urgently needed. Men are disproportionately represented among many problem populations—substance abusers, the homeless, perpetrators of family and interpersonal violence, parents estranged from their children, sex addicts and sex offenders, victims of homicide, suicide, and fatal automobile accidents, and victims of life-style and stress-related fatal illnesses. A new psychology of men might contribute to the understanding and solution of some of these male problems which have long impacted women, men, children, and society in negative ways. Moreover, due to long delays in dealing with many of these problems, we are currently experiencing a "crisis of connection" between men and women (Levant, 1996). As a result, the pressures on men to behave in ways that conflict with various aspects of traditional masculinity ideology have never been greater. These new pressures—pressures to commit to relationships, to communicate one's innermost feelings, to nurture children, to share in housework, to integrate sexuality with love, and to curb aggression and violence — have shaken traditional masculinity ideology to such an extent there is now a "masculinity Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 crisis" in which many feel bewildered and confused, and the pride associated with being a man is lower than at any time in the recent past (Levant, 1997). Many such men are gravitating to organizations such as the Promise Keepers (Promise Keepers, 1994) and the Fatherhood Initiative (Blankenhorn, 1995), which propose to return the male to his "rightful place" as the "leader of his family" by rolling back the gains of the women's movement. A new psychology of men might help men find solutions to the masculinity crisis and the crisis of connection that enhance rather than inflame gender relations, and provide them with tools for the reconstruction of the traditional male code (Levant & Kopecky, 1996). The purpose of this article is to introduce this new field. We will cover the gender role strain paradigm, masculinity ideology, and the three varieties of male gender role strain—discrepancy strain, dysfunction strain, and trauma strain. The Gender Role Strain Paradigm The new psychology of men views gender roles not as biological or even social "givens", but rather as psychologically and socially constructed entities that bring certain advantages and disadvantages, and, most importantly, can change. This perspective acknowledges the biological differences between men and women, but argues that it is not the biological differences of sex that make for "masculinity" and "femininity". These notions are socially constructed from bits and pieces of biological, psychological, and social experience to serve particular purposes. Traditional constructions of gender serve patriarchal purposes; non-traditional constructions, such as Gilmore (1990) described among the Tahitians and the Semai, serve more equalitarian purposes. The Gender Role Strain paradigm, originally formulated by Joseph Pleck in The Myth of Masculinity (1981), is the forerunner, in the new psychology of men, of social constructionism, and of modern critical thinking about masculinity, having been formulated before social constructionism emerged as a new perspective on masculinity (Pleck, 1995). It spawned a number of major research programs that have produced important data which have deepened our understanding of the strain men experience when they attempt to live up the impossibility of the male role. Pleck demonstrated that the paradigm which had dominated the research on masculinity for fifty years (1930-1980)—the Gender Role Identity Paradigm— not only poorly accounts for the observed data, but also promotes the patriarchal bifurcation of society on the basis of stereotyped gender roles. In its place, Pleck proposed the Gender Role Strain Paradigm. The older Gender Role Identity Paradigm assumed that people have an inner psychological need to have a gender role identity, and that optimal personality development hinged on its formation. The extent to which this "inherent" need is met is determined by how completely a person embraces their traditional gender role. From such a perspective, the development of appropriate gender role identity is viewed as a failure-prone process; and, failure for men to achieve a masculine gender role identity is thought to result in homosexuality, negative attitudes towards women, or defensive hypermasculinity. This paradigm springs from the same philosophical roots as the "essentialist" or "nativist" view of sex roles— the notion that (in the case of men) there is a clear masculine "essence" that is historically invariant. In contrast, the Gender Role Strain Paradigm proposes that contemporary gender roles are contradictory and inconsistent; that the proportion of persons who violate gender roles is high; that violation of gender roles leads to condemnation and negative psychological consequences; that actual or imagined violation of gender roles leads people to overconform to them; that violating gender roles have more severe consequences for males than for females; and that certain prescribed gender role traits (such as male aggression) are often dysfunctional. In this paradigm, appropriate gender roles are determined by the prevailing gender ideology (which is operationally defined by gender role stereotypes and norms), and are imposed on the developing child by parents, teachers, and peers—the cultural transmitters who subscribe to the prevailing gender ideology. As noted above, this paradigm springs from the same philosophical roots as social constructionism—the perspective that notions of "masculinity" and femininity" are relational, socially constructed, and subject to change. Masculinity Ideology. Thompson and Pleck (1995) proposed the term "masculinity ideology" to characterize the core construct in the corpus of research assessing attitudes toward men and male roles. Masculinity, or gender, ideology is a very different construct from the older notion of gender orientation. Gender orientation arises out of the Identity Paradigm, and "presumes that masculinity is rooted in actual differences between men and women" (Thompson & Pleck, 1995, p. 130). This approach has attempted to assess the personality traits more often associated with men than women. In contrast, studies of masculinity ideology take a normative approach, in which masculinity is viewed as a socially constructed gender ideal for men. Whereas the masculine male in the orientation/trait approach is one who possesses particular personality traits, the traditional male in the ideology/normative approach "is one who endorses the ideology that men should have sex-specific characteristics (and women should not)" (Thompson & Pleck, 1995, p. 131). Thompson and Pleck (1995) adduced evidence to support the notion that gender orientation and gender ideologies are independent and have different correlates. Masculinity ideologies. The Strain Paradigm asserts that there is no single standard for masculinity nor is there an unvarying masculinity ideology. Rather, since masculinity is a social construction, ideals of manhood may differ for men of different social classes, races, ethnic groups, sexual orientations, life stages, and historical eras. Following Brod (1987) we therefore prefer to speak of masculinity ideologies. To illustrate, consider these brief descriptions of varying male codes among four ethnic-minority groups in the contemporary United States: African-American males have adopted distinctive actions and attitudes known as cool pose ...Emphasizing honor, virility, and physical strength, the Latino male adheres to a code of machismo...The American-Indian male struggles to maintain contact with a way of life and the traditions of elders while faced with economic castration and political trauma... Asian-American men resolve uncertainty privately in order to save face and surrender personal autonomy to family obligations and needs. (Lazur & Majors, 1995, p. 338). Traditional masculinity ideology. Despite the diversity in masculinity ideology in the contemporary U.S., Pleck (1995, p. 20) points out that "there is a particular constellation of standards and expectations that individually and jointly have various kinds of negative concommitants." It is common to refer to this as "traditional" masculinity ideology, since it was the dominant view prior to the deconstruction of gender that took place beginning in the 1970's. Traditional masculinity ideology is thought to be a multidimensional construct. Brannon (David & Brannon, 1976) identified four components of traditional masculinity ideology: That men should not be feminine (labeled by Brannon "no sissy stuff"); that men should strive to be respected for successful achievement ("the big wheel"); that men should never show weakness ("the sturdy oak")' and that men should seek adventure and risk, even accepting violence if necessary ("give 'em hell"). More recently, Levant, Hirsch, Celentano, Cozza, Hill, MacEachern, Marty, & Schnedeker (1992) defined traditional masculinity ideology in terms of seven dimensions: The Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 3 requirement to avoid all things feminine; the injunction to restrict one's emotional life; the emphasis on toughness and aggression; the injunction to be self-reliant; the emphasis on achieving status above all else; non-relational, objectifying attitudes toward sexuality; and fear and hatred of homosexuals. Types of Male Gender Role Strain Pleck (1995), in an update on the Gender Role Strain Paradigm, pointed out that his original formulation of the paradigm stimulated research on three varieties of male gender role strain, which he termed "discrepancy-strain", "dysfunction strain", and "trauma-strain". Discrepancy strain results when one fails to live up to one's internalized manhood ideal, which, among contemporary adult males, is often a close approximation of the traditional code. Dysfunction strain results even when one fulfills the requirements of the male code, because many of the characteristics viewed as desirable in men can have negative side effects on the men themselves and on those close to them. Trauma strain results from the ordeal of the male role socialization process, which is now recognized as inherently traumatic. Discrepancy Strain One approach to investigating discrepancy strain used a version of the time-honored self/ideal-self research paradigm, in which participants were first asked, using adjectival rating scales, to describe the "ideal man," and then asked to describe themselves. The discrepancy between the two ratings was used as index of discrepancy strain, which was then studied in terms of its correlations with other variables such as self-esteem. This line of research has not been particularly productive. Another approach has been more fruitful. This approach does not ask participants whether discrepancy strain exists for them, but rather inquires as to whether they would experience particular gender discrepancies as conflictual or stressful if they did exist. Two major research programs have used this approach: O'Neil's (O'Neil, Good, & Holmes, 1995) work on male gender role conflict, and Eisler and Skidmore's (1995) work on masculine gender role stress. Dysfunction Strain The second type of gender role strain is dysfunction strain. The notion behind dysfunction strain is that the fulfillment of the requirements of the male code can be dysfunctional because many of the characteristics viewed as desirable in men can have negative side effects on the men themselves and on those close to them. Pleck (1995) reviewed some of the research that documents the existence of dysfunction strain, which includes studies that find negative 4 Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 outcomes associated with masculine gender-related personality traits on the one hand, and lack of involvement in family roles on the other hand. Brooks and Silverstein (1995) in a far-reaching discussion of the "dark side" of masculinity, provide a taxonomy of the problems that result from dysfunction strain. These are significant social and public health problems that Brooks and Silverstein (1995) argue result, through one pathway or another, from adherence to traditional masculinity ideology. These problems include: (1) violence, including male violence against women in the family, rape and sexual assault, and sexual harassment; (2) sexual excess, including promiscuity, involvement with pornography, and sexual addiction; (3) socially irresponsible behaviors, including chemical dependence, risk-seeking behavior, physically self-abuse, absent fathering, and homelessness/vagrancy; and, (4) relationship dysfunctions, including inadequate emotional partnering, non-nurturing fathering, and non-participative household partnering. Trauma Strain The concept of trauma-strain has been applied to certain groups of men whose experiences with gender role strain are thought to be particularly harsh. This includes professional athletes (Messner, 1992), war veterans (Brooks, 1990), and survivors of child abuse, including sexual abuse (Lisak, 1995). It is also being recognized that gay and bisexual men are traumatized by male gender role strain by virtue of growing up in a heterosexist society (Harrison, 1995). But above and beyond the recognition that certain classes of men may experience trauma strain, a perspective on the male role socialization process has emerged which views socialization under traditional masculinity ideology as inherently traumatic (Levant & Kopecky, 1996; Levant & Pollack, 1995). Conclusion This article introduced the new psychology of men, reviewing the gender role strain paradigm, masculinity ideology, and the three varieties of male gender role strain. It is the author's hope that this new work will open up new areas for assessment, intervention, and applied research, in order to provide improved psychological services to men that might contribute to the solution of both the contemporary masculinity crisis and the crisis of connection between men and women. References Available upon request to Psychology Teacher Network. Note: This article was adapted from Levant, R.F.(1996). The new psychology of men. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 27, Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 259-265. What Works: Vision Distorting Goggles By Jim Matiya, Lockport, IL Nearly one hundred years ago George Stratton, a psychologist at the University of California, had a most unusual idea. What would happen, he wondered, if we suddenly found ourselves in an upsidedown, wrong-way-around visual world? Would we learn to adjust to this novel visual world, or would it remain forever strange and distressing? Stratton designed special glasses with lenses that both inverted the visual field and reversed it, so that objects perceived on the left were actually on the right and objects perceived at the top were actually on the bottom. To test his idea he needed someone to wear these glasses for an extended period of time. He was unable to find a volunteer for the experiment, so Stratton wore them himself (Stratton, 1897). At first, he found this new upside-down, sides reversed, visual world strange and disturbing. Objects seemed to sweep and swing before his eyes; he bumped into things as he moved around and could not perform such simple tasks as eating a meal or washing his face. As the days went by, however, Stratton adjusted to his distorted vision sensations. Finally, he could move around with ease, eat his meals, and wash his face without difficulty. But, when he took the glasses off, the visual world again seemed strange and he had difficulty readjusting to normal visual sensations. In 1928 Theodor Erismann of the University of Innsbruck had subjects wear prism goggles similar to those designed by Stratton. His subjects, too, had to adjust to their distorted visual world. After several weeks, one of them became so at ease wearing the goggles that he rode a motorcycle through Innsbruck. Erismann also found that subjects were able to adjust to an even more elaborate device that allowed them to see only directly to the rear as if they had eyes at the back of their heads. In one of a series of later experiments, Ivan Kohler had subjects wear goggles in which each lens was divided vertically into blue and yellow halves. When the wearer looked to the left he saw a blue-tinted world, and to the right a yellowtinted world. After several weeks Kohler's subjects adapted to them and the color distortions disappeared (Kohler, 1962). For each of our major senses-vision, hearing, tasting, and smell, there is a relationship between the physical properties of sensation and the interpretation of these physical properties. This intimate relationship between the sensation and the perception of what we see, of what we hear, of what we taste, and of what we smell is important for students to understand and appreciate. For example, the sense of vision is composed of the sensations we receive and the interpretations of these sensations. Transduction is the process by which sensory systems transform physical energy into nerve impulses. At the most basic level, the eye works in the following way: 1) light first enters the cornea; 2) behind the cornea is the iris, which opens and closes and regulates the amount of light entering the eye; 3) this light enters the pupil which is the hole in the iris; 4) the light then passes through the lens; 5) the cilary muscle controls the thickness of the lens, which allows for focusing on objects; 6) and then the light goes to the retina, at the back of the eye. The transduction occurs in the retina. It is here that the sensation of light is converted into impulses that can be interpreted by the brain. The retina has two kinds of photoreceptor cells. They are called rods and cones because of their shape. The rods and cones are sensitive to light and color. Because of the retina's round shape, the image is received upside-down at the back of the eye. The photoreceptor cells (rods and cones) receive the light and send the signal via the optic nerve to the brain. The information of what we see is sent to the part of the brain called the visual cortex. The visual cortex receives an upside-down picture of what we see. The next step in the process of vision is perception. This area of psychology is concerned with how people organize and comprehend these neural impulses. Through parallel processing the brain breaks down the image into different parts such as color and form. This information is sent to other areas of the brain to further interpret and make sense of what we see. What would happen if we try to trick our eyes by wearing special goggles? I've developed special goggles for my students to wear that demonstrate these principles. The first pair of goggles I use is a glass prism. This prism is designed to invert the image of what we see before it enters the eye. These are called "inverted-image goggles." The image is inverted before it travels to the retina. The brain is tricked by seeing an image that is upside-down! The brain turns this picture upside-down (as it would with any object), and thus we see the world in an upside down fashion! We see the floor as the ceiling and the ceiling as the floor! The first steps a person takes with these goggles seem very strange and disorienting. I have students wear the goggles and try to walk a crooked line made of masking tape which is on the classroom See Goggles, page 7 Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 5 Nature vs. Nurture: The Invasion of the Mass Media By Martha Ellis, Ph.D., Collin County Community College, Plano, TX This update is provided courtesy of APA's Community College Working Group. 6 The beginning of the fall 1997 semester has been a hay day for anyone who teaches the introductory psychology course and/or a developmental psychology course. The mass media has provided diverse information in both print and television regarding the nature vs. nurture controversy. Beginning with John Stossel's program on happiness, the issue of genetics and environment was brought to the attention of the public by focusing on a specific emotion that most students are seeking to attain. In this program, Stossel showed various research techniques and interviewed Richard Davidson regarding his MRI studies, David Myers regarding his happiness research, and Thomas Bouchard, Jr. regarding his twin studies. At the same time, the New York Academy of Sciences devoted the September/October issue of The Sciences to the promise and peril of cloning. In this issue, Thomas Bouchard, Jr. interprets his twin studies. The article, Whenever the Twain Shall Meet, includes the history and methodology of the studies at the University of Minnesota. Bouchard summarizes, on average, personality traits of identical twins have a 50 percent correlation, while fraternal twins have a 25 percent correlation, and non-twin siblings an 11 percent correlation (p. 54). He reports that EEGs (usually as distinct as fingerprints) of identical twins are about as similar as two EEGs of the same person plotted at different times (p. 54). Bouchard explains that almost three quarters of a person's 100,000 genes are identical in all humans with the remaining one quarter defining the individual. He goes on to discuss the study of intelligence as it relates to genetics including much of the information that most psychology teachers share with their students. The strength of this article rests in Bouchard's discussion of the weaknesses of his research and the application of the research. With respect to the weakness, Bouchard states "for all their striking findings, twin studies do not explain how genes influence personality, intelligence, or social attitudes" (p. 56). He purports that genes make a personality more likely to respond to its environment in certain ways. Most psychological variability is probably shaped by experiences, however, those experiences are largely self-selected and that selection is guided by the pressure of the genome (p. 56). Bouchard gives examples of applying this viewpoint to both education and to the cloning issue. As he articulates, the twin studies Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 underscore their subjects' individuality because despite having identical genomes and even being raised together, identical twins still respond differently to many of the items on his questionnaires. Bouchard concludes by saying the twin studies refute both biological and environmental determinism. His results, he believes, account for the uniqueness of the individual and "remind us that we are an integral part of a complex biological world" (p. 57). Lastly, the October issue of Discover hit the newsstands about two weeks later with a special issue on genetics. Robert Sapolsky's article, A Gene for Nothing, provides an excellent overview of genetics and the nature-nurture issue. As Sapolsky informally states: "You've got nature-neurons, brain chemicals ...And then there's nurture, all those environmental breezes...Again and again, behavior biologists insist that you can't talk meaningfully about nature or nurture, only about their interaction" (p. 42). By explaining genes, DNA, neurotransmitters, and chromosomes, Sapolsky delineates why there is more to behavior than just genes. Examples about anxiety and depression are given with the interaction between environment and genetic tendencies clearly discussed. He concludes "genes modulate how one responds to the environment" (p. 46). The writing style and humor throughout the article provides readability for students in high school and college. With this influx of information from the mass media, what's a psychology teacher to do!?!? Have a wonderful time. By showing the first 15 minutes of the Stossel program on happiness, students see and hear about current research in a variety of settings. The article by Bouchard is a wonderful read because the author writes in the first person which provides a personable, interview feeling for students. The Bouchard and Sapolsky articles are short and easily understandable for high school and freshmen college students. Although these are not journal articles, they have a depth not usually contained in the popular press. This fall I combined all three of the above. On the first day of class we did the usual "agree or disagree" opinion statements regarding several nature/nurture questions. The next class period I showed the first part of the Stossel program, we discussed research methodology, and I handed out the Bouchard article. Students wrote a reaction paper to the article discussing (1) what they learned, (2) their opinion of the research processes including weaknesses and biases, and (3) any questions they had that the article did not answer. The next class period they broke into small groups and discussed their reactions. During the next week we discussed genetics and the chapter on the brain and I gave them the article from Discover. This time they were to write a reaction paper discussing the nature/nurture issue based upon what they had learned thus far in the course. The process took about ten minutes in each class session. The response I received from the students included an increased depth of understanding on genetics, nature/nurture issue, and research methodology. Also, the relevance of the topic was illuminated because it was in the mass media, not just in their textbook. They found the information fascinating and continue to apply what they learned as we approach new topics in the course. Undergrads, from page 1 left the Institute encouraged that these students may be among the future leaders in the field. The next Summer Science Institute will be held at the Johns Hopkins University in Baltimore, Maryland from Sunday, June 14 through Monday, June 22, 1998. Eligible students for this program are limited to those students who will have college sophomore or junior status following the end of the 1997-98 academic year. This program has proven to be quite competitive. We can accommodate 32 students, and we expect to receive approximately 400-500 applications. The criteria for selection include high school and college GPAs, college entrance exam scores, courses taken, letters of reference, and responses to the short essay questions. Applications are available on the Web at http://www.apa.org/science/ssi98.html. For more information, contact the Science Directorate at (202) 336-6000; (202) 336-6132 (TDD); science@apa.org. Conswelia Livingston, Boston University; Brian Loendorf, Oklahoma State University; Mildred Maldonado-Molina, University of Puerto Rico; Jason C. Oraker, Williams College; Matthew Presley, Austin College; Michael Puma, Fordham University; Scott Ritter, University of Wisconsin, Eau Claire; Kathryn Ruddy, Harvard University; Aaron Sackett, St. Olaf College; Emily Sadigh, Harvard University; Amanda Saw, University of California, Los Angeles; Andrew Shannon, Kenyon College; Ruth Shim, College of William and Mary; Robyn Siperstein, Yale University; Deborah Small, University of Pennsylvania; Stacey Storch, Duke University; and Amir Zarrinpar, Harvard University. By all accounts, SSI was a great success. The students left with a greater appreciation and understanding of the science of psychology, and the faculty Goggles, from page 5 floor, try to catch a ball thrown to them, try to juggle a nerf ball, and try to pour Styrofoam packing from one container into another one. The second pair of goggles are designed with a special flat prism that changes the location of objects. These are called “displacement” goggles because the location of objects has been displaced or relocated. In this particular case, objects are displaced 40 degrees. The prism can be moved around so that the forty degree displacement relocate objects to the left, to the right, and either up or down! In this way, students never know which way the displacement will occur! They can only find objects if they see for References Bouchard, T.J., Jr. (1997). Whenever the twain shall meet. The Sciences, 37, 5, 52-57. Sapolsky, R. (1997). A gene for nothing. Discover, 18, 10, 40-46. Stossel, J. (September, 1997). The mystery of happiness, ABC News. themselves! Each person has to adapt to a new field of vision! I try to have students move objects on a table, play a board game, and play solitaire in order to give them some experience with these goggles. When they reach for objects they realize that the objects are not where they think they "should be!" Both of these goggles challenge the students to know about the structure of the eye, perception of objects, and the ability to adapt to changes in the environment. Most students are unaware of the way the eyes work. They seem unaware of the complexity of how we see. What we see is a product of organization, learning, and physical sensations. We rarely question what we see, believing that our vi- sion is accurate and truthful. We see people who have eye problems everyday. Some people have to wear glasses to help them see. Yet, we never think about how we adapt to wearing glasses. The above vignette is very challenging and thought-provoking. What would happen if we only saw the world upside-down? For more information you may contact the author or TOPSS c/o APA’s Education Directorate. References: Kohler, I. (1962, May). Experiments with goggles. Scientific American, 206, 6272. Cited in Hothersall, D. (1985). Psychology. Columbus, Ohio: Charles Merrill Publishing. Sratton, G. (1897). Vision without inversion of the retinal image. Psychological Review, 4, 463-481. Cited in Hothersall, D. (1985). Psychology. Columbus, Ohio: Charles Merrill Publishing. Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 7 NEWS FROM TOPSS TOPSS Annual Essay Contest: The APF Presidential Scholars Competition By Carol Dean, TOPSS Essay Chair There are some exciting changes in the TOPSS Essay Contest for this year. Dr. Martin E. P. Seligman and TOPSS, in conjunction with the American Psychological Foundation (APF), has designed a competition to identify the brightest and best high school psychology students in America. There will be three student winners. These winners, each with one parent/guardian and one sponsoring teacher, will receive funding to attend the APA Convention in San Francisco in August 1998. Transportation, lodging, and convention expenses for these winners, parents/guardians, and teachers will be covered by APA for up to $1,000 per person. Dr. Seligman’s presidential theme for 1998, “maximizing psychological strength, resilience, and genius,” is reflected in the competition question described below. A variety of groups are working toward having the first week of March identified as Psychology Awareness Week. It is our intent to have the winners announced on approximately March 4, 1998 to coincide with that week’s festivities. Entries for the contest must be postmarked by January 26, 1998. To compete in the APF Presidential Scholars competition a student must answer BOTH PARTS of the stimulus item below. Winners will be selected on the basis of a demonstrated ability to (1) complete a critical analysis and synthesis of empirical research and (2) generate a quality research proposal that is a logical outgrowth of the review of the research literature. Deadline: All essays must be postmarked absolutely no later than January 26, 1998. IMPORTANT NOTE: There should be NO identification pertaining to the student on any page of the paper. Violation of this rule will result in disqualification. Numbering pages is permitted. 8 Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 THE QUESTION (complete both parts) Part I: Conduct and report a critical analysis of the literature on the following topic: Parental/caretaker behaviors can lead to the development and maintenance of optimism in children. Describe how psychologists measure optimism in children and the positive outcomes of optimism from social and educational perspectives. Your report should include: A. An operational definition of what psychologists mean by optimism B. An application of relevant psychological findings from a minimum of three sources C. An explanation of how psychologists know that these parental/caretaker behaviors lead to optimism D. A resource list in APA style Part II: Based on your literature review, generate a research design that addresses issues regarding early life experiences. Your research proposal should reflect the topic studied in Part I and should include: A. A statement of the problem B. An operationally defined hypothesis C. A description of the methodology 1. Identification of proposed method (experiment, survey, etc.) 2. Identification of population and subject pool 3. Identification of important variables (IV, DV, confounding) 4. An explanation of efforts to control potential confounding variables 5. An explanation of how data will be collected 6. An explanation of how data will be analyzed D. A discussion of potential ethical issues E. A prediction of results RULES Eligibility: • Entrants must be high school students. • Entrants must have been enrolled or be presently See Competition, following page New TOPSS Members Arizona Jean Schroeder-McClure, Cibola HS, Yuma California Anthony Carsaro, Palmdale Emmanuel Deligiannis, Orange Verconica Perez-Marchese, Downey Cat Rickets, Elk Grove Marsha Robinson, Boulder Creek Jennifer Tuck, San Francisco District of Columbia John Crane, International School of Prague, US Embassy Prague Olen Kalkus, International School of Prague, US Embassy Prague Florida Maricel Castillo, Miami Stefanie Scher, Boca Raton Todd Sparger, Spruce Creek HS, Port Orange Jodie Weinstein, Miami Northwestern HS, Miami Georgia James Watts, Smyrna Idaho Angela Bergman, Burley Nikki Sayer, Moore Illinois Barbara Bonnet, East Dubuque Matt Niehaus, LaSalle Indiana Kathy Lehe, Frontier HS, Chamlers Iowa Carolyn Clefisch, Guttenberg Kansas Timothy Adams, Hesston North Dakota Deborah S. Medlar, Dickinson Maryland Chris Kinworthey, College Park Claudia Mowen, Annapolis Ohio Steven Boone, Covington Parran Curry-Guy, Columbus Massachusetts Jack Merrill, W. Newton Thomas Miller, David Prouty HS, Spencer Dennis Tavares, Kingston Oregon Laura Dale, Eugene Michigan Patricia Burnside, Siginaw J. Gary Cockman, Davison HS, Davison Cherilyn Ross, Novi Minnesota Dan Elhard, Grand Rapids Brett Joyce, LeCener Montana Joann DeBeaumont, Glendive Nebraska Jan Beeson, East HS, Lincoln Rose Kastl, SE HS, Lincoln Tim Shipman, Omaha New Jersey Jennifer Potschantek, Oakland Mark Schweizer, Clementon Jack Wilson, Towaco New York Cheryl Jordan, E Northport Beth Pollatz, Brookville Laura Sposato, Greenlawn Douglas Weisman, Eastchester HS, Eastchester Competition, from previous page enrolled in a high school psychology course. • Entrants must be sponsored by a TOPSS member. The Paper: • Papers must be typed or word-processed. • Papers must be no more than 3,000 words in length. Papers exceeding this length will be disqualified. • Papers MUST use APA style. • Papers must be submitted in triplicate. • Papers that are word-processed should be submitted with a computer disk. The wordprocessing program used must be identified. The Procedure: • No more than ONE paper per school may Pennsylvania Paul Cech, California David Zimmerman, Conestaga HS, Berwyn South Dakota Darcy Randle, Piedmont Texas Amy LeClair, Sugarland Mary J. O'Brien, Katy Virginia Jean Ferguson, Centreville Washington Jeffrey Beha, Franklin Pierce HS, Tacoma International Holly Drover, Ontario, Canada Frederick Scott Galt, The American Community School, Cobham Surrey, England Joelle Myszkowki, Colegio Americano de Quito, Quito, Eucador Gunter Schweiger, Univ. of Economics-Vienna, Austria be submitted. • All entries must include a cover sheet with the following information: A. The student's name B. The student's year in school (e.g., junior, senior) C. The name, address, and phone number of the TOPSS sponsor D. The student's school name, address and phone number. E. Send the paper to: Seligman Presidential Scholar Competition c/o Peter Petrossian Education Directorate American Psychological Association 750 First Street, NE Washington, DC, 20002 Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 9 ACTIVITY Inquiries, Demonstrations, Experiments and Activities An Exercise in Signal Detection Theory By Alan Feldman, Perth Amboy High School, Perth Amboy, NJ Concept: Materials: Signal detection theory can illustrate distortions in the measurement of perceptual detection by taking into account receiver operator characteristics such as mood, context and personal expectations. (This activity should be used after absolute threshold theory has been discussed and after the section on signal detection has been assigned to be read). Overhead transparency of Chart A Copy of Questions 1-3 for each student Chart A RESPONSE Instructions: SIGNAL YES NO PRESENT HIT MISS ABSENT FALSE ALARM CORRECT REJECTION Discuss the following four outcomes in a game of poker. A. Opponent has good hand - you think they do. (hit) You fold. B. Opponent has good hand - you think they do. (miss) You fold. C. Opponent has bad hand - you think its actually good. (false alarm) You've been bluffed. D. Opponent has bad hand - you think it's bad. (correct rejection) Raise them. Then show students Chart A on overhead and have them apply categories - hit, miss, false alarm, correct rejection to above situation. Have students work individually or in small groups on questions 1 - 3. I. Which outcome is exemplified by each of the following scenarios: A. A stream has just been stocked with fish. A fisherman who has been unsuccessful at this location in the past only tries for five minutes and then gives up. B. He then travels to a nearby pond that has no fish in it. A minute after throwing his line in he snags what he thinks is a big fish. It is actually a boot. 2. A. A babysitter detects frightening noises after watching a scary movie. The noise is the trees rustling in the wind. B. The next day the same babysitter is at soccer practice. She does not detect the trees nearby making the same noise. 3. A young male college student goes to a sorority dance hoping to meet a young woman. Discuss the 4 possible outcomes that could occur according to signal detection theory (e.g. A very attractive, unattached young woman smiles at him and makes eye contact. He does not perceive she is interested in him. This would be a miss - the signal is present but he does not respond to it.) See Activity, page 13 Psychology Teacher Network is looking for good ideas, activities and experiments to share with our readers. Please submit any activities to Psychology Teacher Network, Education Directorate. 10 Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 Division Two 1998 Teaching Awards Nominations The Society for the Teaching of Psychology (Division Two) of the American Psychological Association announces its 1998 program of awards for outstanding teachers of psychology. Teaching awards of $500 and a plaque are bestowed for outstanding performance in each of four environments: 1.Four-year Colleges or Universities (Robert S. Daniel Award) 2.Two-year Colleges 3.High Schools (Moffett Memorial Teaching Award) 4.Graduate Student (McKeachie Early Career Teaching Award) Criteria Nominations will be judged on the following criteria, although nominees will not be expected to have achieved recognition in all areas: 1. Demonstrated influence in interesting students in the field of psychology. Documentation could include papers or projects completed by students; evidence of students presenting papers at professional meetings or of students subsequently publishing their work done with this teacher; actions by the teacher, both curricular and cocurricular, intended to increase student interest and involvement in psychology. 2. Development of effective teaching methods, courses, and/or teaching materials. Documentation could include description and sample of methods, materials, course syllabi, and evidence of their successful utilization. 3. Outstanding performance as a classroom teacher. Documentation could include student evaluations, enrollment figures, evaluative observations by colleagues, teaching awards, and other forms of prior recognition. 4. Concerns with professional identity as a teacher of psychology. This might be seen as professional activity on or away from campus, including publication of articles on teaching, stimulation of student research, attendance at professional meetings or workshops relevant to the teaching of psychology, or memberships in organizations indicating such an identification. Winners will be notified in advance. Awards will be presented at a special Society for the Teaching of Psychology program at the 1998 meeting of the American Psychological Association in San Francisco. If a winner cannot attend, the Society for the Teaching of Psychology will appoint a representative to make the presentation at the winner's home campus at a time convenient to the winner. The nominator and nominee must compile all documentation in support of the nomination. Responsibility for accumulating supporting documentation cannot be assumed by the Awards Committee. Nominations are to be sent to the Chairperson of the Awards Committee and must be received by January 5, 1998. Materials will not ordinarily be returned. Persons nominated need not be members of The Society for the Teaching of Psychology and renominations and self-nominations are acceptable. The Society for the Teaching of Psychology reserves the right to not make an award in a particular category if the Awards Committee believes no nominee is qualified in a particular year. Nomination Format Cover Sheet A cover sheet should accompany the nominator’s documentation. This sheet should include: 1. Nominee's name, address, and telephone number. 2. Category of award for which this person is nominated. 3. Nominator's name, address, and telephone number. 4. The name and address of who (Department Head, Dean, President, etc.) and/or what officer and organization (local, state, or regional psychological association, etc.) should be informed if the nominee wins the Society for the Teaching of Psychology Teaching Award. Materials to be Submitted * 5. Educational history of the nominee: degrees, granting institutions, year(s) granted, major and minor fields. * 6. Nominee's professional work history. * 7. Professional and honorary organizations in which the nominee holds membership. 8. Letters of recommendation from at least three (but not more than five) persons, each of whom can speak to the qualifications of the nominee in light of the criteria. 9. Narrative: A maximum of two pages (doublespaced) of documentation per criterion high See Nominations, page 12 * These items may simply be highlighted on the nominee's curriculum vitae if one is available. Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 11 DEAR DOCTOR Question: I saw a demonstration where a pound of feathers was perceived as lighter than a pound of lead. The two should feel the same, but they did not. Can you explain this? Submitted by: David Powers, Omaha, NE PHOTO B FPO Clifford Fawl, Ph.D Answered by: Clifford Fawl, Ph.D., Nebraska Wesleyan University A panel of noted clinical, experimental and academic psychologists has graciously agreed to reply in this column to questions submitted by teachers and students. We invite you to send your questions to: DEAR DOCTOR, PTN, Education Directorate, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002-4242 Perhaps it will help if I relate the illusion to which you refer to another illusion that may be more familiar to you. You probably have seen in a textbook a figure (perhaps a medium gray rectangle) surrounded either by a light gray ground or a dark gray ground. Figure A (the one surrounded by light gray) will appear darker than Figure B (the one surrounded by dark gray) even though Figures A and B are physically identical. It helps little, if any, to know that they are identical; they still are perceived as different. This is because perception of the figure is affected by the figure-ground relationship, not just by the figure itself. We perceive in terms of relationships. In a sense, Figure A is darker than Figure B: Figure A is darker in relationship to its ground than Figure B is to its ground, and this is the way we perceive it. The same idea pertains to the illusion you ask about, which is known as the sizeweight illusion. In this case we can think of weight as the figure and size (or volume) as the ground. We can take two cans of identical weight but different height as our example, in which case Can A (the smaller can of higher density) will appear heavier in weight when lifted than Can B (the larger can of lower density). As before, even if we are told that the cans are equal in weight they still don't appear to be. Again, there is a sense in which Can A is heavier than Can B: Can A is heavier in relationship to its size than Can B is to its size. To test the effect of size on perception of weight you can have one group of students compare the perceived weights with their eyes closed so they cannot at any time see the size of the cans and the other group compare the cans with their eyes open. You will find that the group that judges the weights with their eyes closed will on average be more accurate. The illusory effect of the size-weight illusion is not as universal as that of the brightness illusion discussed earlier. This is because in the brightness illusion it is virtually impossible to separate the ground from the figure whereas in the size-weight illusion it can be done by closing one's eyes or simply by not looking at the cans as they are being judged for weight. But even here, if the participant initially looks at the cans and thus knows their relative size, the size will continue to have some effect, although not as much. Most participants, however, look at the cans as they judge them for weight, in which case the size is likely to have a major influence on the perception of weight. In my own demonstration I present my students with a set of small (film) canisters of equal size but ranging in weight from 25 grams to 175 grams, in increments of 25 grams, and one much larger can that weighs 150 grams. (I vary the weights of the canisters by placing varying amounts of metals in them.) I ask the participant to pick out the canister which weighs exactly the same as the larger can. Rarely does a student select the canister that weighs 150 grams. Most students will choose one that weighs 75 grams (half the weight of the larger can). A smaller number will select either the 100-gram canister or the 50gram canister as equal in weight to the larger can of 150 grams. A few will even pick the 25-gram canister as equal to it! Clearly the participants are responding to density, which is a relationship (weight in relation to volume), rather than to weight alone. Don’t Forget...Time to renew your TOPSS membership! Nominations, from page 11 lighting the nominee's achievements with respect to one or more of the criteria listed above. 10. Optional: Items 1-9 are the primary pieces of information to be examined by the Awards committee. However, nominators may submit additional materials in support of the narrative portion of the nomination. Optional items might include teacher ratings, teaching devices, syllabi, student papers, or newspaper articles. 12 Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 Send materials to: Mary Kite, Ph.D., Chair, The Society for the Teaching of Psychology Teaching Awards Committee, Department of Psychological Science Ball State University, Muncie, IN 47306-0520. Dr. Kite can also be reached at (765) 285-1702 or via email at mkite@wp.bsu.edu. REVIEW “Laughing Matters” Edited by: Rebecca Hantin Publisher: Baxley Media Group, 110 West Main, Urbana IL 61801-2700, (217) 384-4838 The mentally ill in our society seem to carry with them a badge of dishonor. This stigma can affect many aspects of their lives. Relationships with family, friends, and co-workers may be strained. In addition, the ability to obtain and maintain employment is difficult. How does an individual facing such private and public reprisals deal with such stigma? For one group, laughter has been found to be the best weapon in destroying stigma. "The Project Return Players" are an ensemble of actors struggling with mental illness and the resulting labels. The video, Laughing Matters provides a stage allowing them to demonstrate, through humor, what the mentally ill must face. Performance clips include a "Psychiatric Fashion Show" where one member of the troupe "models" the "31-Flavor Electric Shock Mouthpiece" featuring "tutti-frutti and strawberry...all your favorites." Actors also portray the experiences of "Kelly", a teen experiencing auditory hallucina- Date of Publication: 1995 Length: 32 minutes Reviewed by: Kristin Whitlock, Bountiful, UT tions. This is effectively done. As Kelly tries to talk on the phone, the "voices”, which are the rest of the ensemble, speak nonsense, drown out and take over her thoughts, and render her immobile. Her "parents" emotional response to their daughter's psychotic episode rings true. In addition to performance clips, viewers are introduced to the players through interviews. The actors discuss their illnesses and personal experiences with prejudice and discrimination. I found it interesting that many actors reported negative experiences, not only with society, but also among mental health professionals. As one actor states, "The system wants to keep reminding you that you are [mentally ill]." Through humor and glimpses into their personal experiences, the message the ensemble attempts to leave with its audiences, both live and in video, is that despite their problems, "We are children of the Universe" and they encourage all of us to "take a moment to remember those hurting out there." Overall, I found Laughing Matters to be insightful. I enjoyed the performances and found that the interviews provided an interesting glimpse into the lives of these mentally ill actors. However, the sound quality is poor and at some points it is difficult to understand what is being said. I believe Laughing Matters could be a teaching resource at the high school level. My main concern, however, is time. With so much to discuss and do in a year or semester, I believe there are other sources I would prefer to use to touch on these issues. In a class focusing on the issue of labeling, this video may be appropriate. For example, in discussing Rosenhan's research on labeling ("On Being Sane in Insane Places," SCIENCE, Jan. 19, 1973, vol. 179, p. 250-58) this would provide some additional discussion as to how society views those who are mentally ill. Activity, from page 10 Discussion: A. Lead students in a discussion of how signal detection theory can measure both sensitivity (sensory impressions) and response bias (decision processes). Discuss how the receiver's attitudes, moods and momentary emotional states could affect their outcome distribution. B. Review type 1 and type 2 errors. Which outcome best typifies a type 1 error? A type 2 error? C. Describe the advantage of signal detection theory as compared to threshold analyses in evaluating a hypothetical experiment in which an experimenter is asked to listen for a weak tone. (List the four possible outcomes that can occur on each trial of the experiment according to signal detection theory.) For more information concerning signal detection theory, see the textbook Sensation and Perception, 3rd edition, by Matlin and Foley, published by Allyn and Bacon and Experimental Psychology: Understanding Psychological Research, by Kantowitz, Roedigger and Elmes published by West. Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 13 ANNOUNCEMENTS Advanced Placement Workshops The College Board, via its regional offices, is sponsoring one-day conferences on the psychology Advanced Placement course. For more information or to register, call or write your local College Board office. All workshops are subject to cancellation due to inadequate registration. Midwest Regional Office Western Regional Office APP, 1800 Sherman Avenue #401 Evanston, IL 60201 (847) 866-1700 Suite 480, 2099 Gateway Place San Jose, CA 95110-1017 (408) 452-1400 November 18, 1997 University of Minnesota, Saint Paul, MN February 7, 1998 Aragaon High School, San Mateo, CA March 10, 1998 Calvin College, Grand Rapids, MI January 21, 1998 Alhambra High School, Alhambra, CA March 19, 1998 Triton Community College, River Grove, IL March 28, 1998 UC Irvine, Irvine, CA March 20, 1998 Triton Community College, River Grove, IL November 8, 1997 Cherry Creek High School, Englewood, CO March 26, 1998 University of Nebraska, Omaha, NE February 14, 1998 Roosevelt High School, Portland, OR April 27, 1998 Southern Illinois University, Edwardsville, IL Southern Regional Office April 29, 1998 University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 2970 Clairmont Road, Suite 250 Atlanta, GA 30329-1639 (770) 908-9737 Middle States Regional Office November 17, 1997 Holiday Inn, Ft. Lauderdale, FL Suite 410, 3440 Market Street Philadelphia, PA 19104-3338 (215) 387-7600 December 12, 1997 Pace University, New York, NY January 16, 1998 New York University, New York, NY January 17, 1998 Upper Perkiomen High School, Pennsburg, PA April 25, 1998 Montclair College, Montclair, NJ January 8, 1998 Oglethorpe University, Atlanta, GA Southwestern Regional Office 98 San Jacinto Boulevard, Suite 1050 Austin, TX 78701-4039 (512) 472-0231 January 16, 1998 Edward H. White Senior High School, Jacksonville, FL Next Issue: • Briefing on Pediatric Psychology • TOPSS Election Results 14 Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 ANNOUNCEMENTS Conferences for Teachers of Psychology All conferences are designed to encourage academic linkages between psychology faculty at the graduate, four-year, two-year, and high school levels. These conferences provide participants with an opportunity to discuss issues and to share ideas relating to their profession. The format consists of invited addresses, concurrent sessions, and participant poster sessions. Twentieth Annual National Institute on the Teaching of Psychology Contact: Doug Bernstein, Ph.D. (217) 333-4731 Tradewinds Hotel St. Petersburg Beach, FL January 3-6, 1998 Tenth Annual Southeastern Conference on the Teaching of Psychology Contact: William Hill, Ph.D. (770) 423-6225 fax (770) 423-6432 email: BHILL@KSCMAIL.KENNESAW.EDU Department of Psychology Kennesaw State University Marietta, Georgia February 20-22, 1998 The registration fee for the conference is $110.00 for the first participant from an institution and $100.00 for each additional participant from that institution; graduate students and high school teachers $65.00. Deadline for registration is February 6, 1998. Fifth Annual Midwest Institute for Teachers of Psychology Contacts: David Shavalia, Ph.D. or Patricia Puccio, Ph.D. (630) 942-2187 or (630) 942-2325 fax (630) 858-5424 Department of Psychology College of DuPage Glen Ellyn, IL 60137 February 27-28, 1998 Fee: $95 covers conference registration and meals for both days; hotel accommodations available at a reduced rate. Eleventh Annual Conference on Undergraduate Teaching of Psychology Contact: Dr. Gene Indenbaum (516) 420-2725 fax: (516) 420-2452 email: indenbea@snyfarva.cc.farmingdale.edu Nevele Hotel Ellenville, New York March 18-20, 1998 Nebraska Wesleyan University Psychology Teachers Institute Please help us to make sure that you continue receiving Free Graduate Credit, Room and Board! Outstanding National Speakers! Psychology Teacher Network Dates: June 21 - July 3, 1998 For application materials contact: Ken Keith Department of Psychology Nebraska Wesleyan University 5000 Saint Paul Avenue Lincoln, NE 68504-2796 email: kdk@NebrWesleyan.edu are planning a move, don't and other APA materials. If you have recently moved or forget to notify PTN of the change of address so that delivery will not be interrupted. The address for PTN can be found on the back cover of the newsletter. Psychology Teacher Network November-December 1997 15