APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) Arctic Cognition: A Study of Cognitive Performance in Summer and Winter at 698N TIM BRENNEN*, MONICA MARTINUSSEN, BERNT OLE HANSEN and ODIN HJEMDAL University of Tromsù, Norway SUMMARY Evidence has accumulated over the past 15 years that aect in humans is cyclical. In winter there is a tendency to depression, with remission in summer, and this eect is stronger at higher latitudes. In order to determine whether human cognition is similarly rhythmical, this study investigated the cognitive processes of 100 participants living at 698N. Participants were tested in summer and winter on a range of cognitive tasks, including verbal memory, attention and simple reaction time tasks. The seasonally counterbalanced design and the very northerly latitude of this study provide optimal conditions for detecting impaired cognitive performance in winter, and the conclusion is negative: of ®ve tasks with seasonal eects, four had disadvantages in summer. Like the menstrual cycle, the circannual cycle appears to in¯uence mood but not cognition. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. There are three major astronomical cycles that have a period short enough to be important in the study of behaviour. The ®rst is the 24-hour rotation of the Earth on its axis, the second is the monthly rotation of the moon about the Earth and the third is the 12-month orbit of the Earth around the Sun. While cognitive psychologists have studied the daily cycle using time-of-day as an experimental variable (e.g. Smith, 1989) and the monthly cycle in particular in connection with studies of the menstrual cycle (e.g. Richardson, 1991a), the annual cycles have been seldom studied from a cognitive perspective. An investigation of the eect of the seasons upon normal cognitive performance is timely because evidence is accumulating of seasonal eects on the incidence of moodrelated problems. Rosenthal et al. (1984) named this disorder Seasonal Aective Disorder (SAD). People who suer from this disorder have symptoms of depression that only occur during one part of the year, but that recur from year to year. The vast majority of SAD patients suer in winter, with remission in summer, and frequently reported symptoms include sadness, anxiety, irritation, decreased activity and libido, leading both to social problems and diculty at work. *Correspondence to: T. Brennen, Department of Psychology, University of Tromsù, 9037 Tromsù, Norway. E-mail: Timb@psyk.uit.no Contract Contract Contract Contract grant grant grant grant sponsor: Norwegian Research Council. number: 114162/330. sponsor: Nansenfondet, Den Norske Videnskaps-Akademi. number: 24/97. CCC 0888±4080/99/060561±20 $17.50 Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 13 April 1999 562 T. Brennen et al. It has since also been claimed that there exists a weaker version of SAD, called subsyndromal SAD (sub-SAD), where people report season-related mood swings, but to a level below clinical signi®cance (Kasper et al., 1989). The sub-SAD idea thus introduces the notion that season-related swings constitute a normal aspect of human mental life. While the bulk of the research on SAD has been undertaken in the United States, cases of SAD have been documented in other countries too, e.g. Iceland (Magnusson and Stefansson, 1993) and Britain (Thompson and Isaacs, 1988). Research has documented the disorder's incidence, and has attempted to pinpoint the biological mechanisms by which it arises. One area of consensus regarding SAD is that it is the relative lack of light in winter that triggers the disorder. Two predictions from this hypothesized link between light exposure and SAD have been tested. First, SAD should increase with increasing latitude: places on the equator receive 12 hours of daylight and 12 hours of darkness, every day of the year. Everywhere else on the planet the total annual amount of daylight is roughly the same, but it is shared out unevenly over the year. Therefore the further one goes from the equator, the greater the swing in day and night length, and thus the prediction is that SAD will be more prevalent at higher latitudes due to the relative lack of light in winter. Rosen et al. (1990) investigated this using a questionnaire study on populations at four latitudes ranging from 278 to 42.58N. There was a correlation of 0.11 between latitude and SAD incidence, in the predicted direction. Palinkas et al. (1996) investigated whether SAD incidence would increase among 70 people who spent a year on Antarctica. They administered two questionnaires twice, once between three and four months before the Antarctic winter solstice and once one to two months after it. The number of people found to be suering from sub-SAD increased from seven to 19 over the Antarctic winter, and the number of people with SAD went from nought to one. Thus spending a winter in the Antarctic increased SAD symptoms. Second, the eect of light therapy to relieve the SAD symptoms during the winter months has been much investigated, initially by Rosenthal et al. (1984). A metaanalysis that included studies totalling 332 patients showed that it had bene®cial eects and research is trying to pinpoint exactly when in the day, and for how long, light should be used to maximize the eect (Terman et al., 1989). CIRCANNUAL COGNITIVE RHYTHMS IN SAD Whereas the psychiatric and neuropharmacological correlates of SAD have been much studied, it is only more recently that the relationship between SAD and cognition has been investigated (Drake et al., 1997; Michalon et al., 1997; O'Brien et al., 1993). O'Brien et al. (1993) reported signi®cant de®cits on six out of seven cognitive tasks in 11 SAD patients compared with ten normal control subjects. These tasks included pattern recognition, spatial recognition, delayed match-to-sample tests and paired associate learning. On the remaining task, a simultaneous match-to-sample test, the trend was clearly for SAD patients to perform worse than controls. In other words, this study showed reliable cognitive impairments in SAD patients on a wide variety of tasks. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) Arctic Cognition in Summer and Winter 563 Michalon et al. (1997) administered a wide range of cognitive tests to 30 SAD patients and 29 controls in three consecutive winters. They reported de®cits for the SAD patients on recognition memory for faces and on two tasks involving copying and recalling the Rey Complex Figure. An evaluation of the ecacy of light therapy in improving SAD patients' scores for these tests was inconclusive. A subset of their sample also carried out the tests one summer and while the de®cit in face recognition was absent, the Rey Complex Figure de®cits were still present. In Drake et al.'s (1997) study all subjects had winter and summer testing sessions. They tested ten SAD patients and nine controls on a Stroop task and a patternrecognition task. Season did not aect the SAD patients dierently from the controls on either task, but on Stroop performance there was a main eect of season, with summer better than winter. However, since all subjects were tested ®rst in winter and then in summer, the dierence in Stroop performance may be due to a practice eect. In other words, the confound between season and testing order makes it unsafe to conclude that, in both patients and controls, there are seasonal rhythms in Stroop performance. COGNITION AND EXTREME LATITUDE As described above, Palinkas et al. (1996) studied seasonal mood swings on normals at extreme latitudes. By contrast, there is hardly any research on seasonal cognitive swings in normals at such latitudes. Taylor and Duncum (1987) investigated digit span and a ®ve-digit discrimination task in 33 people wintering over in Antarctica. They were tested before and after their one-year stay, though the months of testing are not mentioned. However there were no dierences in performance, despite the subjects' self-report of mental sluggishness. Establishing whether or not circannual cognitive swings exist is appropriate now for four reasons. First, research on SAD has established that mood swings covary with seasons and a follow-up question regards whether seasonal swings in cognitive performance are also important for humans. In other words, the extent to which humans are seasonal will be investigated in a domain other than mood. Second, the current evidence for human seasonality, from SAD, largely uses selfreport methodology. This study, in contrast, will measure swings in actual human performance. Third, a particular problem with some studies of SAD incidence is that recruiting subjects by advertising the project as an investigation into winter depression is likely to lead to overestimation of true SAD incidence because people with relatively minor seasonal swings are less likely to volunteer. This makes estimates of incidence of SAD and generalizations about human seasonality unreliable. In the present study, participants were recruited without reference to winter depression or the possible eects of the seasons on cognition. It was presented as a long-term study of memory and attention, which, of course, was precisely what it was ± no more, no less. In this way, we sidestepped the problem of attracting participants particularly prone to, or interested in, seasonal swings. Fourth, the practical consequences of any seasonal cognitive swings are obvious in occupations demanding a high level of performance, e.g. piloting, driving. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) 564 T. Brennen et al. TROMSé In this paper we report a study that recorded performance of a battery of cognitive tests by a large group of people living in Tromsù, in northern Norway, at a latitude of 698N, over 300 kilometres north of the Arctic Circle. They were tested in summer and in winter, in order to detect seasonal swings in cognitive performance. Tromsù's population is just below 60,000 and there is no town of that size at a higher latitude anywhere on the planet. By way of comparison, in the Southern hemisphere the 698S parallel passes through Antarctica. Tromsù is a fully ¯edged town: it has a university with over 7000 students, a major teaching hospital, a football team in the highest national division, two daily newspapers, a plethora of bars and restaurants, and over 20 scheduled ¯ights to Oslo per day. Thus Tromsù stands as a rebuttal to Palinkas et al.'s (1996) claim that the diculties experienced by those wintering over in the Antarctic (`prolonged separation from family and friends, the lack of privacy in cramped quarters, and boredom caused by lack of environmental and social stimulation', p. 533) would also be characteristic of more densely populated high-latitude environments! The eect of the seasons on life in Tromsù was broached directly by Rosenthal (1993, pp. 217±18). With the main source of information apparently a New Yorker article, Tromsù comes across as a town where in winter work and sleep were dicult, and in summer there was `widespread celebration' as people went ®shing, hunting and having fun. This is a romanticized exaggeration, and it is therefore not an `excellent description' because it is factually wrong in several respects. At 698N the astronomical seasonal swings are extreme. The `ocial' period of midnight sun is from 21 May until 21 July, but in fact there is constant daylight for perhaps a month on either end of that, and one does not see night for even longer than that if one has a normal sleep pattern. In winter, the ocial period of `mùrketida' (darkness), where the sun does not come above the horizon, is from 21 November until 21 January. A common interpretation of this by non-Arctic people is that it must be pitch black for two months of the year. This is not the case. Even at the winter solstice, the sky is blue at midday in Tromsù, but the days of eective daylight are nevertheless very short. We are using Tromsù as a natural laboratory that provides participants who experience the seasons at 698N. If humans have seasonally driven cognitive swings then they are surely to be found in Tromsù. Indeed, there is plenty of anecdotal evidence suggesting that Tromsù's inhabitants are mentally slower in winter than summer (see e.g. Rosenthal, 1993). Note that the seasonal variation in amount of light in Tromsù is as extreme as the circadian variation in non-Arctic places. The predictions are that performance in winter will be worse than performance in summer. On most tasks this will be re¯ected as higher error rates or longer reaction times. Additionally, some tests have inbuilt measures of `confusability', e.g. the dierence between consistent and inconsistent trials on the Stroop task. The prediction is that these will increase in winter. THE POWER AND THE DESIGN OF THIS STUDY Due to the paucity of studies that have investigated cognition at extreme latitudes, there is no straightforward way of estimating expected eect sizes (and hence the Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) Arctic Cognition in Summer and Winter 565 power) of the seasonal manipulation in this study. Instead, since giving a precise estimate of eect size is not possible, the power of our study was worked out for dierent possible eect sizes. A within-subjects study with 100 participants and a signi®cance level of 0.05 has a power of 0.98 for an eect size of 0.4 SD, 0.84 for an eect size of 0.3 SD, and 0.51 for an eect size of 0.2 SD (Borenstein et al., 1997). Reaction time eects smaller than 0.2 SD are, for many purposes, trivial. One centrally important aspect of this study is that one subgroup of participants was tested ®rst in summer and then in winter, and the other subgroup was tested ®rst in winter and then in summer. Any seasonal eects cannot therefore be due to order of testing, and spurious eects cannot arise due to a confound either. On the other hand, seasonal eects for any one subgroup cannot unambiguously be interpreted as seasonal swings: they may be a practice eect or a boredom eect. However, a seasonally counterbalanced design allows one to conclude that any seasonal swings detected are true circannual swings. An overall seasonal eect may emerge either as a main eect of season or as a particular form of interaction between season and subgroup, namely where the practice eect for one subgroup is reduced or eliminated and for the other subgroup is increased. This would be due to the eect of season working against practice for one subgroup and in the same direction for the other subgroup. This study is a ®rst step: it is a thorough investigation of cognitive swings at a location where the seasonal swings of the environment are enormous. It will be left to subsequent research to determine which of the many variables that are confounded with season (e.g. light, temperature, length of time since a holiday) is responsible for any dierences found. In other words, this study is designed to detect putative cognitive rhythms, but in the case of an armative answer will not specify what the key causal variables are. If, on the other hand, circannual cognitive rhythms cannot be found among Tromsù's inhabitants, then any eects cannot be very large, and can hardly be of practical consequence. There are several features of this study that make it suited to detecting even small circannual cognitive rhythms: (1) reaction time is used as a measure, (2) the number of participants is large, (3) a wide range of tests is used, (4) the studied population live at 698N, and thus experience enormous seasonal swings, and (5) a seasonally counterbalanced design is used. METHOD Participants Posters advertising the study were placed around Tromsù in May 1997, in the buildings of the town council and the county council, in schools, in cafeÂs frequented by bus and taxi drivers, at the hospital, and at the university. People who were interested in taking part in a study of memory and attention where one would perform various tasks like recognizing faces, counting dots or naming colours, and where one would be tested several times, were asked to ring Tim Brennen or Monica Martinussen. In this way, 110 participants were recruited, who were to participate in summer and winter 1997. The same procedure was repeated in November 1997, when 71 participants were recruited. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) 566 T. Brennen et al. There were dropouts from the ®rst testing session to the second. These either broke appointments or could not be reached by letter, telephone or e-mail. In total, 62 (56%) of the original 110 summer±winter participants were retested in winter 1997, and 38 (54%) of the original 71 winter±summer participants were retested in summer 1998. The mean age of the 62 returning summer±winter participants was 30.0 years, SD 10.1 (range 16±61), and 26 (42%) were women. The mean age of the 38 returning winter±summer participants was 31.6 years, SD 12.9 (range 18±63), and 19 (50%) were women. Note that although dropout rates were relatively high, running participants on the battery for a second time was equally dicult in winter and in summer, and that the age and gender makeup of the resulting samples were very similar. Of the 100 subjects, 53 had lived in northern Norway all their lives. At the time of their ®rst test session the remaining subjects had lived in the area for a mean of 9.3 years (SD 10.0; range 0 to 56 years), and for the sample as a whole the mean number was 19.1 years (SD 13.9). DESIGN The independent variable of main interest is season, a within-subjects factor with the levels of summer and winter. All participants performed the tests in summer and in winter. One group of participants began their tests in summer and another group in winter, giving the control between-subjects factor of subgroup. The two subgroups will be referred to as the summer±winter and the winter±summer groups respectively. Any dierence (or absence thereof) between summer and winter will thus not be attribut7able to practice eects that carried over from one session to the next. It was necessary to have two forms of the verbal ¯uency task, the face-recognition task and the word-memory task, to prevent savings or long-term repetition priming confounding the results. The order of presentation of the two forms of test was crossed with the subgroup factor, so that in both subgroups some participants performed one test ®rst and others performed the other one ®rst. In this way, artifactual explanations of any eects of season on these tests are ruled out. APPARATUS All computer-based tasks were run on a 166 MHz PC with a colour monitor. The software package used was ERTS from Berisoft (Beringer, 1992). For some of the tasks, the ®rst author developed a version of a script written by Berisoft; for other tasks the script was written from scratch. For the reaction time tasks participants responded by use of a response pad directly connected to the hard drive, accurate to within 1 ms (Beringer, 1992). PROCEDURE Participants were tested individually by a research assistant in one of two labs at the University of Tromsù. Two research assistants ran approximately half the Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) Arctic Cognition in Summer and Winter 567 participants each.1 The twelve tests were administered in the same order for all participants in both sessions. In all three test sessions, the research assistants were available to run participants from 8 a.m. until 6 p.m. The summer sessions began in mid-May and were run until early July. The winter session began in early December and ran until mid-January. Participants sat in front of the computer at a distance of approximately 60 cm. Their posture was not restricted, e.g. by a chin-rest, due to the length of the test session. All instructions were shown on the computer screen. TASKS Reaction time Speed Participants had to respond by pressing a button as quickly as possible to a yellow circle presented in the middle of the screen, where the background colour was grey. This was thus a simple reaction time task. The temporal characteristics of this test were as follows. There was a 4-second gap between the participant's key press to clear the instructions and the presentation of the ®rst stimulus. Thereafter the length of the interval between a participant's response and the presentation of the next stimulus was determined by an exponentially decreasing random process, with a maximum of 7500 ms. There were forty trials. Dot numerosity judgements In this task the screen was divided by a vertical line down the middle. On each trial participants had to decide on which side there were more dots, pressing the left or right button accordingly. On half of the trials there were more dots on the left side and on the other half there were more on the right. On each trial, one side had 20 dots in a random pattern. The other side had the same 20-dot pattern plus from one to ®ve extra dots. All dots were presented simultaneously. The visual stimulus remained on screen until the participant's response or 3000 ms were reached, whichever came ®rst. The dots were yellow on a grey background and there were forty trials in total. On each trial, after the participant had responded, feedback was given by showing the location of the key `extra' dots in blue. Attention tasks Stroop This Stroop task used manual responses on a four-button response pad. The four colours were red, yellow, green and blue, and the words were the equivalent Norwegian colour words: rùd, gul, grùnn and blaÊ. Participants were instructed to respond to the colour of the stimulus and not the word. There were sixty trials of which 24 were consistent (e.g. `blaÊ' written in blue) and 36 were inconsistent (e.g. `blaÊ' written in yellow). Feedback was provided on the screen on each trial as to whether the participant's response was correct or not. 1 The same two research assistants (BOH and OH) shared the running of the great majority of participants equally in all three sessions. Kristin Johannessen and Tim Brennen stepped in when either of them was unable to work. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) 568 T. Brennen et al. Mapping On each trial either an X or an O was presented, either left or right of centre. The participants' task was to press the left-hand button when an O was presented and the right-hand button when an X was presented. This is thus a task where confusion arises due to response mapping con¯ict: a stimulus is presented on the left-hand side and the correct response is to press the right-hand button. There were forty trials. The visual angle subtended by the X and the O was 28, and the eccentricity was 78. Time estimation Participants were given the task of estimating time intervals. Participants initiated each trial by a key press which triggered a tone that indicated the beginning of the time period. The participant pressed the button when he or she reckoned the time period was up. There were three trials, carried out in the following order: 60 seconds, 30 seconds, 15 seconds. Throughout each trial the target number of seconds was on screen. Memory tasks Sternberg This task required scanning of short-term memory, and was based on Sternberg's (1966) task. On each trial to-be-remembered yellow digits were presented one at a time, for 1000 ms. All 10 digits had an equal chance of being selected, and no repetition within trials was allowed. Then a dash was presented for 3000 ms, followed by a single white digit. The participants' task was to decide whether the white digit was a member of the previously presented set. Forty trials were presented in total, half of which with three digits to remember and half with ®ve. For half of each of these set sizes, the white digit did in fact come from the set (target present), and for half it did not (target absent). Face recognition On each trial a face was presented in the middle of the screen. The participant's task was to decide whether or not it was a famous face. Of the 42 faces in the experiment, 25 were famous and 17 were not. Each face was presented twice during the task, giving a total of 84 trials for each participant on each test session. There were two sets of faces, and about half the participants saw Set A in their ®rst test session and Set B on their second, whereas the other participants saw Set B and then Set A. Word memory Words were presented for 3000 ms, one at a time in the middle of the screen. The participant triggered the presentation of the following word by pressing the space bar. Participants were told that their memory for these words would be tested later. In every test session, 61 words were presented. The ®rst six and the last ®ve words were held constant in order to reduce any eects of primacy and recency, and memory for these items was not tested. This gave 50 target words for which memory was tested. Memory for these items was tested approximately 10 minutes after this presentation phase, the time it took to perform the mapping and Sternberg tasks. The technique employed was that used by Jacoby et al. (1993) for separating conscious and unconscious in¯uences on memory. This consists of a stem-completion task Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) Arctic Cognition in Summer and Winter 569 where the instructions varied according to the colour of the stem: when the stem was written in green, the participant had to complete it with a word from the previously seen list. If the participant did not remember a word from the list, any other word could be written. When the stem was written in red, the participant had to complete it with a word NOT from the previously seen list. These instructions are known as the inclusion and the exclusion tests, respectively. Each participant saw 25 stems in the inclusion condition and 25 in the exclusion condition. In conjunction these two types of test provide estimates of the contributions of recollection and automatic in¯uences on memory performance. There were two counterbalancing factors. First, there were two lists of 50 words and half the participants saw List 1 in their ®rst test session and List 2 in their second, while the other half had the reverse order. Second, each wordlist was divided into two, so that across each test session any particular word was seen by equal numbers of participants in the inclusion and the exclusion conditions. The last task in each test session was a surprise-free recall test for the words in the original list. Although we expected this test to be less of a surprise at the end of the second session, the number, variety and intensity of tests meant that the participants were surprised the second time too! Fluency This was a verbal ¯uency test. Participants were given a letter from the alphabet and told to generate as many words as possible in one minute. Proper names and foreign words were not allowed, and words with the same stem as a previously generated word, e.g. plurals, did not count. The letters used were `F' and `S'. About half the participants generated words from the letter `F' in the ®rst test session and `S' in the second, while the others generated in the reverse order. Order of tasks The order of tests was identical for all participants in all test sessions: (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) Dot numerosity judgements Fluency Word memory±presentation phase Sternberg Mapping Word memory ± stem completion Time estimation Speed: break Face recognition Stroop Word memory ± free recall. RESULTS A separate analysis of variance was carried out for each test. The analysis for all tests includes a season factor (with the levels of summer and winter) and a counterCopyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) 570 T. Brennen et al. Table 1. A summary for each task of the seasonal means (with standard deviation), the eect of season, and eect size, ES Task Summer Winter Seasonal eect? Speed 271 ms (46) 260 ms (38) Dot numerosity Stroop 1290 ms (454) 7.1 errors (3.6) 984 ms (326) 1180 ms (393) 6.8 errors (3.2) 935 ms (301) Mapping 508 ms (124) 493 ms (97) Time estimation Sternberg Face recognition Word memory 0.212 (0.22) 1004 ms (244) 887 ms (162) A: 0.12 (0.08) R: 0.27 (0.16) FR: 12.1 (6.6) 15.7 (4.6) 0.194 (0.17) 974 ms (226) 864 ms (170) A: 0.10 (0.07) R: 0.30 (0.17) FR: 14.4 (8.7) 15.0 (5.0) Yes: summer slower than winter, ES 0.26 Yes: summer more error-prone than winter, ES 0.08 No: no seasonal eect and no season by consistency interaction Yes: increased mapping confusion in summer (28 ms versus 12 ms in winter) No (see Table 2) No No Yes: free recall better in winter, ES 0.33 Fluency Yes: summer better than winter, ES 0.12 balancing factor of subgroup (with the levels of summer±winter and winter±summer). Some of the tests have additional counterbalancing factors, as described in the Tasks section above. Where appropriate, tasks had one analysis for correct responses and another for errors. Tests had slightly dierent numbers of participants due to causes such as equipment failure and corrupted data ®les. The resultant number of participants per task ranges from 93 to 99. An alpha level of 0.05 was used for all statistical tests. All signi®cant sources of variance are mentioned below. Table 1 summarizes the main results of each task by indicating whether a season eect was observed. Speed The mean reaction time was 271 ms (SD 46) for summer and 260 ms (SD 38) for winter. A two-way ANOVA with the factors of season and subgroup showed that the main eect of season was signi®cant, F(1,91) 4.46, MSE 822. The main eect of subgroup was also signi®cant, F(1,91) 4.90, MSE 2538: the summer±winter group had a mean of 272 ms (SD 44) and the winter±summer had a mean of 255 ms (SD 37). These main eects were modi®ed by an interaction between subgroup and season, F(1,91) 6.19, MSE 822. The winter±summer group had a mean of 256 ms (SD 40) in winter and 255 ms (SD 34) in summer, whereas the summer± winter group had a mean of 262 ms (SD 36) in winter and 282 ms (SD 49) in summer. Therefore, for one subgroup there was an advantage in winter and for the other there was no eect. These data are consistent with a model of simple reaction time where performance is quicker in winter than summer, and where there is a practice eect. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) Arctic Cognition in Summer and Winter 571 Dot numerosity judgements Wrong key presses and latencies in excess of 3000 ms were counted for each subject in each test session. The mean number of errors was 7.07 (SD 3.6) for summer and 6.8 (SD 3.2) for winter. A two-way ANOVA showed that the main eect of season was signi®cant, F(1,95) 4.3, MSE 6.2. The main eect of subgroup was not signi®cant, F(1,95) 0.02, MSE 16.0, whereas the interaction was signi®cant, F(1,95) 25.5, MSE 6.2. Inspection of the means showed that both subgroups produced more errors on the second time of testing. This was con®rmed by separate one-way ANOVAs on each subgroup. For the summer±winter group, the mean number of errors per subject was 6.3 (SD 3.6) in summer and 7.4 (SD 3.2) in winter, F(1,60) 6.6, MSE 0.56. For the winter±summer group, the mean number of errors per subject was 8.3 (SD 3.2) in summer and 5.7 (SD 3.0) in winter, F(1,35) 17.4, MSE 0.72. The mean reaction time for correct responses in summer was 1290 ms (SD 454) and in winter it was 1180 ms (SD 393). An ANOVA showed that the main eects of season and subgroup were not signi®cant, F(1,95) 1.46, MSE 104,109, and F(1,95) 2.57, MSE 234,797, respectively. The interaction was signi®cant, F(1,95) 17.6, MSE 104,109. The mean reaction times for the summer±winter group were 1408 ms (SD 462) in summer and 1149 ms (SD 367) in winter, and for the winter±summer group were 1091 ms (SD 364) for summer and 1235 ms (SD 433) for winter. Separate one-way ANOVAs for each subgroup showed that for the summer±winter group a signi®cant practice eect was observed, F(1,60) 27.9, MSE 73,622, whereas for the winter±summer group the trend did not reach signi®cance, F(1,35) 2.36, MSE 156,373. In summary, performance of the dot perception task in summer had a signi®cantly higher error rate, as well as a trend for longer latencies. There was also a speed± accuracy tradeo between participants' ®rst and second times of testing: their correct reaction times were shorter in the second testing session, but their error rates were higher. Stroop For each participant and for each test session, the proportion of trials that were errors in the consistent and inconsistent conditions were computed. Errors included wrong key presses and latencies exceeding 3000 ms. The mean proportion of errors was 0.027 (SD 0.04) for the consistent condition and 0.082 (SD 0.08) for the inconsistent condition. The mean error rate for summer was 0.053 (SD 0.08) and for winter it was 0.056 (SD 0.07). An ANOVA with the factors of season, subgroup and consistency was performed on these data. There were no main eects of season, F(1,93) 0.50, MSE 0.004, or of subgroup, F(1,93) 2.68, MSE 0.008. There was a main eect of consistency, F(1,93) 90.2, MSE 0.003, and an interaction between consistency and subgroup, F(1,93) 4.29, MSE 0.003 which was explored by separate two-way ANOVAs for each subgroup. These showed that while both subgroups had signi®cantly higher error rates on inconsistent trials than on consistent ones, the eect was larger in the winter±summer subgroup (0.029 versus 0.099; F(1,35) 34.1, MSE 0.005) than in the summer±winter subgroup (0.026 versus 0.072; F(1,58) 55.9, MSE 0.002). In Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) 572 T. Brennen et al. both of these supplementary analyses the main eect of consistency was signi®cant and the interaction was not. The mean reaction time of correct responses on consistent trials was 896 ms (SD 284), whereas for inconsistent trials it was 1024 ms (SD 331). The mean reaction time of correct responses in summer was 984 ms (SD 326), and in winter it was 935 ms (SD 301). An ANOVA with the factors of season, subgroup and consistency showed that season did not have a main eect, F(1,93) 0.63, MSE 34,274, and nor did subgroup, F(1,93) 0.65, MSE 310,856. There was a main eect of consistency, F(1,93) 126.6, MSE 11,736. The two-way interaction between season and subgroup was also signi®cant, F(1,93) 47.0, MSE 34,274. Separate two-way ANOVAs for each subgroup showed that this was due to participants in both subgroups responding more rapidly in their second session. The summer±winter group had a mean of 1052 ms (SD 332) in summer and 903 ms (SD 277) in winter, F(1,59) 35.5, MSE 37,677, whereas the winter±summer group had a mean of 990 ms (SD 333) in winter and 871 ms (SD 283) in summer, F(1,35) 17.7, MSE 28,537. In both of these supplementary analyses the main eect of consistency was signi®cant and the interaction was not. There was no eect of season on performance of the Stroop task. Note also that in all the above analyses, both for error rate and reaction time, there was a main eect of consistency, and also that on none was the interaction between season and consistency signi®cant. Only on the reaction time data for the summer±winter group did it approach signi®cance, F(1,59) 3.3, MSE 5678, p 0.07, and this was a tendency to a reduced Stroop eect in winter. Mapping Errors were counted for each condition, subject and test session. They consisted either of reaction times over 2000 ms or wrong key presses. The mean number of errors per condition in summer was 0.83 (SD 1.2), and in winter was 0.82 (SD 1.1). The mean number of errors on compatible trials was 0.59 (SD 1.0), and on incompatible trials was 1.1 (SD 1.2). On a three-way ANOVA with the factors of season, subgroup and compatibility, the only signi®cant source of variance was a main eect of compatibility, F(1,97) 16.1, MSE 1.2. The mean reaction time on correct trials in summer was 508 ms (SD 124), and in winter was 493 ms (SD 97). The mean correct reaction time on compatible trials was 491 ms (SD 107), and on incompatible trials was 511 ms (SD 115). A three-way ANOVA with the factors of season, subgroup and compatibility was carried out on the data. The main eect of season was not signi®cant, F(1,97) 0.77, MSE 3740. A main eect of compatibility was observed, F(1,97) 24.0, MSE 1510. There were signi®cant interactions between season and subgroup, F(1,97) 33.0, MSE 3740, and between season and compatibility, F(1,97) 5.0, MSE 1012. These interactions were explored below. The mean reaction times for the summer±winter group were 533 ms (SD 132) in summer and 491 ms (SD 107) in winter, and for the winter±summer group were 465 ms (SD 92) for summer and 496 ms (SD 80) for winter. Separate one-way ANOVAs for each subgroup showed that for each subgroup a signi®cant practice Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) Arctic Cognition in Summer and Winter 573 eect was observed; for the summer±winter group, F(1,62) 24.6, MSE 4580, and for the winter±summer group, F(1,35) 15.5, MSE 2251. The interaction between season and compatibility was due to a reduced eect of compatibility in winter compared to summer. The compatibility eect in winter was 12 ms and in summer was 28 ms. Both were signi®cant eects, t(99) 2.6, p 5 0.01 and t(99) 5.8, p 5 0.0001. Compatible trials in summer were only 8 ms longer than compatible trials in winter and this dierence was not signi®cant, t(99) 0.39. However, incompatible trials in summer were 24 ms longer than incompatible trials in winter and this dierence was signi®cant, t(99) 2.6, p 5 0.01. There were no main eects of season in this task. Both a signi®cant practice eect and the classic Simon eect were observed. The Simon eect on reaction times was modi®ed by season: there was a reduced eect in winter. Time estimation While the raw data gives one indication of the accuracy of performance, a more informative way of presenting and processing the data is to use the absolute proportion deviation from the target time period. This avoids two problems: measuring absolute deviation ensures that over- and underestimates do not cancel each other out, and measuring proportion deviation makes a comparison between the three trial lengths fairer. The mean estimates for the three time periods in summer and winter and the absolute proportion deviations are shown in Table 2. An ANOVA on the absolute proportion deviation data revealed no main eect of season, F(1,97) 0.1, MSE 0.05. There was a main eect of subgroup, F(1,97) 7.31, MSE 0.125, due to the fact that participants in the winter± summer subgroup were more accurate than those in the summer±winter subgroup (mean absolute proportion deviation for summer±winter group 0.234 (SD 0.217); for the winter±summer group 0.153 (SD 0.147)). There was also a main eect of time period, F(1,97) 10.72, MSE 0.012. Contrasts showed that estimates of 15 seconds were the least precise, and that those for 30 and 60 seconds did not dier from each other in accuracy: for 60 versus 30, F 2.25, MSE 0.028; for 30 versus 15, F 9.3, MSE 0.114; for 60 versus 15, F 20.7, MSE 0.254. There was also a two-way interaction between season and season order, F(1, 97) 5.6, MSE 0.05. Separate one-way ANOVAs on each subgroup showed that Table 2. Mean time estimates in ms (standard deviation) and mean proportion of absolute deviation on time estimates (standard deviation) in summer and winter for three time periods 60 seconds Summer Winter Mean 30 seconds 15 seconds Mean Deviation deviation Time Deviation Time Deviation Time 61322 (16006) 61079 (14501) 0.177 (0.20) 0.179 (0.16) 33383 (8775) 32675 (6711) 0.215 (0.23) 0.183 (0.16) 17548 (4499) 17301 (3578) 0.245 (0.24) 0.221 (0.18) 61200 (15234) 0.178 (0.18) 33029 (7799) 0.199 (0.20) 17424 (4056) 0.233 (0.21) Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 0.212 (0.22) 0.194 (0.17) Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) 574 T. Brennen et al. this interaction was due to a tendency for each subgroup to be more accurate at the second time of testing: For the summer±winter subgroup the mean absolute proportion deviation for summer was 0.26 (SD 0.25) and for winter was 0.21 (SD 0.17), F(1,61) 3.8, MSE 0.07, p 0.055, whereas for the winter± summer group the mean absolute proportion deviation for summer was 0.13 (SD 0.13) and for winter was 0.17 (SD 0.16), F(1,36) 3.1, MSE 0.03, p 0.087. There was no seasonal eect on this task. Sternberg The raw data were checked for errors, which were counted up for each subject, condition and test session. There was a mean of 0.42 (SD 0.78) errors per condition in summer and 0.36 (SD 0.68) in winter. A four-way ANOVA with the factors of season, subgroup, target presence and set size was carried out on these data. There was no main eect of season, F(1,96) 0.30, MSE 0.52. The main eect of target presence was signi®cant, F(1,96) 31.4, MSE 0.39, with a mean of 0.52 (SD 0.84) errors in the present condition and 0.26 (SD 0.58) in the absent condition. There were fewer errors on the trials with three digits than those with ®ve; the mean error rates were 0.24 (SD 0.54) and 0.54 (SD 0.54) respectively, F(1,96) 39.9, MSE 0.402. There was a signi®cant interaction between set size and target presence, F(1,96) 4.6, MSE 0.47. This was due to a larger eect of target presence on set sizes of ®ve compared to set sizes of three, t(98) 5.1, p 5 0.0001 versus t(98) 2.1, p 5 0.05. The interaction between season and subgroup was also signi®cant, F(1,96) 6.9, MSE 0.52. Inspection of the means showed that both subgroups produced fewer errors at the second time of testing. For the summer±winter group, the mean number of errors per subject per condition was 0.42 (SD 0.8) in summer and 0.28 (SD 0.6) in winter. For the winter±summer group, the mean number of errors per subject was 0.38 (SD 0.7) in summer and 0.49 (SD 0.8) in winter. Separate oneway ANOVAs on each subgroup showed that this practice eect was signi®cant for the summer±winter subgroup, F(1,61) 6.6, MSE 0.54, but not for the winter± summer subgroup, F(1,35) 1.8, MSE 0.48, p 0.18. The mean correct reaction times were 1004 ms (SD 244) in summer and 974 ms (SD 226) in winter. A four-way ANOVA was carried out on these data, and showed no main eect of season, F(1,96) 0.18, MSE 53,334. There was a main eect of set size, F(1,96) 149.0, MSE 9837, trials with ®ve digits taking longer than trials with three, 1035 ms (SD 236) and 944 ms (SD 226). There was an interaction between season and subgroup, F(1,96) 26.2, MSE 53,334. This was due to a practice eect, both subgroups being quicker in their second test session; for the summer±winter subgroup the mean in summer was 1058 ms (SD 250) and in winter 963 ms (SD 242); for the winter±summer subgroup the mean in summer was 913 ms (SD 203) and in winter 994 ms (SD 195). This was con®rmed by separate three-way ANOVAs, where the main eect of season was signi®cant for both subgroups, F(1,61) 17.9, MSE 62,088, and F(1,35) 12.2, MSE 38,078, respectively. There was no eect of season on this task, just an eect of practice. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) Arctic Cognition in Summer and Winter 575 Face recognition For each participant, the number of errors on trials with famous and unfamiliar faces was computed, for summer and winter. Error arose in two ways: the incorrect key was pressed, or the latency exceeded 2000 ms. The mean number of errors for summer was 6.32 (SD 6.95) and the mean for winter was 6.36 (SD 6.91). The mean number of errors on famous faces was 10.18 (SD 7.45) and for unfamiliar faces was 2.50 (SD 3.29). A three-way ANOVA was carried out on these error data with the within-subjects factors of season and fame (with the levels of Famous and Unfamiliar faces) and the between-subjects factor of subgroup. There was no signi®cant eect of season, F(1,97) 0.34, MSE 7.1. The only signi®cant main eect was of fame, F(1,97) 95.6, MSE 56.0. There was a signi®cant interaction between season and subgroup, F(1,97) 8.3, MSE 7.1. This was explored by separate two-way ANOVAs for each subgroup, revealing that participants made signi®cantly more errors when tested the second time, regardless of whether that fell in summer or winter. For the winter±summer group the mean number of errors in winter was 5.4 (SD 5.9) and in summer was 6.3 (SD 6.6), F(1,36) 4.3, MSE 7.9. For the summer±winter group the mean number of errors in winter was 7.0 (SD 7.3) and in summer was 6.3 (SD 7.2) which approached signi®cance, F(1,61) 3.8, MSE 6.6, p 0.056. In both supplementary ANOVAs there was a main eect of fame and no signi®cant interaction. The mean reaction time on correct responses in summer was 887 ms (SD 162), and in winter was 864 ms (SD 170). The mean reaction time to recognize a famous face was 833 ms (SD 137) whereas the mean to correctly reject unfamiliar faces was 918 ms (SD 182). A three-way ANOVA with the factors of season, subgroup and fame was carried out on reaction times to correct trials, revealing no main eect of season, F(1,97) 0.47, MSE 10,037. There was a main eect of fame, F(1,97) 50.97, MSE 13,698, and a signi®cant interaction between season and subgroup, F(1, 97) 33.20, MSE 10,037. The three-way interaction was also signi®cant, F(1, 97) 11.22, MSE 5367. The two-way interaction between season and subgroup was explored by means of separate two-way ANOVAs for each subgroup. They showed that both subgroups were quicker when tested the second time. For the winter±summer group the mean reaction time in summer was 852 ms (SD 165), and in winter it was 905 ms (SD 165), F(1,36) 9.0, MSE 11,456. The means for the summer±winter group were 907 ms (SD 158) in summer and 840 ms (SD 154) in winter, F(1,61) 30.3, MSE 9199. Taken in conjunction with the error analysis of the same interaction, a speed-accuracy trade-o is observed: at the second time of testing participants were both quicker and made more errors. The three-way interaction is due to a reduction of the advantage of famous faces over unfamiliar faces in the second test session for both groups. The dierence in latency between famous and unfamiliar faces for the summer±winter group went from 103 ms in the ®rst test session to 61 ms in the second. For the winter±summer group it went from 121 ms to 62 ms. In summary, in the face-recognition task there was no seasonal eect, either on error rates or on reaction times. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) 576 T. Brennen et al. Word memory For each participant, and for each season, the proportion of old words produced in the inclusion and exclusion conditions was computed. The means for exclusion were 0.09 (SD 0.07) in summer and 0.07 (SD 0.06) in winter. The means for inclusion were 0.36 (SD 0.07) for summer and 0.37 (SD 0.14) for winter. Estimates of recollection and automatic processing were obtained using Jacoby et al.'s (1993) equations: Recollection Inclusion ÿ Exclusion, and Automatic Exclusion/(1 ÿ Recollection). The means for recollection in summer and winter were 0.270 (SD 0.16) and 0.298 (SD 0.17) respectively. The means for the automatic in¯uences were 0.12 (SD 0.08) in summer and 0.10 (SD 0.07) in winter. A separate two-way ANOVA was carried out for each of these scores, with the factors of Season and Subgroup. For recollection there were no signi®cant eects. For the automatic in¯uence, the interaction between season and subgroup was signi®cant, F(1,95) 9.8, MSE 0.004. Inspection of the means for each subgroup showed that there was a reduced automatic in¯uence upon the second time of testing. For the summer±winter group the mean in summer was 0.12 (SD 0.09) and in winter was 0.09 (SD 0.07), whereas for the winter±summer group the mean in summer was 0.09 (SD 0.08) and in winter was 0.11 (SD 0.07). Separate one-way ANOVAs were carried out on the automaticity scores for each subgroup. The summer±winter group scored higher in summer than winter, F(1,62) 10.8, MSE 0.004, whereas there was no signi®cant dierence for the winter±summer group, F(1,34) 2.1, MSE 0.003. At the end of the test session there was a surprise-free recall test. The mean number of words recalled in summer was 12.1 (SD 6.6), and in winter was 14.4 (SD 8.7). On a two-way ANOVA the eect of season was signi®cant, F(1,95) 6.2, MSE 22.9. The interaction between season and subgroup was also signi®cant, F(1,95) 8.6, MSE 22.9. The mean number of words recalled by the summer± winter group was 11.5 (SD 6.3) in summer and 15.4 (SD 9.1) in winter. The mean number of words recalled by the winter±summer group was 13.1 (SD 7.2) in summer and 12.8 (SD 7.7) in winter. Separate one-way ANOVAs for the two subgroups showed that only for the summer±winter group was there a signi®cant eect of season, F(1,61) 18.0, MSE 25.8. The data from the free recall test are thus consistent with a model where there are advantages of winter over summer, and of second-time performance over ®rst-time performance. There was no eect of season or practice on recollection. The automatic in¯uence was reduced at a second time of testing (a reliable eect for one subgroup) and was not aected by the seasonal manipulation. In free recall there was evidence of a winter advantage. Fluency The mean number of words generated in summer was 15.74 (SD 4.57), and for winter was 15.03 (SD 4.96). The scores for each participant in each test session were analysed by an ANOVA with the factors of season and subgroup. There was no eect of subgroup, F(1,95) 0.646, MSE 28.4, whereas the eect of season was marginal, F(1,95) 3.34, MSE 16.3, p 0.07. This was modi®ed by a signi®cant two-way interaction, F(1,95) 6.17, MSE 16.3. Separate one-way ANOVAs for Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) Arctic Cognition in Summer and Winter 577 each subgroup revealed that whereas for the summer±winter subgroup there was no dierence between summer and winter (mean in summer 15.4 (SD 4.4), mean in winter 15.8 (SD 5.1); F(1,60) 0.35, MSE 13.6), for the winter±summer subgroup there was a signi®cant advantage for summer over winter (mean in summer 16.3 (SD 4.9), mean in winter 13.7 (SD 4.4); F(1,35) 5.8, MSE 20.8). The pattern of performance on the verbal ¯uency task is consistent with a twoprocess model where the eect of practice tends to increase a participant's score on the second testing, and where summer has an advantage over winter. DISCUSSION There is a growing literature on the eects of the annual ¯uctuations in day length on humans. Mood swings through the seasons are now well documented, with winter being a time of worse moods than summer. The hypothesized causative agent is shortage of daylight in winter, thus leading to the idea that swings will be larger, the further from the equator. Circannual cognitive swings have not been previously studied on a large scale. In this study, a group of 100 people living at 698N were tested on a battery of cognitive tests in both summer and winter. One dependent variable used was reaction time, in order to detect even subtle disadvantages in winter performance. Summarizing across tests, this large database does not provide evidence of cognitive impairment in winter. Indeed, on the tests showing summer±winter dierences, four showed winter advantages, whereas only one showed a winter disadvantage. Of all the cognitive capacities covered in the battery of tasks, the only one to show the predicted disadvantage in winter was verbal ¯uency: on average, participants generated 0.7 more words in summer than in winter. On the purest measure of simple reaction time, where subjects responded to a visual stimulus with a key press as quickly as possible, there was a signi®cant advantage for winter. Bearing in mind that the participants in this study experience extreme seasonal swings in the amount of natural daily light to which they are exposed, this result rules out any notion of a pure low-level cognitive de®cit in winter, and sets limits on accounts of seasonal processing in humans. Performance on another simple speeded reaction time task, the dot numerosity task, was more error-prone in summer, again irreconcilable with a winter disadvantage. Another possibility was that even if reaction time did not increase in winter, attention would be less focused and participants would be more easily confused. By looking at the dierence between consistent and inconsistent trials on the Stroop and mapping tasks, we have measures of confusability. There was a signi®cant reduction in confusability in winter, as measured in the mapping task, and no seasonal dierence in the Stroop task. Again, claims that humans are more confused in winter can be rejected because if Tromsù participants are not impaired in winter then there is no reason to expect more southerly humans to be so. Memory for a word list was tested in three dierent ways. On the measures of recollection and automatic in¯uences there were no seasonal eects whereas on the free recall task, there was an advantage for winter performance. On the remaining tests ± face recognition, short-term memory scanning and time estimation ± there were Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) 578 T. Brennen et al. no seasonal eects. Overall, this study has found that there is very little in¯uence of the seasons on cognitive performance. Furthermore, the majority of the circannual variations found in this study are winter advantages, rather than disadvantages. Note that the age of participants in this study (M 31, SD 10) is in the right range for them to be susceptible to SAD. In all the investigations of SAD and cognition described above, the patients' mean age was between 30 and 40. The participants in this study were thus at peak sensitivity to seasonal changes on mood. Although there is no data on SAD incidence in our sample for the period of the study, the size and the heterogeneity of the sample puts the burden of proof on anyone wishing to claim that cognition performance is subject to circannual rhythms, in SAD or non-SAD populations. The battery of tests took over 90 minutes to complete, so that it demanded considerable eort and concentration. It would be most unconvincing to argue, for instance, that participants were able to compensate for a winter impairment by making an extra eort ± this would absolutely not be a trivial feat. Any concerns about the power of this study to detect cognitive disadvantages in winter become irrelevant when four of ®ve signi®cant circannual eects are in the unexpected direction. It should also be pointed out that the size of the eect on the verbal ¯uency task was just less than SD 0.12, and so this study was powerful enough to detect even small eects. Above, we have listed many reasons why this study was suited to detecting any rhythms in cognition. Despite the subjective feeling one may have that one is mentally sluggish in winter, our data do not lend empirical support to the intuitive claim. This result is similar to that reported in the literature on menstruation and cognition. While women's self-report is that they are intellectually impaired in the premenstrual and menstrual phases compared to the preluteal phase, there is converging evidence for a total absence of such impairment, both in cognitive tasks and in real examination performance (Richardson, 1991a, b). It has also been found that neither women with, nor women without, premenstrual dysphoric disorder show cognitive rhythms over the menstrual cycle: those with premenstrual dysphoric disorder may show a marginal de®cit that is constant over the cycle (Keenan et al., 1995; Resnick et al., 1998). It will be interesting to see if future investigations show more similarities between the menstrual and the circannual cycles. It is tempting to extrapolate from the literature on the menstrual cycle and predict, for instance, that SAD patients will fail to show a cyclical cognitive pattern, just a general non-cyclical de®cit when compared to normals. Another parallel between menstrual and circannual cycles is that, just as Richardson (1991b) argued that the counterintuitive absence of menstrual cognitive rhythms could be used to remove psychological barriers to women in places of study, we would argue that the absence of a winter disadvantage on cognitive tasks should act as a counterbalance to one's intuitions that intellectual performance at high latitudes must be worse in winter. Interestingly, the intuition of cognitive impairment during menstruation and the intuition of cognitive impairment in winter are both relatively recent in origin. Richardson (1991b) shows that the idea that menstruating women are less eective mentally originates from the ®rst half of the twentieth century. The link between annual rhythms and human performance is even more recent. Rosenthal et al. (1984) ®rst reported the circannual mood disorder SAD, and it has been widely assumed since that time that cognition is also aected. The idea that the lack of daylight in Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) Arctic Cognition in Summer and Winter 579 winter at 698N will have a debilitating eect on human cognition apparently has great plausibility, to both academics and non-academics, north and south of the Arctic Circle, but is simply not supported by this study. Although the ideas in the SAD literature and anecdotal evidence and the extreme latitude were all reasons to expect a winter de®cit in cognition in this study there was little evidence for one: there was more support for a summer de®cit. This study, which was carried out at 698N and which was designed to detect any circannual cognitive rhythms, provides no support for the idea that cognitive performance is impaired in winter. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This study was supported by grant 114162/330 from the Norwegian Research Council and also by grant 211/97 from Nansenfondet, Den Norske Videnskaps-Akademi. Thanks are due to Kristin Johannessen, Charlotte Kristiansen, Serge BreÂdart and Ragnhild Dybdahl for assistance. REFERENCES Beringer, J. (1992). Timing accuracy of mouse response registration on the IBM microcomputer family. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 24(3), 486±490. Borenstein, M., Rothstein, H. and Cohen, J. (1997). Power and precision: A computer program for statistical power analysis and con®dence intervals. Englewood Clis, NJ: Biostat. Drake, C. L., Schwartz, P. J., Turner, E. H. and Rosenthal, N. E. (1997). Cognitive performance in Seasonal Aective Disorder: Pattern recognition and the Stroop task. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185, 56±59. Jacoby, L. L., Toth, J. P. and Yonelinas, A. P. (1993). Separating conscious and unconscious in¯uences of memory: Measuring recollection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 139±154. Kasper, S., Rogers, S. L. B., Yancy, A., Schulz, P. M., Skwerer, R. G. and Rosenthal, N. E. (1989). Phototherapy in individuals with and without subsyndromal seasonal aective disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 837±844. Keenan, P. A., Lindamer, L. A. and Jong, S. K. (1995). Menstrual phase-independent retrieval de®cit in women with PMS. Biological Psychiatry, 38, 369±377. Magnusson, A. and Stefansson, J. (1993). Prevalence of seasonal aective disorder in Iceland. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 941±946. Michalon, M., Eskes, G. A. and Mate-Kole, C. C. (1997). Eects of light therapy on neuropsychological function and mood in Seasonal Aective Disorder. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 22, 19±28. O'Brien, J. T., Sahakian, B. J. and Checkley, S. (1993). Cognitive impairments in patients with Seasonal Aective Disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 338±343. Palinkas, L. A., Houseal, M. and Rosenthal, N. E. (1996). Subsyndromal Seasonal Aective Disorder in Antarctica. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 184, 530±534. Resnick, A., Perry, W., Parry, B., Mosto®, N. and Udell, C. (1998). Neuropsychological performance across the menstrual cycle with and without premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Psychiatry Research, 77, 147±158. Richardson, J. T. E. (1991a). Cognition, memory and the menstrual cycle. CPC: Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 11, 3±26. Richardson, J. T. E. (1991b). The menstrual cycle and student learning. Journal of Higher Education, 62, 317±340. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999) 580 T. Brennen et al. Rosen, L. N., Targum, S. D., Terman, M., Bryant, M. J., Homan, H., Kapser, S. F., Hamovit, J. R., Docherty, J. P., Welch, B. and Rosenthal, N. E. (1990). Prevalence of Seasonal Aective Disorder at four latitudes. Psychiatry Research, 31, 131±144. Rosenthal, N. E. (1993). Winter blues. New York: Guilford Press. Rosenthal, N. E., Sack, D. A., Gillin, J. C., Lewy, A. J., Goodwin, F., Davenport, Y., Mueller, P. S., Newsome, D. A. and Wehr, T. A. (1984). Seasonal aective disorder, a description of the syndrome and preliminary ®ndings with light therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 72±80. Smith, A. (1989). Diurnal variations in performance. In A. Colley and J. Beech (Eds), Acquisition and performance of cognitive skills ( pp. 301±325). Chichester: Wiley. Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652±654. Taylor, A. J. and Duncum, K. (1987). Some cognitive eects of wintering-over in the Antarctic. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 16, 93±94. Terman, M., Terman, J. S., Quitkin, F. M., McGrath, P. J., Stewart, J. W. and Raerty, B. (1989). Light therapy for seasonal aective disorder ± a review of ecacy. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2, 1±22. Thompson, C. and Isaacs, G. (1988). Seasonal aective disorder ± a British sample: symptomatology in relation to mode of referral and diagnostic subtype. Journal of Aective Disorders, 14, 1±11. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: 561±580 (1999)