Psychology Teacher Network is published by the Education Directorate of the American Psychological Association. Subscriptions are free to High School Teacher Affiliates of the APA and APA Members and $15 a year for all others. Address editorial correspondence to Psychology Teacher Network, Education Directorate, APA, 750 First St., N.E., Washington, DC 20002-4242; (202) 336-6021. Address inquiries regarding membership or affiliation to the Membership Office, APA, at the same address. Senior Editor ...........................................................Julie Goldstein jgoldstein@apa.org TOPSS Editor..................................................................Mary Spilis mspilis@aol.com Two-Year College Editor...................................Martha Ellis, Ph.D. mellis@fs9host.ccccd.edu APA’s Pre-College and Undergraduate Officer.......................................Peter Petrossian ppetrossian@apa.org Psychology Teacher Network Education Directorate American Psychological Association 750 First Street, NE Washington, DC 20002-4242 Nonprofit US Postage Paid Washington, DC Permit No. 6348 For Teachers of Introductory Psychology APA EDUCATION DIRECTORATE September-October 1998 • Volume 8 • Issue 4 PTN PSYCHOLOGY TEACHER NETWORK 1998 Society for the Teaching of Psychology Award Winners Announced The Society for the Teaching of Psychology (Division Two) celebrated the 19th year of its annual Teaching Awards Program at the August convention of the American Psychological Association in San Francisco, California. The 1998 winners were announced during the Division’s social hour; the winners received a plaque and a check for $500 at that time. Recognition for outstanding teaching was given in each of the following categories: (a) Robert S. Daniels Award (4 year college or university professor), (b) Two Year College, (c) Moffett Memorial Award (high school teacher), and (d) McKeachie Early Career (graduate student). Robert S. Daniels Award Inside: Briefing . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 A New Trend in Teaching Introduction to Psychology. . . . . . . . . 6 Goodies for Psychology Teachers . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 Alternatives to the Use of Animals in Education . . . . . . . . . . . . PRISM II Conference . . . Activity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Dear Doctor . . . . . . . . . . Review . . . . . . . . . . . . . . New TOPSS Members. . 8 9 11 12 13 14 The winner of the Robert S. Daniels Award for outstanding teaching in a 4-year College or University is William G. Graziano, Professor of Psychology at Texas A&M University. Dr. Graziano earned a B.A. from Franklin and Marshall College and a Ph.D. in Social Psychology from the University of Minnesota. While at the University of Georgia, Dr. Graziano won three Psi Chi teaching awards. He also has won the Sandy Beaver Teaching Award, an honor given by the Franklin College of Arts and Sciences at the University of Georgia, and several other awards. Dr. Graziano’s nomination materials highlight his many characteristics: accessible, prepared, supportive, energetic, professional, a scholar with breadth and depth. He nurtures careers by collaborating with young re- searchers; Dr. Graziano has prsented over 40 papers that have been co-authored with undergraduate or graduate students and he has published A over 20 articles with FPO students. William Graziano, Ph.D. Dr. Graziano’s research credentials are stellar; he has numerous publications, has received major grants from the National Science Foundation and the National Institute of Mental Health, and has served or is serving on editorial boards for several major journals in psychology. Yet as his work with students makes clear, his research can not be easily separated from his teaching; both are central to his identity as a professor. While teaching graduate students, for example, he often points to research papers and topics that would appeal to undergraduates, thus concurrently teaching the discipline itself and how to teach it to others. One nominee ended his letter as follows: “...One of his singular contributions to psychology is the energy he puts into his teaching and the model of scholarship he provides his students. I am honored to know Bill as a friend and colleague and to nominate him for this much deserved award.” For these qualities and more, we recognize Dr. Graziano with the Robert S. Daniels Award. See Awards, Page 5 BRIEFING Recent Developments in Close Relationships Theory and Research By John H. Harvey, Ph.D., University of Iowa Paper presented at the 1998 American Psychological Association annual convention, San Francisco, CA. Background In the following paper, I provide a general, selective review of some major developments in the field of close relationships (CRs). Particular attention is given to ideas and findings that might be incorporated into lectures on CRs in secondary school offerings of psychology. As a social psychologist, I have taught CR courses for 20 years, beginning at Vanderbilt University in 1978—a course that was one of the first formal undergraduate offerings on this topic in a psychology department in the U.S. The reason that CRs was not a part of psychology curriculum before the late 1970s was that the area of work did not develop until close to the 1980s. It had not developed prior to that point because, I believe, the discipline of psychology ironically enough demurred in viewing this topic as scientific enough. By contrast, the field of family sociology had been in existence almost half a century before psychology began to study close relationships in earnest. Now there are two interdisciplinary scholarly organizations and two related journals focusing on close relationships. A recent, comprehensive review of the field may be found in Duck (1997). My close relationships class at Iowa each term subscribes a maximum enrollment of 125 juniors and seniors. It involves two months of lectures and one month of group presentations (with about 9 presentations, presented by groups of 10 to 12 students each) on books such as Bernard’s (1982) The Future of Marriage. The students seem to appreciate this format, and in the presentations can teach one another. Presentations involve videos, panels, and skits drawn from peer interaction, family dialogue, and the media. I am convinced that my relationship course has had such a strong following over the years because relationships dynamics are so vital a part of the lives of people in their twenties—not to say they are not vital in all of our lives. According to a recent Gallup survey (USA Today “snapshot” 4-14-98) of young adults’ (ages 18-34) priorities, 83% rated a close-knit family as their highest priority—greater than the 68% who rated a career the highest. This elevated focus on relationships has evolved from the turbulence so many 2 Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 young people have seen in their own families, personal lives, and friends’ lives for so many generations. Census Bureau statistics concerning flux in relationships and the family over the last four decades attest to the changes from some of this turbulence, including: (1) Since 1960, the number of divorces has tripled. It is estimated that there will be 1 million divorces in 1998, involving more than 1 million children. There are 70 million people in the U.S. who have been divorced at least once. (2) Today the average marriage lasts 7 years, and there are approximately 27 million children under 18 living with only their mothers, up from 8 million in 1960. (3) At least 150,000 divorces, or 1 in 7, will involve custody battles. (4) The number of single-parent families has more than quadrupled since 1960. (5) Several million grandparents in the U.S. now are recycling as parents due to their children’s need to have childcare due to divorces, work-situations, being in prison, or for other reasons. Also, family sociologists have given us the term “starter marriage” for all the beginning marriages that end in divorce after a few years and in which no children are involved—the idea being that a first marriage is somewhat like a first home, from which one usually moves on! Definition I define a CR as “a relationship that has extended over some period of time and involves a mutual understanding of closeness and mutual behavior that is seen by the couple as indicative of closeness.” According to this definition, empty shell and convenience marriages do not constitute CRS, nor do perceptions or acts by only one party. Implied as a key part in this definition of CRs is an emphasis on interdependence in people’s thoughts, feelings, and behaviors; their lives are intertwined in myriad ways. A Revolution: Gender and The Family Since the late 1960s, a revolution in how CRs are carried out, the nature and functioning of the family, and in sexuality has occurred in the West and many parts of the world. The hallmarks of this revolution are greater equality between women and men, greater freedom, and consent-mutual decision-making. Writers & scholars such as Jessie Bernard, Barbara Eichenreich, and Pepper Schwartz have been pioneers in advocating egalitarian relationships and in effecting this revolution. More specifically, Bernard (1982) in The Future of Marriage argued that traditional marriage made women psychologically sick because it typically kept them at home and unfulfilled, both professionally and personally. She also argued that such marriages contributed to men’s health and success. Eichenreich and colleagues in a 1986 book Re-making Love denounced the sexual double standard. They advocated and foresaw sexual behavior patterns among women that were as open and pluralistic as found among men both in and out of marriage. Sexual behavior data over the last 2 decades attest to Eichenreich et al.’s prescience in predicting women’s much greater sexual activity pre-maritally and extra-maritally. In her (1994) book Peer Marriage, Schwartz contends that couples in the U.S. increasingly are adopting versions of egalitarian relationships and trying to balance each partner’s needs and abilities, whether in work or family life. Schwartz based her argument on a sample of 56 peer, role-sharing marriages spread across the U.S. She found four common features for peer marriages: (1) the division of household labor stayed within a 40/60 split for labor and responsibility; (2) both spouses believed they had equal influence over important and disputed decisions; (3) both partners felt each had equal control over family economic resources and equal access to discretionary spending; and (4) each partner’s work was given equal weight in the couple’s life plans. Schwartz described deep friendship and primacy of the relationship as rewards of peer relations. She also said that social support still is rare for such intense commitment to each other and to sharing. Coontz’s (1992) valuable work took issue with some people’s belief or memory that the 1950s represented a time of stability and happiness in the family and marriage because, in part, the divorce rate was lower in that decade relative to the WWII years and subsequent decades. She pointed to the high incidence of drinking among men in relationships in the 1950s and the frequency of abuse that often went unreported. In effect, she suggested that there may have been some secret dark recesses in the “Leave It To Beaver” families of those years. The debilitating effects of divorce on many children and families have led to some states’ development of the covenant marriage law. This law makes divorce more difficult, and presumably is in the interests of creating stronger families. As critics have argued, however, the issues of abuse and deeply problematic patterns in relationships and whether covenant marriages will make it more difficult to divorce have yet to be addressed adequately by lawmakers supporting such laws. The Development of Close Relationships People are using whatever ways they can to meet others for closeness in the 1990s. Consider this advertisement from the Washington Post about a woman seen for a split second: “1225 Connecticut Ave. lobby Friday afternoon, 1/30, you tall, beautiful brunette, long skirt, talking to security, work for building? Me: blonde, male, black/brown leather jacket, our eyes locked. Nobody knows who you are, please call—” (Washington Post ad, 2-22-98). What are the early variables in determining whether people start the courting process? At the top of the list for relationship development in early dating is physical attraction and matching our physical attraction with those with whom we might be close. The matching hypothesis “The says that while we may prefer a more attractive person in the abstract, in reality debilitating we end up choosing someone who is close effects of to our own level of attractiveness (Berscheid & Walster, 1978). divorce on many The physical attractiveness variable children and often acts like a filtering variable and, after it is fulfilled, other variables such as families have led similarity of attitudes and backgrounds to some states’ become important. Indeed, this filter idea was presented by Bernard Murstein development (1970) as the way relationships often of the covenant develop: passing through the physical attractiveness filter to the similarity in marriage law.” values and attitudes filter, and then to a role filter in which people may play complementary roles in relationships. Hendrick and Hendrick (1993) show in their book Romantic Love that romance is a valued part of relationships, especially early and then at different points throughout the history of relationships. The Hendricks have done considerable work showing that people exhibit romantic love styles, including: eros (strong sexual desire), storge (companionship), ludus (game-playing), mania (jealousy), and pragma (practical considerations). Berscheid & Walster (1978) distinguish between romantic or passionate and companionate love. The former is filled with feelings of lust for other, that other is the one and only other partner for you, and that you cannot live without other; the latter is oriented toward friendship and companionship. Another love style model has evolved from Hazan and Shaver’s (1987) influential linking of adult love and early attachment patterns. They argue that early attachment experiences with mothers and parents Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 3 carry over to latter adult relationships and that some people are successful because they have learned to be secure, while others are less successful because they have learned to be ambivalent or avoidant in social relations. The Maintenance/Enhancement of Close Relationships What do we know about how to maintain and grow close relationships over long periods? There is strong evidence that people are healthier, happier, and live longer if they have satisfying CRs (Argyle, 1987). In a valuable research program concerned with examining the process of conflict, John Gottman (1995) argues that what he calls the “4 horsemen of “In a valuable the apocalypse,” contempt, criticism, defensiveness, and stonewalling conresearch program tribute to the endings of most CRs. More concerned with recently, Gottman and colleagues (1998) have compared several models of examining the predicting marital happiness among process of conflict, newlyweds and concluded that most couples who are satisfied figure out ways John Gottman to gently raise issues and de-escalate argues that what negativity. In addition to therapy and effective he calls the address of ongoing problems, a lot of ‘4 horsemen of work now concerns prevention programs. An example of a successful prevention the apocalypse,’ counseling program is the PREP apcontempt, criticism, and proach (Prevention and Relationship Enhancement Program) created by Howard defensiveness, Markman and colleagues at the Univerand stonewalling sity of Denver. They have shown the value of premarital counseling aimed at contribute to inducing young persons to be more open the endings of in expressing their desires in relating and in discussing their pasts (Markman, most CRs.” Stanley, & Blumberg, 1994). Julia Omarzu and I have proposed a theory we refer to as “minding the close relationship” (1997). This theory posits that in order for two committed people to achieve relationship closeness, it is necessary to have: a never-ending reciprocal pattern of behavior aimed at knowing one’s partner and allowing one’s partner to know you, attribution (i.e., interpretive inferences) about a partner’s traits, motives, and behavior that is fair and positive, and acceptance of and respect for what one discovers about one’s partner. Endings of Close Relationships, Grief, and Recovery Amato and Booth (1998) conclude from their survey research that only one-third of marriages with high 4 Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 levels of conflict end in divorce. They suggest that there are broad negative impacts on children of divorce, and that too many parents are unnecessarily getting divorced—even though grounds such as hate and discord do not exist. They say that children sometimes benefit from divorce if parents are constantly quarreling. For the majority of marriages ending in divorce that they studied, the parents were getting along fairly well. Thus, divorce in such marriages may contribute to children’s increased psychological distress and lesser ties with kin. These effects, therefore, are not necessary if the marriage could have been readily salvaged. Amato and Booth speculate that Americans are leaving “nonconflicted marriages” earlier because of their acceptance of divorce, unrealistic expectations regarding marriage, and for a virtual “kitchen sink” of reasons. Amato and Booth’s argument, however, requires careful scrutiny. As Gottman (1995), Harvey (1995), and other theorists and therapists have argued and shown, many marriages involve subtle, difficult-toobserve types of negativity, such that children and partners are damaged by the processes unfolding. Further, even with the archival data provided by Amato and Booth, it is difficult to conclude that only one-third of all divorces occurring involve prolonged, intense conflict. In his book Marital Separation, Weiss (1975) published a classic study of the role of people’s reliance on similar others and telling their stories of marital separation as a mean of coping with such loss. This work, as well as research on nonmarital romances (Harvey, 1995), suggests that recovery from a dissolved close relationship can be a laborious process, not unlike recovering from the death of a close other. Work on adaptation to such losses points to the value of people’s private emotional-cognitive work to recognize and understand their losses, and the confiding of their stories of loss to close, highly empathic others (Harvey,1996). The Future of Research on and Teaching about Close Relationships Given the problems inherent in relating and achieving closeness, the future of the CR field of scholarship is one in which the fervor of theory, research, and debate is ensured for decades to come. Recognizing the value of this topic and this fervor, I hope that educators will be encouraged to bring the work of this field to the high school classroom for students to consider. It may not be too much to hope that our close relationships will be enhanced via such dialogue and, in effect, our families and institutions will be more thoughtfully appraised and constructed in the twenty-first century. Awards, from page 1 Two Year College The recipient of the award for outstanding teaching in a two year college is Mark W. Vernoy, Associate Professor of Behavioral Sciences at Palomar College in San Marcos, California. Dr. Vernoy earned his B.A. in psychology from Occidental College and his Ph.D. in psychology from University of California, Irvine. Dr. Vernoy’s skills as an excellent teacher have been apparent since graduate school, where he taught labB oratory sections in Experimental PsyFPO chology and his own course in perception. His innovative classroom demonstrations, such as using magic to teach Mark W. Vernoy, Ph.D. classical conditioning, have been published in Teaching of Psychology and reprinted in several instructor’s manuals. His teaching excellence is appreciated by both colleagues and students and has led to national recognition: Dr. Vernoy is the recipient of the 1997 Excellence Award from the National Institute of Staff and Organizational Development (NISOD) and was also named a Master Teacher in 1986 by that group. Dr. Vernoy teaches a variety of courses, including statistics, experimental psychology and methodology, cognitive psychology, and physiological psychology. He is noted for his ability to present difficult material in an engaging, accessible way. Dr. Vernoy has translated his engaging presentation style into his written work as well. He publishes regularly in teaching journals, is a frequent presenter at national teaching conferences, and has co-authored several editions of two successful textbooks, Psychology in Action (4th ed.) and Behavioral Statistics in Action. The integration of technology into teaching has been Dr. Vernoy’s longstanding goal. As early as 1986, Dr. Vernoy was integrating computers into his physiological psychology course. A 1994 Teaching of Psychology paper outlines a computerized Stroop experiment for demonstrating a 2 X 3 factorial design. In 1987, he co-authored PsychAid: Computer tutorials for psychology in action, a computer program published by John Wiley and Sons. He has also developed a statistical package for introductory statistics courses. Although this award focuses on Dr. Vernoy’s teaching, his colleagues are quick to point to his excellence in research and his exemplary service to Palomar College. Dr. Vernoy is an exceptional professor and colleague and has well earned the recognition provided by the Society’s Teaching Award. Moffett Memorial Award The 1998 Moffett Memorial Award winner for outstanding psychology teaching in high schools is Margaret Davidson of L. V. Berkner High School in Richardson, Texas. Ms. Davidson earned her B.S. in Education Curriculum and Instruction—Sociology/Social Studies Composite from Texas A & M University. She has taken graduate course work at New Mexico State University, University of North Texas and Texas A & M University. Although in the past Ms. Davidson taught geography, history, economics and government, since she took over the course at Berkner High School, student interest has grown and psychology classes now fill her teaching schedule. Evidence of her teaching excellence has been echoed by supervisor evaluations, C indicating that her performance is FPO “clearly outstanding” and by local Margaret Davidson awards such as being recognized as Applebee’s Teacher of the Month each of three consecutive years. From a district with some 3000 teachers, she was named one of the RISE Teachers of the Year. Ms. Davidson’s influence on high school psychology extends far beyond the classroom. Locally, she is a member of such organizations as the Texas Council for the Social Studies and the Richardson Council for the Social Studies, which work for the advancement of the field. Regionally, she is working to form the Texas Teachers of Psychology in Secondary Schools (T-TOPSS). Nationally, she represents high school teachers of psychology as a teacher consultant with the Education Testing Service. Through this affiliation, she has presented workshops for new and experienced AP Psychology teachers. She has also been a test reader for several years, working to score the free response section of the AP test. She is currently a high school representative of the steering committee for the American Psychological Association’s (APA) Psychology Partnerships Project, a new initiative of the APA Board of Educational Affairs and the Education Directorate. Another important way Ms. Davidson has contributed at the national level is through her work on the Teaching of Psychology in Secondary Schools (TOPSS) Executive Board. By recognizing her creative teaching, advocacy for high school psychology, strength as a role model for teachers teaching teachers, and service to psychology, we are pleased to confirm the belief that Ms. Davidson is the ideal candidate for the Moffett Memorial Teaching Award. McKeachie Early Career Award The recipient of the McKeachie Early Career Award for outstanding teaching by a graduate student is Amy T. Galloway, who recently earned a Ph.D. in Biopsychology at the University of Georgia. Dr. Galloway earned her B.A. in Psychology from FurmanUniversity and her M.S. in Animal Behavior from Bucknell University. Dr. Galloway is currently on a teaching post-doc at Northern Michigan University. Dr. Galloway’s three years as D instructor of Experimental Design, FPO Statistics, and Comparative Animal Amy Galloway, Ph.D. Behavior earned her both departmental and university wide recognition from the University of Georgia. These awards were preceded by the Marshall C. Prevost Medal for Excellence in Psychology, presented at See Awards, page 11 Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 5 The Experimental Lab: A New Trend in Teaching Introduction to Psychology By Ann T. Ewing, Ph.D., Mesa Community College, Mesa, AZ This update is provided courtesy of APA’s Two-Year Working Group 6 There is a trend in the teaching of the introductory course in psychology that is proving to be exciting and very beneficial to the educational experience of thousands of students who take this course every year. The trend referred to is the inclusion of an experimental laboratory as a course requirement for Introduction to Psychology. The experimental laboratory experience enhances the learning of students and reinforces the perception of psychology as a scientific field. Psychology is a scientific field and therefore it should be taught as a scientific field in much the same way as biology, chemistry and physics, which are traditionally taught with a laboratory component. Usually during the first few chapters and lecture sessions of the semester, students are informed that psychology is defined as the scientific study of behavior. They are instructed that psychology is a science as the scientific method is strictly employed. There is no better way to teach concepts such as independent variable, dependent variable, extraneous variable, experimental and control group, etc., than to have students set up an experiment, identify these elements, and run the experiment. Many experiments can easily be run during a single class hour. The laboratory experience allows students to gain a much better understanding of difficult concepts such as experimenter bias and the confounding of variables. Students find they learn a lot by replicating original research from a journal article and comparing their findings or doing some variation of the previous research. Many times the data can be collected on campus or by students between class sessions, then analyzed and discussed during the following lab session. This also provides a wonderful opportunity to demonstrate how statistics are used to analyze research findings. I have found that taking students off campus on a field trip to gather data, early in the semester, greatly facilitates group interaction and the formation of affiliative bonds. This has lasting effects on student satisfaction and retention. The laboratory component of the Introduction to Psychology course affords the opportunity for students to gain a good understanding of various types of research methods used by psychologists. Students can conduct correlational research, surveys, naturalistic observations, and personality inventories as well as experimental research. This opportunity exposes students to many different ways that behavior can be studied and under what circumstances each Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 methodology is most appropriate. One common complaint, voiced by teachers and students alike, is that there is too much material to be effectively covered in the Introduction to Psychology course. The inclusion of a laboratory requirement provides extra hours every week during which some of the many concepts can be introduced, demonstrated and reinforced in a memorable way. There are many demonstrations and exercises that we longingly think would be fun and valuable to do with our students but there simply is not enough time to incorporate them into our available class time. The experimental lab course provides the opportunity to effectively demonstrate difficult principles such as classical conditioning, operant conditioning and schedules of reinforcement. There are many exciting ideas for experimental labs that can be found in Teaching of Psychology, a journal published by Division Two of APA, and the three volumes of Activities Handbook for the Teaching of Psychology, also published by APA. Students can be given the opportunity to condition a live rat in a Skinner box or to condition a simulated rat using a computer. Many laboratory exercises require little or no equipment. Using lemonade powder or Pixie Stix, I have had my students classically condition each other to salivate at the sound of the word “Pavlov”. Even though the demonstration doesn’t always work perfectly, students make a strong connection between Pavlov’s name and the concept of classical conditioning and its inherent elements. I have had students come back years later and claim that they still salivate at the word “Pavlov”. The experience is enjoyable and makes the concepts memorable. In summary, the inclusion of a laboratory component in the Introduction to Psychology course can greatly enhance the learning experience of students. The lab will make it very evident to students that psychology is truly a scientific field. Students will have a much better appreciation of how data is collected and analyzed when they have first hand experience with the methods used by psychologists. Students are much better able to learn the terminology of the field when they are given the opportunity to utilize it in an interactive setting. Finally, an experimental lab course for Introduction to Psychology makes the study of psychology more understandable, more memorable and more enjoyable for the student. Goodies for Psychology Teachers By Alan Feldman, Perth Amboy High School, Perth Amboy, NJ Demonstration of a visual cliff. If you don’t have the visual cliff on video here is a neat way to represent the visual cliff to students. Take a clear videotape holder that can be bought in many stores. Make a black and white checkerboard design. Measure the surface of the videotape container-they are approximately 8 inches by 5 inches. Cut the black and white checkerboard design in half and glue half to the inside top of the container, and glue the other half to the bottom of the other side of the container. You have just made a miniature visual cliff! If you want to show your class the spectrum of light using a prism, place the prism on an overhead projector(practice before you show the students) and you will be able to project a clear bright visual spectrum on the wall. Use two prisms to recreate Newton’s famous experiment in making white light again. For more information on prisms see the recently published, The Fire Within the Eye: A Historical Essay on the Nature and Meaning of Light by David Park, published by Princeton University Press. ISBN: 0-691-04332-9. Check out the Freud Toy Company at (800) 255-8371. This neat company sells a Freud tie, a working Freud watch (has a fifty-minute hour), Freudian slip writing pads, a Freud pillow doll, a Freudian sleeper T-shirt, Freudian slippers and other items. Ask for the Unemployed Philosophers catalog as well. A set of after-dinner Freud cards is available. These are 100 cards printed on thick glossy paper with a question about Freud’s life on the front and the answer on the back. A set of Shakespeare cards is also available. They are both published by Viking-Penguin, originally Becker & Mayer, LTD produced them. The ISBN for the Freud deck is 0-670-86325-4 Optical illusion playing cards and other optical illusion materials are available from Y and B Associated Inc., 33 Primrose Lane, Hempstead, New York 11550. Two new audiotape series on psychology are available from The Teaching Company. One series is by Professor Richard Gerrig of Yale University, and the other series is by Professor Daniel Robinson of Georgetown. Both of these series are outstanding. Call The Teaching Company at 1-800-TEACH12 for more information and a catalog. The owl-lark questionnaire is published in Bodyrhythms: Chronobiology and Peak Performance by Lynne Lamberg, published by William Morrow and Co. N.Y. ISBN: 0-8779-5991-9 Some recent psychology related videos follow: A Science Odyssey: In Search of Ourselves (this is volume 3 of the series) available from PBS at 1-800-828-PBS. This is a two-hour tape on the history of psychology. Born To Rebel: Birth Order & The Revolutionary Personality featuring Dr. Frank Sulloway, and For Whom The Bell Curve Tolls: IQ, Race, & The Nature of Intelligence featuring Dr.’s. Vincent Sarich, Diane Halpern, and Daniel Kelves. Both of these videos are available from The Skeptic Society at 1-818-794-3119. Buy an issue of the Skeptic Magazine for other psychologyrelated audio and videotapes. Four videotapes concerning sleep are available from Dr. James Maas, 210 Uris Hall, Department of Psychology, Cornell University 14853. Phone: 1-607-255-6266. The tapes are 1) Sleep Alert 2) Asleep in the Fast Lane: Our 24 Hour Society, 3) Keep Us Awake 4) When Nights Are Longest. Two tapes produced by the Cerebellum Corporation that may be useful follow: 1) The High Stakes World Of Statistics and 2) The Stimulating World Of Psychology. They are narrated by the Standard Deviants and are available in bookstores or from 1-800-238-9669. An interview with Dr. Stephen Pinker entitled “Language and Consciousness” is available from Thinking Allowed Productions at 5966 Zinn Drive, Oakland California 94611. A video version of The Man Who Mistook His Wife for A Hat (with commentary by Oliver Sacks) is available from Facets video at 1-800-331-6197. Facets also have the Francois Truffat film The Wild Child. Recent printed materials follow: Freudian Slips: Cartoons on Psychology by Sidney Harris, published by Rutgers University Press-ISBN: 0-8135-2478. These cartoons are wonderful and funny. Welcome to the World: A Celebration of Birth and Babies from Many Cultures, is a beautiful book of poetry and pictures depicting childbirth and parenting from around the world. Compiled by Nikki Siegen-Smith and published by Orchard books in NY. ISBN: 0-531-36006-7. Two recent review books helpful for the AP Psychology test are: 1) Introduction to Psychology by Robert Sternberg, published by Harcourt Brace (as part of their College outline series). ISBN: 0-15502071-4. 2) Test Yourself: Introduction to Psychology by Deborah Winters published by NTC Learning Works. ISBN: 0-8442-2366-2. Three books on being a successful psychology student including information about careers in psychology follow: 1) A Student’s Guide to Studying Psychology by Thomas Heffernan published by Psychology Press. ISBN: See Goodies, page 13 Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 7 Alternatives to the Use of Animals in Education By Paul F. Cunningham, Ph.D. and Mary Lou Randour, Ph.D. Psychologists for the Ethical Treatment of Animals Psychologists’ attitudes toward using animals in research seems to be changing in the direction of more qualified and less enthusiastic support. For example, according to a survey sponsored by the National Science Foundation of 3,982 randomly selected psychologists, psychologists have very mixed feelings about using animals in research (Plous, 1996). While there is general support for animal research, a majority of respondents disapproved of experiments involving pain or death. Specifically, even when research was described as “institutionally approved and deemed of scientific merit,” 62.1% of psychologists indicated that research was unjustifiable if it involved pain or death to primates; 44.4% of psychologists indicated that research was unjustifiable if it involved pain or death to rats. (Plous, 1996, p. 1171-1172). Generational and gender differences also were found: less than a quarter of psychologists who received doctoral degrees in psychology during the 1990s voiced strong support for animal research, compared to 48 percent of those who earned their degrees before 1970, and female psychologists were significantly less supportive of animal research than were male psychologists. There are, of course, many reasons for these shifting attitudes. One significant one is that in the last decade there have been significant advances made in the use of technology. Human cell tissue, organ culturing, imaging, and computer modeling are rapidly replacing animal use, rendering it unnecessary and inefficient. Supporting such a shift are the APA Guidelines for Ethical Conduct in the Care and Use of Animals (1996), developed by the Committee on Animal Research and Care (CARE), which encourage the consideration of alternatives to the use of animals in research. Additionally, the APA’s Principles for Quality Undergraduate Psychology Programs endorse both a discussion of the ethics and value of animal use in the classroom and the development of independent critical thinking and decision-making in students. Computers as an Alternative to Animal Use Particularly in education contexts, most of the many attractive and useful alternatives to animals are, not surprisingly, computer-based. Each successive generation of students grows more sophisticated in computer technology, not only in the field of psy- 8 Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 chology but many other disciplines as well. There are good reasons for the popularity of computers. All of the computer-learning programs have proven to be educationally effective as well as economic. They not only preserve the integrity of scientific psychology education, but enhance it (Dewhurst, Hardcastle, Hardcastle, Stuart, 1994; More and Ralph, 1992; Phelps, Nilsestuen, and Hosemann, 1992). The computer-based programs in teaching psychology cover four areas: learning, animal behavior, physiological psychology, and experimental psychology. For those unfamiliar with what a computer-based program might look like and accomplish, we offer the following brief description of one popular computer-based learning program. Sniffy the Virtual Rat Born in 1992 in a psychology department at Erindale College in Toronto, Sniffy the simulated rat quickly became the star attraction of the introductory psychology students there. (His “parents” are Jeff Graham, who was a young laboratory tutor in psychology at Erindale, Tom Allowsay, the associate chair of the Department of Psychology, and Greg Wilson, an independent programming consultant.) Because of Sniffy’s popularity with students and educational effectiveness, Sniffy the Virtual Rat is now distributed by Brooks/Cole Publishing (Todd, 1993). By interacting with Sniffy, students can learn about shaping, response acquisition, schedules of reinforcement and other phenomena in operant conditioning. Sniffy introduces students to the vagaries of the scientific process, helping them appreciate the complexity of it. Professor Lester Krames observed that “Many of the students end up conditioning Sniffy to do things they don’t want him to do. Many of them are being conditioned by Sniffy. The experience they’re getting is analogous to what would happen if they were doing it in a real lab” (Todd, 1993, p. 11). Sniffy isn’t alone; there are numerous other programs on the market. For example, Op-Rat adds discrimination learning to its repertoire of operant principles. Classical conditioning software also is available. Laboratory Classical Conditioning, the creation of James Benedict of James Mason University, simulates salivary conditioning suppression, block effects and taste aversion. See Alternatives, page 11 Two and Four-Year Psychology Faculty Attend PRISM II Conference By Sherry Lynn Kinslow, Ph.D., PRISM-Project Director and Mary Helen Spear, Ph.D., PRISM-Project Co-Director, Department of Psychology Prince George’s Community College, Largo, MD At the beginning of the summer, a group of dedicated psychology faculty from two year and small four year colleges met at Prince George’s Community College (PGCC) to attend PRISM II (the Psychology Retooling Institute and Seminar of the Mid-Atlantic). Like its predecessor, PRISM, which was held at PGCC last summer, PRISM II was funded by a grant from the Division of Undergraduate Education of the National Science Foundation. For four days from May 17th to 20th, PRISM II participants immersed themselves in a series of workshops and activities designed to help them update their knowledge and skills and exchange their best teaching ideas with fellow participants. In the words of one PRISM participant, “I can’t begin to tell you how much I have learned and how honored I feel to have been a part of this. My tool box is overflowing.” Many of the presenters so popular with last year’s participants returned for PRISM II including Dr. Douglas Bernstein, Professor of Psychology at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign and Program Chair for the National Institute on the Teaching of Psychology. Dr. Bernstein demonstrated activities to teach research methods and critical thinking. Dr. F. Scott Hall, a research postdoctoral fellow at the National Institutes of Health discussed new developments in biopsychology including his own research which addresses, in part, the relative importance of nature vs. nurture on certain aspects of behavior. This issue was also addressed by returning presenter, Professor Peter P. Panyon of PGCC, whose presentation challenged conventional thinking on the nature-nurture question. Professor Panyon also guided this year’s participants through a new session during which they dissected the brain of a sheep and discussed parallels with the human brain. Many of the teachers thought this activity would be an excellent hands-on way of teaching brain structure and functioning to introductory psychology students. Dr. Patricia Wallace, Associate Vice-President and Chief Information Officer at University of Maryland University College who served as a panel member at a session at PRISM, conducted her own session at PRISM II on using the world wide web to teach psy- chology. Also returning for a second year to the institute were Dr. Alan Kraut, Executive Director of the American Psychological Society, who discussed psychology as a research based discipline, and Dr. Lester Lefton, Dean of the Columbian School of Arts and Sciences at George Washington University, who gave a presentation on gender and ethnic issues in the classroom. Several new presenters joined the ranks of PRISM II faculty including the keynote speaker, Dr. Jane Halonen, Psychology Department Head at James Madison University. Dr. Halonen used an interactive format to answer the question: “What Can I Do With Psychology?” by having participants examine what they most want their students to learn from taking their introductory psychology course. Dr. Robin Hailstorks, Psychology Chair at PGCC and past-President of Psi Beta, was a panel member last year and returned to serve as a panelist for a session on “Connecting With the Discipline.” Dr. Hailstorks also discussed the two year honorary for psychology students. She was joined on the panel by Dr. William Southerly, an Associate Professor of Psychology at Frostburg State University who discussed connecting with each other electronically and by Dr. Martha M. Ellis, Provost of Spring Creek Campus, Collin County Community College District in Texas. Dr. Ellis discussed APA’s Community College Working Group as well as the Partnerships in Psychology Project that investigates ways to link psychology educators at all levels from high school to two-year and four-year colleges. APA, particularly Dr. Jill Reich, Executive Director of Education, has been very supportive of both PRISM and PRISM II. Dr. Nancy Felipe Russo, Regents Professor of Psychology and Women’s Studies at Arizona State University, was also a featured presenter at PRISM II. She addressed “Teaching Diversity: The ABC’s.” Like last year’s participants, PRISM II attendees also visited the AT & Teaching Theater at the University of Maryland where Dr. Kent Norman introduced them to the paperless classroom in a presentation designed to explore teaching psychology in the digital age. Following this visit, PRISM II participants were feted at a reception at PGCC See PRISM II, page 11 Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 9 ACTIVITY Inquiries, Demonstrations, Experiments and Activities Emotion Perception and Field of Vision By Melissa Doubek, Alpena High School, Alpena, MI Concept: Emotional perception tends to be specialized in the right hemisphere of the brain. This can be shown using odd pictorial images of faces referred to as chimeric stimuli (Temple, 1993). Chimeric faces are constructed with one half of the mouth appearing to smile and the other appearing sad or glum (see Figure 1). Subjects are asked to report whether each face looks happy or sad. Typically a right handed person will say the face in his left field of vision (on his left side) appears happier. Remember, our eyes are wired so the information coming from our left side is projected to our right brain hemisphere. Further evidence comes from stroke victims. Right hemisphere damage in neurological patients can interfere with their ability to make judgments about emotional expressions and to make facial expressions (Temple, 1997). All of this evidence tends to support the Right Hemisphere Dominance theory. The Right Hemisphere Dominance theory suggests that the right hemisphere of the brain is mostly responsible for all emotional processing. The other predominate theory of localization of emotional functioning is the Valence Theory. The valence theory suggests that the right and the left hemisphere each have responsibilities for processing information about different emotions. The right hemisphere is believed to be responsible for negative emotions, such as sadness and the left hemisphere is responsible for positive emotions. The evidence for this theory comes from observations of people with brain damage. People with right hemisphere brain damage sometimes lack negative emotions and people with left brain damage seem to lack positive emotions. This investigation will test the Right Hemisphere Dominance theory and open many questions. Figure 1 (Temple, 1993) Materials: Procedure: Discussion: 1) Take photographs of happy, smiling people. (Ask your subjects if it is OK to use their photo for this experiment). Another option is to use magazine or newspaper pictures. 2) Make three transparencies of the photo(s). 3) Cut two of the three transparencies directly down the center of the face. Keep one transparency as the original with the unaltered photograph. 4) Tape the two left sides transparencies of the face together to make a complete face (L/L). Repeat with the two right sides (R/R). 1) 2) 3) 4) Place the unaltered transparency on an overhead projector. Instruct students to write down which of the next two faces look most like the 1st picture on the overhead (the original). Present one of the other transparencies (either the R/R or L/L), then present the other transparency. Compile the class data on the blackboard. Discuss with the students, the source of their preference for one picture. Typically the R/R is chosen as the match for the original transparency since it matches the side of the face of the original in their left field of vision. To reinforce this phenomena, turn the original transparency over on the overhead projector so you view the face in reverse. Now, subjects typically choose the L/L face as the face that most closely resembles the reversed original. See Activities, page 12 Psychology Teacher Network is looking for good ideas, activities and experiments to share with our readers. Please submit any activities to Psychology Teacher Network, Education Directorate. 10 Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 Awards, from page 5 Furman University in 1991. Teaching excellence programs supported by The University of Georgia have fueled Dr. Galloway’s desired to pursue an academic career. Last year, she was selected to participate in a year-long Teaching Assistant Mentor Program designed to address both the theory and technique of teaching. Another University of Georgia program provided Dr. Galloway with a Graduate School Teaching Support Seminar Assistantship; as the first student from the Department of Psychology to receive this award, she was funded to design a course for new teaching assistants. Narrative evaluations provided by students highlight her strengths in the classroom. Comments about her teaching style highlight her willingness to answer questions, her knowledge, her concern for students, and her enthusiasm. Students reported being stimulated by her teaching style and appreciated her willingness to give them a voice and a choice about how the course proceeded. It is obvious that Dr. Galloway has touched the lives of both undergraduates and peers at the University of Georgia. As one referee put it “Amy has excelled at teaching from the very beginning. I can tell you as a direct observer that she truly loves the teaching process, in both the traditional classroom setting and outside the classroom, in the laboratory and field. She has been the most outstanding graduate instructor of her cohort.” We are pleased to have the opportunity to recognize such an outstanding individual and look forward to her future success as a psychology faculty member. Note: This article was adapted from Teaching of Psychology. The Society for the Teaching of Psychology (Division Two) of APA is taking nominations for the 1999 program of awards. For more information, please contact Dr. Mary Kite, Chair, The Society for the Teaching of Psychology Teaching Awards Committee, Department of Psychological Science, Ball State University, Muncie, IN 47306; (765) 747-6058; mkite@bsu.edu. All Nominations must be postmarked no later than January 4, 1999. Alternatives, from page 8 In the areas of animal behavior, physiological psychology, and experimental psychology, there also are a variety of computer-based programs, as well as other alternatives. (Request a copy of the appendix for a listing of some of these programs.) Because of Sniffy, and many of his virtual kin and other computer programs, there is evidence that educational institutions have reduced their use of animals in teaching. For example, over 50% of medical schools no longer have “dog labs.” And all of the 125 U. S. civilian medical schools offer alternatives to students who decline to participate in live animal laboratories, a choice supported by the American Medical Student Association (Cunningham, 1997). Psychology departments also are discontinuing the use of animals for teaching. Currently from 32% to % 53% of under- graduate departments and 19% of graduate programs do not use animals in teaching (Benedict & Stoloff, 1991; Hull, 1996). Others are entering serious discussions about the discontinuation of animals for a variety of educational, social, political, and scientific reasons (Cunningham, 1997). Conclusion The increased use of computer technology solves a lot of problems. First, it satisfies the goal of the “3R’s”—replacement, refinement, and reduction—a standard supported by the scientific community for a number of years by replacing live animals with technology. Second, computer-based technology is fun for students with proven educational efficiency. A third benefit is that it satisfies the requirement of student choice—the ability of students to decline to participate in classroom activities that are ethically unac- PRISM II, from page 9 hosted by representatives from several publishing companies. Publishers sponsored the appearances of several of the presenters. Like their predecessors, PRISM II participants are now working ceptable to them—a choice affirmed by the Ethical Guidelines for the Teaching of Psychology in the Secondary Schools. Educators recognize that student choice is one of the characteristics of a quality psychology program (McGovern & Reich, 1996) at both undergraduate and secondary education levels (Ernst & Petrossian, 1996; McGovern, 1993). In addition to their education effectiveness, a fourth advantage is that computerbased programs also are economic since they offer reusable tools that become cost-effective in the long run, resulting in cost savings. Finally, they offer they allow students more of an opportunity to pace their own learning, as well as the opportunity for repeated use. References and appendix available upon request to Psychology Teacher Network. in groups of three to create teaching modules appropriate for teaching introductory psychology. Modules created by last year’s participants are available this fall as a teaching resource. Just visit: http://www_STRC.PG.CC.MD.US/PSY/PRISM2.HTML. Psychology Teacher Network September-Pctober 1998 11 DEAR DOCTOR QUESTION: I was wondering about the term “working memory” in relation to another or second part of shortterm memory? Could you tell me how this working memory is different from short-term memory? Who is doing the research on it and which part of the brain might account for its function? Submitted by: Elaine Solomon, Old Mill High School, Millersville, MD PHOTO E FPO Martha Ellis, Ph.D Answered by Martha Ellis, Ph.D., Collin County Community College, Plano, TX A panel of noted clinical, experimental and academic psychologists has graciously agreed to reply in this column to questions submitted by teachers and students. We invite you to send your questions to: DEAR DOCTOR, PTN, Education Directorate, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002-4242 Most of us are familiar with the Atkinson-Shiffrin model of memory involving sensory, short-term, and longterm memory stores. According to this model, these three stores are distinct, and information moves directly from short-term to long-term memory and from long-term to short-term but is never in both locations at once. An alternative to the traditional model is a parallel-processing model with an emphasis on working memory. Alan Baddeley (1986) delineated the architecture of this multistore model in his book Working Memory. Working memory is conceptualized as an active system for temporarily storing and manipulating information needed in the execution of cognitive tasks such as learning, reasoning, and comprehension. Sometimes people do use the terms working memory and short-term memory interchangeably. There is a difference. Working memory is involved in such tasks as holding an address in your mind just long enough to write it down. Working memory also is involved in higher cognitive functions such as planning, organizing, and reasoning. A significant feature of working memory is that it moves activated elements of in- formation into and out of short-term memory from long-term memory. Working memory is the part of memory that comprises all the knowledge of facts, processes, and procedures that has recently been activated from long-term memory as well as the contents of the brief short-term memory. Baddeley’s tripartite model of the working memory system is comprised of two storage and one central executive functions. The two storage systems, the articulatory loop (AL) and the visuospatial scratchpad (VSSP), are fairly passive systems responsible for temporary storage. The articulatory loop is composed of a phonological memory store for speech based material which lasts about two seconds unless it is maintained by the second part of the AL which is a rehearsal mechanism. The VSSP is responsible for the temporary storage and manipulation of visual and spatial information. The most important part of working memory is the central executive which is proposed to be responsible for the selection, initiation, and termination of processing such as encoding and retrieval. This component provides control and flexibility of processing routines for tasks involving decision making, novel situations, trouble shooting, and reasoning. Recent research using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has found that the frontal lobes are the part of the brain involved in working memory (NIMH, 1997). The neurons in the prefrontal lobes, just behind the forehead, appear to be the neural basis of working memory. These cells retrieve and temporarily hold information pulled from longterm memory stores that are dispersed throughout the brain. According to Yale psychologist GoldmanRakic (1997), “working memory is the mental glue that links a thought through time from its beginning to its end.” Patients with damage to the frontal lobes have a number of deficits in working memory and make reasoning errors. A clinical implication from research on working memory includes a better understanding of some schizophrenia symptoms, such as the inability to continue a train of thought or line of reasoning. To find more information on working memory, visit the website “Academic articles on working memory” (http://psyserver.pc.rhbnc.ac.uk/ events/wmfullist.html). Activity, from page 10 Discussion: (continued) References: Harris, Lauren (1997). Michigan State University Professor of Psychology. Personal interview. Temple, Christine (1993). The Brain. England: Penguin Books. 12 Why did more people choose one transparency over the other? (Show them the original.) From where do your emotions originate? What are emotions? What does our data suggest about emotion expression? Brain hemispheres? Recent research has shown that mothers usually cradle their babies in their left arms. Scientists suggest that this behavior lends evidence to the fact that the mother’s left field of vision is better able to judge the infant’s emotional communication. Researchers have also found that schizophrenic mothers cradle their infants in their right arms (Harris, 1997). People suffering from schizophrenia often lack emotional judgment. Data was collected from my classes by Michigan State University researcher Timothy Carbary to investigate whether there is any correlation between a bias for left field of vision for emotional perception and left side baby cradling. From a sample size of 121 students, 76% said their degree of handedness was strong right, 19% moderate right, 2.5% strong left, 1.7% moderate left. There was significantly more left-attention biased subjects for the emotional judgment of faces when the question posed was, “Which face is happier? (p<.05).” There was also significantly more left cradlers than right cradlers (p<.05). Unfortunately there was not a correlation between the two. So what could this mean? This would lead to a good discussion with your students for other possible explanations rather than right hemispheric dominance for emotional perception. Scientists are still researching this question. Possible questions to consider may include: What is the role of handedness? Do learning and experience influence cradling? Does learning to read from left to right influence left field of vision emotional perception? Future tests for scientists could include testing very young children who have not yet learned to read or testing people from other countries who read from right to left. Some people argue that we cradle a baby in our left arm to keep our dominant hand free. Again, we could test young children before they find comfort in a dominant hand. Another investigation could be devised to test adults by asking them to hold a grocery bag equal to a baby’s weight in their arm to see if they hold it in their dominant arm or leave their dominant arm free for other activities. Students should generate further research questions. Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 REVIEW Teaching Modules: THE BRAIN, Second Edition — Even Better Than The First! Publisher: The Annenberg/CPB Multimedia Collection & Worth Publishers, Inc. 901 E Street, NW, Washington, DC 20004-2037; (800) 532-7637 Students remember Phineas Gage long after they forget other names from psychology courses when they have witnessed a reenactment of the tragic accident that hurled a railroad spike through his brain. More importantly, viewers grasp the concept that structure is related to function by seeing how his brain injury affected his behavior. Updated modules from the award-winning series The Brain not only depict favorite excerpts such as Gage’s, but also seamlessly integrate new sequences and “cutting edge” information in 32 segments keyed to introductory psychology units in Teaching Modules: The Brain, Second Edition. The new edition of The Brain can motivate and facilitate learning in the psychology classroom. Noted psychologist Frank Vattano, PhD with Thomas Bennett, PhD and Michelle Butler, PhD from Colorado State University authored and compiled ten all-new modules, extensively revised seven, slightly revised twelve, and omitted three of the original (first edition) modules. They essentially replaced the eliminated material with different sequences that better represent current concepts in psychology. George Page continues his clear narration of added video. The companion Faculty Guide organizes contents in topical units consisting of module clusters: “Foundations”; “Cortical Organization and Specialization”; Goodies, from page 7 0-86377-473-3; 2) The Handbook of Psychology by Drew Appleby published by Longman-Addison-Wesley. ISBN: 0-673-98455-9; 3) Career Paths In Psychology: Where Your Degree Can Take You edited by Robert Sternberg published by APA. Available at (800) 374-2721. Two recently edited books published by APA every teacher should own are: 1) Close Up On Psychology: Supplemental Readings From the APA Monitor; 2) The Evolution of Psychology: Fifty Years of the American Psychologist. This book is full of wonderful articles including “Freud and The Image of Man” by Jerome Bruner; “What is Psychology?” by Jean Piaget; and other articles by Roger Brown, Albert Bandura, and an article on the teach- Date of Publication: 1997 Length: 32 modules on 3 cassettes; each module is 3 to 15 minutes long Cost: $149.95; Faculty Guide: $15.00 Reviewed by: Laura Lincoln Maitland, Mepham High School, Bellmore, NY “Vision and Movement”; “Circadian Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming”; “Memory and Amnesia”; “Stress and Coping”; “Personality and Aggression”; “Psychological Disorders”; “Treatment of Brain Disorders”; which simplifies appropriate use for inexperienced teachers. The guide also links modules to chapters of a popular textbook which is similar to many others. More seasoned educators may choose to regroup modules to maximize effectiveness for their own students. In the new module “Living with Amnesia: The Hippocampus and Memory,” affable Mike exemplifies a memory-impaired individual who describes how he has developed compensatory strategies that help him function successfully. Mike’s mother and employer characterize his abilities and deficiencies whereas Dr. Bennett relates Mike’s memory difficulty to damage of his hippocampus. The last few minutes of the module shifts to research of long-term potentiation (LTP) that results in facilitation of synaptic transmission hypothesized to be responsible for memory storage. The organization of this and other modules enables each instructor to adapt the series for a variety of courses. An Introduction To Psychology or A.P. Psychology educator might show the complete module whereas a high school psychology teacher might select only the first part of the module featuring Mike; a college physiological psychology professor might use only the second part to focus on experimental research with rats in the Morris water maze. Other new modules spotlight Dr. Nancy Wexler’s research on Huntington’s Disease, brain functions during sleep, a super-memorist’s advice on how to study, Dr. Temple Grandin’s reflections on her own autism, etiology and pharmacological treatment of schizophrenia, treatment of epilepsy, transplants in Parkinson’s patients, and neurorehabilitation. Each of these engaging additions features real people, not actors, and communicates new knowledge in digestible form. Of course the authors retained excellent modules from the first edition. Unforgettable encounters with Tony/DeDe/Richard who exhibits dissociative personality disorder, Vicki, a split-brain patient, young aborigines who display “natural” intelligence, Charles Landry who is aphasic, Susannah who adapts to an upside-down world, and hair-twirling Jerry who manifests multiple symptoms of schizophrenia remain part of the video set. Owners of the first edition of Teaching Modules: The Brain concerned that much of the information is out-of-date may want to acquire the new edition. For Introduction to Psychology and AP Psychology teachers not in possession of either set, this is on my list of “must haves.” ing of psychology by Henry Gleitman entitled “Introducing Psychology”. According to Charles Brewer, former Editor of Teaching of Psychology, “This is the best article on teaching psychology ever written.” practices. I found that my high school and psychology students were fascinated by the information that Dr. Small reports. A new collection of articles and personality tests can be found in an edited book entitled, Experiences in Personality: Research, Assessment, and Change edited by Matthew Merrens and Gary Brannigan. John Wiley publishes the book and the ISBN is 0-471139378. This book is a gem. Look for the book entitled, Our Babies, Ourselves by Meredith Small and an article by Meredith Small in the October 1997 Natural History. This work is a cross-cultural account of child rearing Get a hold of the beautiful and amazing book, Babies: History, Art, and Folklore. This book discusses the history of childcare from antiquity to the present in regards to pregnancy, birth, feeding, washing, dressing, carrying, rocking and understanding babies. It is full of beautiful illustrations concerning these topics. The first volume of the official journal of Division 26 (History of Psychology) has just been released. It is published quarterly and is $30.00 to APA members and affiliates. For further information or to order call APA. Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 13 NEWS FROM TOPSS New TOPSS Members Alabama Ms. Gamard, Birmingham Shirley Horton, Sylacauga California Ann Ban-Weiss, Laguna Creek High School, Elk Grove Connie Barker, Stockton Joe Barron, Fresno Lisa Boenitz, Academy of Our Lady of Peace, San Diego Ann Bon-Weiss, Davis Deborah Brown, Ahwahnee Judith Cameron, Fullerton June Cochran, Manteca Elaine Culerwell, Freemont Karen Elitzky, Drew College Prep, San Francisco Daurice Graves, Menlo Park Terry Hodges, Immaculate Conception Acad., San Francisco Gloria Joe, Pacifica Jim Leduk, Corona John Lowe, Dana Point Juliet Mohit, San Anselmo Ed Neugebauer, Pueblo Shawn Parker, Aptos Ruth Peterson, Sunland John Prirto, East Union High School, Manteca Phil Reddish, Banning Mike Reeske, Valley Center Laura Roth-Shofron Windward School, Los Angeles Susan Schmitt, St. Joseph High School, Santa Maria Barbara Simon, Exeter Union High School, Exeter Linda Williamson, Camarillo Colorado Patrick Lang, Englewood Thomas Lukich, Evergreen Connecticut Kathy Finch, Moreno Valley Peter Wells, New Haven Florida Charles Gartrell, North Lauderdale Joseph Saraceno, Jacksonville Idaho Laurie Hoss, Boise Illinois Carmen Gonzalez, Van Steuben Metro Science Center, Chicago Cynthia Krause, Naperville Nick Romano, Lockport Fricke Thomas, Chicago Becky Wulf, Nashville High School, Nashville Cheryl Ann Zygadlo, Downers Grove Iowa Nancy Marks, Ames Fred Vanderlay, East High School, Des Moines Kansas Judith Buback, Maple Hills Tenny Dewey, Overland Park Sheila Norton, Shawnee Kentucky Eva McGowan, Hopkinsville Maryland John Newby, Silver Spring Massachusetts Judith Brenner, Montague Jennifer Carey, North Andover Michigan David Bihl, Clarkston LJ Lamreaux, Clarkston Ann Lehman, Ferndale Minnesota Stuart Lasky, Minneapolis Kyle Smith, Livonia Angela Tucci, New Baltimore Nebraska Linda Bosshamer, Kearney Detta Holscher, Smithfield Ruth Slominski, Holdrege Dixie Torres, Lincoln New Jersey Janice DeLorenzo, Nutley Patrick Kiernan, Hopelawn Vincent McGowen, Madison Paul Mueller, Florham Park Susan Morton, Morris Knolls High School, Rockaway Lisa Reda, Pequannock New York Cynthia Garcia, Brooklyn Caroline Lipnicki, Melville Sharien Muhammad-Cordero, New York North Carolina Paul Deal, Manteo Richard Murphy, Charlotte Ohio Nina Goforth, Middletown Oklahoma Ronald Foore, Owassa Eric Spaulding, Broken Arrow Jan Votravis, Quinton Oregon Linda Christianson, Portland Brian Fenderson, Salem Todd Nickerson, Medford Pennsylvania Mary Donecker, E. Petersburg Spencer Morrison, Cranberry Township Christopher Rumbaugh, Hanover Rhode Island Ann Nevola, Cranston South Carolina James Carbaugh, Taylors Texas Robin Goodson, DeSoto Stacy Lynn Hays, Denton Susan Highfield, Arlington Thomas Mitchell, Flower Mound Ron Montgomery, Garland Jennifer Owens, Dallas Fred Sidneider, Ursuline Academy, Dallas Janice Strubbe, Baytown Russell Taylor, Smyer Melissa Walston, Lubbock Theresa Wilson, Orange Utah Travis Leifeste, Sunset Kimberly Searle, Copper Hills High School, W. Jordan Virginia Clyde Harrell, Berryville Veronica Moore, Norfolk Washington Steve Berq, Lake Stevens Tracey Bromley, Mansfield High School, Mansfield Matthew Ellis, Tacoma Scott Morrison, Lynnwood Susan Schmitt, St. Joseph High School, Santa Maria Thomas Weaver, Olympia West Virginia Janice Huter, Hurricane Wisconsin Roberta Grimm, Madison Mary Harker, Middleton Sharon Kuckan, Pewaukee Brain LeCloux, Richland Center Robert Peterson, Eau Claire Aaron Sadoff, Lincoln High School, Manitowoc Wyoming Cece O’Keefe, Casper Brian Yoder, Laramie International Affiliates Jean Atkinson, British Columbia Leonard Coastis, Melbourne, Australia Stephanie Cox, Hants, United Kingdom Salvarej Grapragasem, Malaysia Judith Hayes, New Zealand Winston Hulme, Surrey, United Kingdom Rae Neilson, Vancouver, British Columbia Megan Waring, Conway, United Kingdom Heather Watson, Vancouver, British Columbia Ninka Watts, Our Lady of Sion College, Roxhill, Australia Rena Zayit, Ontario, Canada Are your students interested in personality & social psychology? 14 Then join the Society for Personality and Social Psychology! With nearly 3,000 members, SPSP is the world’s leading organization for social and personality psychology teachers and researchers. To join, send this form with a check for $28 to: SPSP, Dept. of Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627. For just $28 per year, members and affiliates receive monthly issues of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin (now in its 24th year, ranked #2 for impact by Science Citation Index); our quarterly journal of theory and reviews, Personality and Social Psychology Review, our newsletter, Dialogue; and special member rates at SPSP’s annual conferences. Want more information? Check out our website, www.spsp.org. Address ______________________________________________ Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 Name ________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________ Highest Degree ________________________________________ Courses Taught ________________________________________ ANNOUCEMENTS October Teacher Workshop To Be Held in Rhode Island The APA’s Education Directorate is sponsoring a one and a half day workshop for high school teachers of psychology at the University of Rhode Island, Providence, RI, October 23-24, 1998. Presenters will be Bill Addison, Ph.D., Eastern Illinois University, Charleston, IL, and Carol Dean, Lake Park High School, Roselle, IL. The workshop will provide teachers with an opportunity to: gain a greater knowledge and understanding of psychology; develop new concepts and skills; explore scientific ways of thinking; engage in problem-solving and decision-making activities; and, exchange ideas and experiences with other teachers. Participants will be involved in many exciting hands-on activities and demonstrations, and receive numerous handouts for use in their classrooms, as well as information about the Advanced Placement (AP) program. To sign up for the workshop, please complete the form below and return it to APA by October 9, 1998. Space is limited! For additional information, contact Martha Braswell, APA Education Directorate, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002, (800) 374-2721, extension 6140. Rhode Island Teacher Workshop Registration Form (please print or type) FEE: (check one) ❒ $35.00 for High School Teacher Affiliates, TOPSS members and APA members ❒ $45.00 for non-affliates and non-members Name: Home Address: City: State: Zip: State: Zip: School: School Address: City: Daytime telephone number: APA High School Teacher Affliliate APA Member Yes ❒ Yes ❒ No ❒ No ❒ PAYMENT: (check one) ❒ Check payable to APA enclosed ❒ School Purchase Order enclosed Charge to: ❒ American Express ❒ Visa ❒ Mastercard Credit Card #: SPECIAL OFFER FOR NON-AFFILIATES: Join APA as a High School Teacher Affiliate and register for the workshop for only $60. For a High School Teacher Affiliate application call the APA Membership Office at (202) 336-5580 or the Education Directorate at (202) 336-6076. Send completed form and payment to APA, Attn: Workshop Accounting, 750 First Street, NE, Washingon DC 20002-4242. Registration paid by P.O. or credit card may be faxed to (202) 336-5693. Exp. Date: Name as it appears on card Cardholder Signature Billing Address City State Zip ✯ Don’t forget to vote in the TOPSS Executive Board elections ✯ Psychology Teacher Network September-October 1998 15