cfiWtSf. * M.I.T. LIBRARIES - DEWEY Dewey hd28 .M414 00'A- A GRAMMATICAL MODEL OF ORGANIZATIONAL ROUTINES IN A TECHNICAL SERVICE ORGANIZATION by Brian T. Pentland CCS TR# 177, Sloan ^ He^fu WP # 3721-94 August 1994 fj . EuejU^- MASSACHUSETTS INSTI rUTE OF TECHNOLOGY DEC 01 1994 LIBRARIES A GRAMMATICAL MODEL OF ORGANIZATIONAL ROUTINES IN A TECHNICAL SERVICE ORGANIZATION DRAFT Accepted for Publication in Administrative Science Quarterly Brian T. Pentland University of California. Los Angeles Henry H. Rueter University of Michigan The authors wish to thank Stephen Barley, Michael Cohen, Connie Gersick, Barbara Lawrence, Lance Sandelands, Robert Sutton, Burt Swanson, Karl Weick, Elaine Yakura and the anonymous ASQ reviewers for their comments on earlier drafts of this paper. We would also like to thank Linda Pike for her expert editing, and Louise Soe and Sidne Ward for their help with the data analysis. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the Academy of Management, Las Vegas, NV, August 1992. Organizational Routines A GRAMMATICAL MODEL OF ORGANIZATIONAL ROUTINES IN A TECHNICAL SERVICE ORGANIZATION ABSTRACT This paper explores the sequential structure of work processes involves high numbe .. in a task unit whose work of exceptions, low analyzability of search, frequent interruptions and extensive deliberation, and cannot be characterized as routine under any traditional definition. Yet a detailed analysis of the sequential pattern of action in a sample of 168 service interactions reveals that most calls follow a repetitive, functionally similar pattern. This apparent contradiction presents a challenge to our theoretical understanding of routines: apparently non-routine work display such a high degree of regularity? question, patterns we propose a new and a illustrate a definition of organizational routines new methodology for how can To answer this as a set of functionally similar studying the sequential structure of work processes using rule-based grammatical models. This approach to organizational routines juxtaposes the structural features of the organization against the reflective agency of organizational members. Members enact potentially large) set of possibilities that regular patterns of action we specific performances from among a constrained (but can be described by a grammar, giving label routines. rise to the * . i Organizational Routines Routines occupy a used critical position in to explain the organization theory. As genetic medial quality of organizational structure Hannan & Freeman, in evolutionary theories (Nelson have become a cornerstone & Winter, 1982; in theories of organizational learning and adaptation, as well (Levitt 1991; Walsh action, & Ungson, 1983). As memory, & March, 1988; Cohen, Routines occupy the crucial nexus between structure and 1991). to organizational theory, there have been very few attempts the sequential structure of organizational routines. summary measures collected difficult to In spite of their to deal empirically with Instead, existing research has usually that ignore the sequential structure of the action (Lynch, 1974; and Macintosh, 1981; Withey, Routines are Our routines between the organization as an object and organizing as a process. importance Daft material, routines are Daft, and Cooper, 1983). study because they are essentially complex patterns of social action. tools for characterizing the variable properties of static objects are well developed, but our tools for characterizing the sequential structure of patterns of action are not. difficulty lies in objectify two different senses of the word "routine." As a noun, An "routine" additional is used to a collective capacity to perform recognizable patterns of action (Nelson and Winter, 1982). The needed to patterns themselves summarize them as a may vary considerably from instance to instance, so a label fixed entity. As an adjective, "routine" indicates a is judgment about a variable property of a pattern of action, such as repetitiveness (Gersick and Hackman, 1990), analyzability (Perrow, 1967), variety (Daft and Macintosh, 1981), or even "mindlessness" (Ashforth and Fried, 1988). These two senses of the term considerable amount of confusion about the underlying construct. are usually spoken of vaguely, The purpose of this paper is in Perhaps the plural, rather than concretely, to present a theoretical model seem in to lead to this is why a routines the singular. that juxtaposes our concept of routines as patterns of action with our concept of routineness as a property that such patterns Organizational Routines might possess. function of a We illustrate this medium model with an example, taken from the software support sized software vendor, that presents us with an apparent contradiction. This task unit exhibits repetitive, functionally similar patterns of action stimuli (classic features of organizational routines), yet and a highly deliberative search process analyzability, has high task in response to defined low task variety, with interruptions (classic filled features of non-routine work). To resolve routines this apparent contradiction, we believe and the methods between routines as patterns we believe so, we suggest it is necessary we with which to of action it is study them. necessary and routineness as a variable property of such patterns, examine the sequential structure of the patterns themselves. To do a novel methodology for representing routines using grammatical models work by applying the grammatical modeling technique organization. the distinction In addition to clarifying (Pentland, forthcoming; Olson, Herbsleb and Rueter, forthcoming). prior concept of to rethink the There are many ways to This paper builds on our to sequential data from a specific model sequences of events (Abbott, 1991), but grammatical models have distinct advantages when applied to organizational routines and other repetitive organizational processes. For that is this a grammar does not specify a fixed outcome; among which members accomplish defines a set of possibilities from action. First, highly congenial to the theoretical conceptions of routines have "sub-routines" and Simon, 1958). between events are that in may be follows. models are widely separated organizational routines. in Each in a way mentioned above. Second, organizations, to form new where routines routines (March well suited to representing dependencies an observed sequence (Chomsky, 1956), which Finally, because they provide a clear way a routine from other instances. in can be rearranged and combined Third, grammatical that commonplace practical value sequences of reason, grammatical models acknowledge both structure and agency grammatical models can capture the layered quality of action often specific it grammatical models have potential to distinguish normatively correct instances of of these features will be demonstrated in the analysis that Organizational Routines We begin by examining various concepts of routines Drawing on these perspectives, literature. in we argue that an organizational routine can be seen as a set of possible patterns that need not be fixed or automatic. Simon (1969) some in complex patterns respects, of behavior. in that we argue and sociological the organizational Our argument can generate that situational structures To demonstrate our argument, we use the parallels that of example of a task that violates the traditional definitions of routineness from the organizational literature. then use a action degree new grammatical method in this task unit to show (Olson et al, in that in spite of of sequential structure as defined its unit We press) to analyze the sequential patterns of apparent non-routineness, it exhibits a high by the grammar. These findings reinforce the need a new, broader formulation of the concept of organizational routines, and a new for set of tools for analyzing their sequential structure. DEFINITIONS OF ORGANIZATIONAL ROUTINES At the risk of oversimplifying a large, diverse body of work, one can discern perspectives in the literature on routinized behavior. There is, for some broad example, a micro-level perspective that emphasizes the cognitive processes of individuals (Ashforth and Fried, 1988), and a more macro-level perspective (Nelson and Winter, 1982). that emphasizes structural and institutional constraints Levels of analysis are blurred, of course, because situational factors influence script selection and performance (Gioia and Poole, 1984), and the cognitive processes of individuals can enact situational structures (Weick, 1979). There is also a difference of opinion as to whether routines are fixed entities that exist independently of their accomplishment (March and Simon, 1958) or as the performances unfold anew each distinction time (Giddens, 1984). The difference is of acting subjects that similar to Weick's (1979) between organization and organizing; the noun form encourages us phenomenon as a static thing, while the verb form encourages us to see the dynamic process. And as mentioned above, "routineness" possess, whether they are fixed or dynamic. In is to see the phenomenon as a a property that such patterns might the discussion that follows, we distill some Organizational Routines insights from each of these themes and introduce a novel, grammatical perspective on organizational routines. Routines as Automatic Responses Early concepts of routines defined stimulus. emphasized a fixed pattern of individual behavior governed by performance programs." March & Simon (1958) went on in organizations, to consider is when an could be considered "routinized": We will been regard a set of activities as routinized, then, to the degree that choice has development simplified by the of a fixed response search has been eliminated, but a choice remains and systematic computing The idea of a fixed the foundation Throughout routine, we will in to defined stimuli. the form of a clearly defined say that the still If activities are (March & Simon, 1958: 142) routinized. response to a given stimulus, accompanied by the absence of search, provides upon which much subsequent theorizing about routines has been based. their discussion, and the response More response to a Using the newly popular computational metaphor, March & Simon (1958: 142) asserted that "[m]ost behavior, and particularly most behavior activity in lies in to that the link the perceptions and reactions of the individuals and Fried (1988) use the concept recently, Ashforth and Abelson, 1977) March and Simon (1958) argue of in between the stimulus the organization. an event schema or script (Schank argue that routines are sustained by the cognitive structures of individual organizational members. Ashforth and Fried (1988) argue that scripted behavior occurs under the following circumstances: (1) presence of an event schema; stimulus cues; (3) presence of action rules; (4) minimal required unstructured subroutines; and (6) llgen (1985) offer a similar list that these conditions to appear absence of interruptions effort; (5) (2) categorizable absence and unmet expectations. of criteria for routinization of individual behavior. of Weiss and Given be rather common, Ashforth and Fried (1988) argue that 6 Organizational Routines scripted or mindless behavior and informal is commonplace and power-based interactions, in operating routines, decision making, formal interactions. argument respect, their March and Simon (1958). mirrors that of At the group level of analysis, Gersick and Hackman routines' that retains the notions of a fixed stimulus A In this habitual routine exists (1990: 69) offer a definition of "habitual and the absence when a group repeatedly of search: exhibits a functionally similar pattern of behavior in a given stimulus situation without explicitly selecting it over alternative ways of behaving. Unlike Ashforth and Fried (1988), Gersick and Hackman (1990) do not concept of individual scripts as an explanatory construct. habitual routines emerge out of the complex interaction of They argue rely directly that at the group members. on the group Routines are repetitive patterns of action that are "functionally similar," but not necessarily fixed. may be some words, there variety in the patterns enacted by the group, but level, no In other explicit deliberation. At the organizational level of analysis, Nelson & Winter (1982) introduced a wide variety of metaphors for routines: routines targets for control, replication, kind of thing. The as genes, routines as memory, routines as truce, routines as and imitation. Each definition of routine offered of these metaphors portrays a routine as a by Winter (1986) expands on March & Simon's (1958) notion of a fixed response to include patterns of behavior that function as a recognizable unit: ...to the extent to which that reference is made to a relatively complex pattern of behavior (or the theoretical representation of such a pattern) triggered by a relatively small number recognizable unit in of initiating signals or choices and functioning as a a relatively automatic fashion, to that extent reference is Organizational Routines made to something called a routine in evolutionary theory. (Winter, 1986: 165) Instead of focusing on measurable properties of a task unit's work flow (Perrow, 1967), Nelson and Winter (1982) shifted the focus of theoretical interest to the patterns themselves, objectified The under the label "routines." elements of stimulus definition retains the familiar and automatic response, but unfortunately, the concept of routine as used is unacceptably vague and encompassing, as Nelson and It I use the word this in evolutionary theory quote from Winter (1986: 165) illustrates: a way of doing things. "routine" as the generic term for simultaneously the counterpart of a wide range of terms employed is everyday life and in in various theoretical languages, including those of orthodox and behavioral economic theory; among these terms are decision rule, technique, standard operating procedure, management practice, policy, strategy, skill, information system, information structure, program, script and organization form. Winter's (1986) definition of constructs across all is both troubling and intriguing because levels of analysis. In all In as Effortful draws upon a wide range the manifestations Winter (1986) alludes however, routines are essentially automatic, executed without Routines it to, explicit deliberation or choice. Accomplishments recent social theory, routines are also a central concept, but have received a rather different theoretical treatment. of structuration: Giddens (1984) "The concept of routinization, as grounded to the theory of structuration" (p. 60, The as a crucial aspect of the theory identifies routinization emphasis in original). regular or routine features of encounters, in in practical consciousness, He goes on to argue time as well as represent institutionalized features of social systems. Routine is in is vital that: space, founded in Organizational Routines tradition, custom or habit, phenomena need no but is it a major error to suppose that these explanation, that they are simply repetitive forms of behavior carried out 'mindlessly'. On the contrary, as Goffman (together with ethnomethodology) has helped to demonstrate, the routinized character of most social activity sustain it in something that has is their effortful is accomplishment. "an achievement from at' who continually by those explicit definition of organizational routines that routinized social activity conversation analysis. be 'worked day-to-day conduct. (Giddens, 1984: 86). While Giddens does not offer an insight here to A similar in the literature on discourse and 89) argues, interaction possibilities," not critical . not mindless or automatic, but rather an theme can be found As Schegloff (1982: among is per se the must always be treated as something given or pre-planned. Garfinkel (1967) and his followers have demonstrated repeatedly that the normal troubles and practical contingencies of daily Even some of the life require constant interpretation most routinized kinds and buying stamps (Ventola, 1987) on the part the participants to of encounters, and such as exhibit a considerable repair (Heritage, 1984). fast food service (Leidner, 1993) amount of variety and require effort accomplish successfully. Customers need to be trained and controlled so that they conform to the expectations of the service provider (Leidner, 1993). Service interactions are especially prone to variation, of course, because they are interactional Note that organizational routines frequently require the participation of multiple products. individuals; as complex interactional products, they are unlikely to unfold the same way every time. These studies call into serious question any concept of routines as fixed or automatic. potentially resolve the apparent different disagreement by suggesting that the authors are phenomena; perhaps what Ashforth and than what Giddens (1984) means. Fried (1988) mean by a Unfortunately, insofar as either of One talking routine is could about different them uses concrete Organizational Routines examples (neither reports any activity: appear to be about the talking formal and informal encounters and task situations of The differences that data), they these two broad perspectives on routinized in each perspective emerged as a corrective to activity economic dogma that all are not surprising, given alternative. organizational behavior literature, the idea that routines are automatic the universe of kinds. all an overzealous same In was a the reaction against decision making involves perfect knowledge and optimal choice. Behavioral economics can also be seen as a continuing reaction against this entrenched orthodoxy (Winter, 1986). effortful In sociology (and ethnomethodology, in particular), accomplishments was a reaction against the Parsonian notion the idea that routines are of the social agents as judgmental dopes whose behavior was predetermined by social structure (Heritage, 1984). Seen in the context of their antitheses, each of these theses makes a There may also be a methodological bias that can help account The perspective In contrast, all of the minor troubles the difference and improvisations (e. g., Garfinkel, of daily life the perspective that views routines as automatic responses methods designed to elicit Leigh and McGraw, 1989). and then confirm a It in perspectives. accomplishments usually draws on detailed that views routines as effortful ethnographic observations of people working and interacting unavoidable that for great deal of sense. specific pattern in the is 1967), so it is should be apparent. associated with minds of subjects (e. g., should not be surprising that these two methodological approaches lead to differing interpretations of the phenomenon. ROUTINES AS GRAMMARS OF ACTION To help reconcile these apparently conflicting perspectives on routinized social action, propose a theoretical model that allows us model of organizational routines to integrate the insights must acknowledge that routinized behavior enabled by the cognitive structures of individuals, such as 1 from each of them. scripts, is we Such a constrained and as well as physical and social Organizational Routines At the same, structures of the organization. we that gives rise to the particular patterns We must allow it define sets of possibilities. sequence of elements or events, The concept Kennedy and Thomason, 1978) and nearly anything else create grammars we can chromosomes (Gonzalez and To set. Our method builds sequences the of action the extent observed on Salancik and Leblebici (1988), who used a grammatical work by using a grammatical model processes as observed in an in detect sequential structure and functionally We to represent the possible patterns of interaction in food service transactions. their for stories processes (such as routines) can be described as ordered sets of actions, organizational process using a grammar, on applied to any ordered can be described as an ordered that By expressing model One can problem, because Milanesi, 1990), tasks (Sandelands, 1987), they can also be described by grammars. similar patterns. for this grammar can be 1987), regular polygons (Miclet, 1986), political history (Alker, that organizational of good choice linguistic or otherwise. (Prince, 1973; Ryan, 1979; Colby, and agency observe. believe that grammatical models are a particularly grammars for the individual effort extend express the sequential structure of work to a particular organization. Pentland (forthcoming) presents a detailed analogy between organizational routines and grammar, as shown the same way grammar in Figure that linguists In that . analogy, an organizational routine has a grammar. use grammar to describe certain never the whole routine 1 (or to aspects of a routine. What we a whole language), but only specific instances of we it. same way allows speakers to produce a variety of sentences grammar organizational routine allows members natural language like English can generate an large lexicon and a (Coulmas, 1981). large An number to produce variety of will call performances. infinite variety of of rules), only some of , In 1 the an Of course, a sentences (due to an extremely which are ever actually uttered organizational routine would typically allow a comparatively small 1 is In linguistics, them performances. in the behavioral world, use will actually observe, empirically, these are called sentences, but that English we describe certain aspects of a language, In t « - . . * Organizational Routines number of possible performances, only may have language, which interrogative), a task unit Employees rentals, which are enacted by the participants. several different categories of sentences may have (e. g., declarative, several different categories of routines may have identifiable pieces embedded some its within them. In organization theory, repertoire. In has long been observed that processes are also it constructed from constituents, sometimes called "subroutines" (March domain in sentences are constructed from syntactic constituents such as noun phrases, verb phrases, and so on. the Like a returns. Like sentences, performances linguistics, of a video store, for example, would enact different kinds of performances for sales, in and some of customer service, a typical subroutine might other authorization number. (Pentland, 1992). Moves are like Finally, at the the words in be the most concrete & Simon, 1958). In validation of a credit card or level, we have actions or moves the organizational lexicon from which performances are formed, and can be operationalized through a coding scheme, as described below. from This multi-level analogy allows us to linguistics, where the unit of analysis is the unit of analysis is make the best use of the corresponding concepts the sentence. In the method described here, the performance, and the goal of the analysis to induce a grammar that describes the language (organizational routine) by analyzing a set of individual sentences (performances). (*** This analogy suggests a INSERT FIGURE somewhat 1 ABOUT HERE "* surprising insight: an organizational routine pattern, but rather a set of possible patterns (enabled and constrained by a is not a single variety of organizational, social, physical and cognitive structures) from which organizational members enact particular performances. There are many activity, for fall example, but within certain to bounds. different variations of a be recognizable as competent customer Each instance is customer service service, each instance must also, to a greater or lesser extent, the effortful 12 Organizational Routines accomplishment of the participants and the emergent product of this definition is quite similar to that of similar (but not fixed) patterns that is emerge through Hackman (1990), What interaction. in that points to it is fixed, to some extent, the space of possibilities for action (although that too can change, with sufficient effort). What are accomplished, with varying degrees of reflective agency and we patterns A Gersick and Note that their interactions. are the particular observe. part of this conception of routines lies critical effort, in and the relationship between structure agency. As Giddens (1984) and others have argued, rules, norms, schema, scripts, and other cognitive artifacts are resources for action, but they cannot be understood as determining action. Furthermore, individual cognitive structures of artifacts that can shape the flow of action actions called structure, moves are shaped by as well as by the need among distributed individuals in to in a Pentland (1992) shows situation. the physical and draw upon the the organization. are only one category of this kind ritual how dimensions of organizational distributed cognitive resources that are In Pentland's framework, moves are constrained and enabled by features of the situation, so that a limited repertoire of be available to members at any given time. The argument here beyond quality of action in organizations extends The enabling and constraining hierarchy, division of labor, unitary individual is moves moves will simply that the situated to include sequences of moves. structures that typify organizational work situations (such as and a myriad of task- and situation-specific particulars) naturally give rise to regular patterns of action. Hutchins' (1991) analysis of a bridge crew manually navigating a ship navigation system failed provides an excellent illustration. (obtaining sightings, charting the current position, etc.) constraints ways of and affordances present accomplishing while, the in to settle the electronic distributed nature of the task combined with the physical the situation, created the possibility of several different After struggling with these possibilities for a their navigational task. crew was able The when on one particular division of labor 1 3 and sequence of actions that Organizational Routines was most nature of efficient (Hutchins, 1991). many This example highlights the collective and distributed organizational routines: because no single individual could navigate the ship, the was a pattern of action Defining routines in this collective way accomplishment. also helps to clarify and preserve the distinction between routines as sequences of action and routineness as a variable property that can be attributed to such a To pattern. for the reinforce this distinction, ambiguous and somewhat repetitive, more or less automatic, one might substitute the less value-laden term "process" pejorative term "routine." embody more Processes can be more or less or less variety, search, perspective, an organizational routine need not be especially "routine," as in the example From and so on. we this demonstrate shall that follows. AN EMPIRICAL EXAMPLE Database International (DBI — a pseudonym) creates and mainframe computers that we will call 24 hour-a-day technical assistance The staff of to customers who are having 1 00 new software support group on a telephone "hot of the operators in The software support group SystemTen. 25 specialists received about answered by one a software product sells line" calls a week. difficulty Customers at for DBI offers using their product. call the DBI or by electronic mail, both of which the "customer service center". These incoming messages provided the "stimulus" necessary to invoke the customer service routine at DBI. After operators determine whether the caller has a valid service contract (which nearly do), there was essentially no deliberation about how to proceed. similar response: the operator "opens" a call in DBI's call tracking problem to that follow, one of five "functional areas," we use data for (b) to all The stimulus always callers elicited document In the sections the non-routine develop a grammatical model that describes of functionally similar patterns. 14 a database and assigns the a solution begins. collected at DBI for two purposes: (a) to nature of the problem-solving work and work as a set where the search were this Organizational Routines Data The data reported here were conducted by the access to the which to work. informed that: unit and provided a passcard needed Before data collection began, (1) no individual group was being evaluated individuals The management author (Pentland, 1992). first work collected as part of a larger study of software support hot lines in was all members of the and with management; and (3) confidentiality in level of office in DBI support group were any way; (2) no individual or would be maintained both with other (4) the confidentiality of the involved would be protected with the public. DBI granted a high and an unoccupied to gain entry obligated to participate any way; at The members of the organization and all persons DBI support group were very cooperative and generous with their time, thereby allowing the collection of the observational and archival data reported here. Data collection continued Observational Data. observation. looking things up in he was sometimes asked manuals, or running down the observe a situation that an informant the first notes. felt first five days a week, of participant was predominantly author's role to help with simple tasks hall to get help. such as copying, Occasionally, he might be especially interesting. Most was invited of the time, author sat with a support specialist, observing what he or she did and taking detailed It was usually possible to ask questions to clarify what have been done, and so on. Unless the discuss their work. situations It was situation was the day, and the had happened, what else might urgent, informants were generally happy Throughout the page number. text, of each day, quotations from field field notes were typed For example, "(DBI, 6.3)" would refer to day six, page Archival Data. With the help of DBI personnel, a random sample of 335 calls was extracted from DBI's tracking database, which contained the histories of over 20,000 problems. 1 5 in notes are referenced by the three of the field notes. call to also possible to retreat to an office and write up observations about where note-taking was not possible. At the end the customary manner. site, months, Given the technical nature of the work, the that of observer, although to for three Organizational Routines Colby, Kennedy and Milanesi (1990) recommend a minimum creating grammatical models of folktales, so this sample size we chose one random day extract this sample, all calls received database it would is per of 50 instances as a basis seemed more typically involve many distinct is To than adequate. week (Monday through Friday) on that day concerning DBI's main product, SystemTen. assigned a unique number and for and selected Each instance referred to by the staff at DBI as a phone conversations. To get 335 calls, "call," a in this although total of 21 days were sampled between March 1988 and February 1989. The average elapsed time on the calls in the The sample was 102 days. call tracking database provided convenient access often stretched out over a period of weeks or months. to the entire history of The database was used by the DBI keep contemporaneous notes on what steps had been taken, what needed Observations by the first to be done next, staff to and so on. author indicated that the specialists were quite diligent about maintaining these records. Since the database served a variety of useful purposes for the support specialists and their managers, the information recorded not necessarily complete problems that in every problem (an essential feature for detail. The records cover our analysis), but they was quite reliable, although the complete history of each limit the amount of detail that can be recovered about any particular interaction because specialists recorded only the actions and events that were needed follows, the tendency for their own purposes. As we to omit certain actions shall demonstrate in from the database reduces the the analysis that fit of our hypothesized grammar, thus understating the extent of the regularity of the actual patterns of activity. Coding the archival data . The coding scheme used here was adapted from Pentland (1992) take advantage of the call tracking database used at DBI. set of basic problems moves that support specialists Pentland's (1992) analysis defined a use to resolve problems, such as transferring to other areas, escalating to the engineering staff, This framework was extended to include to and referring to other vendors. problems that were transferred 1 6 to the customer for Organizational Routines Additional moves, such as opening the call additional information or for testing. call, were added for this analysis, changes the status as shown one of a call to reflect development), their action in Figure 2. of these moves automatically recorded is unambiguous way. Thus, the codes could be read need little archival grammar INSERT FIGURE sample was divided (N = 168). two into = 167) and a inductively (N statistically in The calls). code from the sequence) Non-routine As mentioned Problem in test sample lack of that The evidence supporting summarized in call 96% of to test the grammar which coded one half of the unit-by-unit sequences agreement (326 calls. of 338 to minor unitizing errors off Hewes and by one position, resulting in Poole, 1984). Work this contention is tracking database based on an analysis of the training sample and from observational data. This evidence the following paragraphs. High Numbers of Exceptions input stream (or was used agreement was due that shifted the Solving "*) the introduction, this task unit's work would be difficult to characterize as (N = 167) taken from DBI's is a very clear, a training sample that was used to develop the parts, apparent disagreement on subsequent codes (Folger, routine. call to from the database records with very ABOUT HERE 2 Two coders were employed, each a subset of 50 (omitting a in DBI at example, escalating the the database directly After a brief training session, the coders achieved codes (for a support specialist for interpretation. (*** The in When and closing the . There are several indicators of the frequency of exceptions raw materials) to the DBI software support group. 1 7 One indicator is the in the Organizational Routines frequency of duplicate calls (different customers calling with the In many with duplicate topics its in software support have not previously been encountered, To assess this frequency, for five calls was unique. (calls evidence that the problem was call for Seventy three of the faci that solutions for new problems which a solution cannot be looked up or problems" that had not previously been encountered. by the the frequency of is we examined each recognized as one with a known solution. partly explained were only compatibility with DBI's software); every other topic Another indicator of task variety retrieved). the training sample, there In publish (IBM had recently released a new operating system and these customers had questions about that or similar problems). common and some vendors technical service hot lines, similar questions are newsletters of "commonly asked questions." same calls (43.7%) were "new The high frequency of new problems is "known" problems are distributed to customers on a regular basis via maintenance tapes. Thus, customers tend to call with new or exceptional problems. Even if a customer has a familiar kind of problem, there are frequently details about that customer's installation that make their specific problem exceptional. large data processing centers; they frequently have multiple networks, and databases containing dependent problems many millions of records. customers have a similar kind of problem, the of the large telecommunications There are a variety of system that result from unique combinations of processors, storage devices, networks, operating systems, and other software assessment CPUs, DBI's customers operate frequency of in use at the similarity new problems tends to customer can be Even when two site. difficult to detect. Our understate the variety introduced by these contingencies. Low Task Analvzabilitv rationalized . Although the world of computer software can sometimes be highly and well-understood, the task environment many problems difficult to analyze. First, the product explained: 18 at DBI had certain features that made was very complex, as this engineer Organizational Routines I can tell way on you that SystemTen enough earth to create the realm of human result, was it the scale cases for terms of complexity. There's no in There's no doubt that it. beyond Certain modules you just have to put a skull as a black box. (DBI, 34.6) treat even where was This to look. especially true of "data dependent" or timing which could usually only be reproduced on the customer difficult to it's not always possible to be sure of what caused a problem, what a feasible solution might be, or errors, test understanding. and crossbones on and As a is off when construct an explanation for a problem even Second, site. the it was sometimes symptoms were known, as this engineer explained: Even if you have the dump, see that it "footprints in the call tracking database. it was a "one symptoms was really have a footprint is Out of these 167 calls, forty the sand. You can in the training sample taken from DBI's seven (28.1%) were never solved. Either time occurrence" for which no meaningful diagnostic data could be some aspect not always possible to obtain of a problem, in happened. (DBI, 5.13) it sand" phenomenon can be detected collected or the customer upgraded Finally, you happened, but you have no idea why The the problem all much less to explore of their system and the problem went away. enough information from users its causes. In to isolate the these cases, there was no way to analyze what had happened. Extensive Deliberation and Search. Given the complexity of the problems being analyzed, should not be surprising that considerable deliberation and search was required. The it first author often observed technical support specialists working several hours on a single problem, sifting through "dumps" and "snaps" (foot-thick stacks of paper representations of the file buffers, filled with hexadecimal main memory, and other diagnostic information), or stepping through programs one instruction at a time. Other members of the support frequently consulted, and calls were frequently transferred 1 9 between staff functional areas or were Organizational Routines escalated to the developers to bear on the problems. who wrote the code so that the appropriate expertise could be brought Because so many problems were necessary to transfer responsibility for site-specific, a problem to the customer who more information could be gathered. Customers were sometimes asked to run In a test procedure and report the no sense could problem was to fix do about this In one should be issued new version. In was an additional phase some frequently reported so that it, to recreate a problem or When the source of a of deliberation involved in deciding particularly difficult case, several for the current version of the was example. work be described as mindless or automatic. identified, there it. results, for it meetings were required to decide product (as a "patch") or delayed cases, a customer could simply "work around" the problem. what until In if the the other cases, a problem could be fixed by supplying a customer with a customized "ZAP" that changed their copy of the program. to the underlying product. In still other cases, it might require more extensive design changes Choosing among these alternatives often required escalated to higher level technical or managerial personnel. navigational task, this work required the collaboration of solve Taken that a problem be Like Hutchins' (1991) many individuals; no individual could of the problems. all together, these indicators strongly suggest that this task unit's terms of definitions based on Perrow (1967), analyzability of search, at best. this unit They operated work was non-routine. In had high task variety and moderate with considerable deliberation and search and frequent interruptions, thus violating Ashforth and Fried's (1988) criteria for routinized behavior. routine, Given these findings, we might be tempted to speculate that no functionally similar pattern that characterized it this task unit work processes. had no This conclusion, however, would be based on an unwarranted inference from the properties of the work to the sequential structure of the actions required to accomplish develop a grammatical model and use it it. In the following section, we to test for repetitive, sequential patterns of action in the work. 20 Organizational Routines Constructing Grammatical the There are several steps required They are described and steps will method 1990; and be familiar, builds on a it The first in to the call. As be less familiar and will In this .,hich is one performance two different times (W in row (1) first step W), transferred were taken coded data can be read as respect, this (Gottman and Roy, creating a in to is become grammar in familiar with will in (C). In to as "strings" or was opened to the user (US), transferred worked on two more times (W W), and then closed The the process of responding be referred follows: the call Each row Figure 3. of the software support routine. (or actions) that the discussion that follows, the of these receive special attention here. twenty coded calls from the training sample are shown call, Some to allow replication. The more specialized steps involved each row represent moves In in sufficient detail with any inductive procedure, the "sequences." The sequence (PS), here Poole, 1990). to the data will represents a single symbols illustrated variety of familiar techniques for handling sequential data Exploring the data. the data. a grammatical model of an organizational process. to construct such as the basic coding procedure described above. Van de Ven and fitting Model back (O), worked on to product support general, the coding procedure reduces a great deal of other information down to these strings of symbols. For example, we record the sequence of events but not the interval between them (Abbott, 1991, refers to this as "event time" as opposed to "clock time") and a wide variety of work activities is compressed a single category (W). (*** Nonetheless, the variety constructed from the INSERT FIGURE 3 ABOUT HERE among same the performances in Figure 3 "*) is striking. They are all basic lexicon of moves, but the sequences vary considerably from 21 into . } Organizational Routines performance to performance. Of these 20 and pattern (lines 4, 8, 11, 12 the other calls entail unique support the conclusion that The goal was the of action, yet they responding to a customer problem. similar" objective of the data. 18) (none of these sequences this same only the five simplest calls had the calls, same problem, by is to All of accomplish the "functionally all seem Superficially, these data task unit has no routine; there of the next step in the analysis the way). seems summarize to be too much variance to this sequential in variance using a simple grammar. Create a Grammar that Describes the Pattern . towards a formulating a hypothetical grammar. As we explore the data, The grammar consist of a set of phrase will structure rules (sometimes called "rewrite rules," for reasons that moment) that specify patterns in the coded data. the usual sense of rules that organizational Members of the the kind in Formulating a grammar is top down, by starting with the whole routine Alternatively, one can work from the bottom common sub-sequences. A top-down of the routine is in members art. known in rules, Rather, phrase structure rules of observed it about the sequence of actions observed Whether one is in These These are like coded data and priori is identifying overall logic expectations based on theory. to priori account A theory and wants to for the largest number of rules form a kind of working hypothesis the routines. working top-down or bottom-up, parts of the routine. the coded data. most appropriate when the bottom-up strategy would be more appropriate when one has no a observations with the simplest set of rules. in into constituent parts. up, by starting with the case, the goal any more than they would begin to formulate a grammar from the and breaking strategy would be In either will follow or refer to while doing their work. One can advance, or when one has a work inductively from the data. become apparent Note that these rules need not be thought of for describing patterns something of an be working a a conventional regression model. used here are simply devices will in DBI technical support group do not follow these follow the rules implicit we it may be helpful to look for the constituent the "syntactic constituents" (Cook, 1988) that 22 make up a Organizational Routines These are sub-patterns formal grammar. can be meaningfully isolated from the that sequence. For example, each software support for the solution, followed by a closing. Software Support "-->" where the symbol This "followed by." is Call be rule could rules as describing into The next step its in procedures Solution, for and the commas in the list Closing. indicate Each side. rule In has a single symbol on the left-hand al., side, and English grammar, for example, one might have written how as follows: S --> NP, VP. One can think of these H ,irase structure a particular entity (such as a customer service interaction) can be constituent parts. In this respect, the is to enumerate the ways in which each constituent can method shares some common ground with ethnographic mapping means-ends semantic domains (Spradley, 1979). The semantic domain for here can be thought of as the "ways to accomplish" the constituent general, there model Search could be expressed as: a simplified version of how phrase structure grammars (Gazdar et developing a grammar be accomplished. of interest of" this says that a sentence (S) can consist of a noun phrase (NP) and a verb phrase (VP). rule that expanded grammatical form, can be read as "consists one or more symbols on the right-hand Such as consists of an opening, followed by a search call Opening, --> 1985) are used to represent sentences. a In rest of the this, may be several possible one simply includes alternative gets one rule. ways to Every usually several ways retail that question. alternative expansions of that constituent; each possible Theoretically, these alternatives are functionally similar satisfy a in the sense common means-ends transaction, for example, involves form of payment, but there are payment can be accomplished (e.g., cash, check, credit card, Spradley's (1979) terms, these alternatives comprise the domain, "ways of paying." terms, they would be included as separate rules forthcoming). In accomplish a given constituent of a routine. To suggested by Gersick & Hackman (1990) because they relationship. in in Each constituent must be expanded 23 a grammar in this etc.). In In our of retail transactions (Pentland, manner until it can be expressed in Organizational Routines terms of the basic lexicon or coding scheme. This is what allows the grammar to be tested against the data, as described below. was used This procedure process, as shown examining the sequences carefully observational data. As see how well sample was of in to The required. first O. with a single more If more it rule in Figure in detailed data detail to include the The body of a solution. each a calls are new ways call, this moves include any there are of judgment some is trivial: constituent could have initial opening been coded conversation with the caller, In that case, we would add be accomplished. that represent the process of searching for sequence of transferring, escalating, or working special constituents within the search process, that For example, in the second line of Figure 3 the that responsibility for the problem was turned over to and then taken back by product support (PS). This sequence happens frequently problems where the only way to advance the information or test something on their system. solution When completed, the specialist assumes responsibility again. patterns amount by a closing. Since the opening was the tracking database. that an opening could sequence "US PS" occurs. This means in each answered, the call into labeled "user" and "developer." the user (US) for consists of a sequence of In addition, to the training it 4 states that the overall structure of the pattern consists for the solution, followed The search process can on the problem. we have call insights gained from the the lexicon, the rule that expresses the opening way rules to express the alternative and applying were developed by grammar took shape, we applied were available to enter identified Like any inductive exercise, a certain the data. fit DBI software support for the we have the training sample the hypothetical symbol and the steps required particular rules in an opening, followed by a search coded --> The Figure 4. in a hypothetical grammar to induce is to have the customer gather more the information arrives or the test Figure 4 also includes a similar set of between the developer (DV) and product support (PS) constituents. 24 is that we have labeled "developer" Organizational Routines (*** Each INSERT FIGURE 4 ABOUT HERE ends with a closing sequence call indicating that the call is that involves a tentative fix, comment "done", followed by a trailing Observations of the work process and an examination of the call ***) followed by an action what was done. that explains data indicated that closing a call does not necessarily imply solving the problem. Many of the calls that are handled by reports of one-time problems that are never duplicated and never solved. Nonetheless, these calls are usually either alternative sequence is codes indicating that a involving a test of the comment Rewriting it that I fix rules, The "done" fix). (inactive). some In is very similar to what we would tentative fix internal structure contains one of three is represented by either C D (closed) or another symbol indicating an additional calls, there is call (E). grammar against to test the have an or T) followed by a work episode (usually constituent consists of taking each coded string and Rewriting R A explains what steps were taken to close the The procedure used . tried (F or Figure 4. includes an and developer episodes, level constituents that in grammar reason, the this Like the user fix. as shown has been For inactive. that omits the tentative expressed by further (deferred) or become and "done" are intermediate "tentative fix" that closed or DBI are literally rewriting the data it is called "rewriting", since using the hypothesized rules. One searches normally describe as coding. data for a specified pattern, but instead of simply annotating the data, one actually replaces the original data with the code (hence the term rewriting). with the "search one looks For simple rules, one could accomplish this and replace" option available for patterns in the found, that part of the data in any word processing program. data that match the right-hand side of a is rule. When replaced with the left-hand side of the rule (the way, detailed patterns constituent). In this constituents. This basic procedure is in the data are replaced by the repeated for 25 each of the rules in the In rewriting, a match name is of the more abstract grammar until no Organizational Routines further rewriting This can be accomplished manually, or possible. is it can be accomplished using a computer program designed for pattern matching and re-writing. 1 The Figure 5 illustrates the procedure for rewriting a single string, one rule at a time. row in Figure 5 shows the raw data, consisting of a sequence of seven moves. row shows the same data the path of after each data element as indicate that Thus, the rule "Opening --> O." The it an element has been simple sentence diagram. C." has been rewritten by a single it ability of rewriting to the rule "Closing --> Done, to a single were If grammar is perfectly of strings in a Comment" was used symbol grammar in sample fits is been be sequences in in the data that For the analysis that follows, I lei it Double lines would resemble a been rewritten using lines. "Done --> where the fourth row, two constituents with one, as In re-written the final row, three constituents that represents the overall pattern. to in this example, then use do not it fits the have been "parsed" by the grammar. This would be fits this measure by the grammar in is one measure the analysis that follows. fit we used the hypothetical grammar. a program written personal computer. 26 in like The number the model with an error term of zero. observed sequences can always be completely all and run on a Macintosh will vertical lines trace rewritten using the rule demonstrated to replace that are completely rewritten we inverted, Each successive each of the next three rows, two symbols were In a regression that the data; were the second row, note that the "O" has summarize patterns and can be said example, however, not 1 this figure completely rewritten to a single symbol, as having a data point the If symbol, as indicated by the converging unified into a single a string rewritten. in indicated by the converging lines. The transformed by the rewriting process. is the next row, the "C" has In rule. first of how well Unlike this rewritten. There may Since rewriting tends to MacPROLOG 4.0 (Quintus Corp.), Organizational Routines reduce the length of the string being rewritten, the percentage reduction indicator of the degree of (Olson et fit Our hypothesis Testing the grammar. is length another is forthcoming). al, INSERT FIGURE 5 ABOUT HERE (*** in "*) that in spite of frequent interruptions and extensive search, the support process embodied sequential patterns that could be represented by a grammar. A convenient hypothesis that allows us to formulate a null statistical test is that the observed sequences of action are random and do not follow the hypothesized pattern. To whether the service process rewriting procedure on second data were not included One hundred strings, in DBI conformed half of our to our hypothesized grammar, coded data, the the training sample that test was used most were rewritten to some grammar to we needed reduction the remaining in the length the kind described by Olson et We random permutations al of (forthcoming). each same length grammar to each of the Thus, each randomized dataset and composition (To visualize this procedure, imagine taking the sentences each sentence 100 times simple bootstrap procedure of generated 100 sets of random data by string in the real dataset. consisted of 168 strings, each with the it a set of random sequences. Since no probability we performed a the hypothesized much In to estimate the probability that distributions are available for this kind of data, in that these These ungrammatical cases are discussed below. could be obtained by applying the rearranging the words the to formulate the hypothetical model. extent, but not always with test the statistical significance of this result, creating we performed sample (N = 168). Note nine of the strings, or about 64.9%, were rewritten completely. of the string of symbols. To at test in this to create of original. paragraph and randomly 100 new paragraphs.) 100 randomized datasets; the 27 elements as the We mean number applied of strings * - - * - . Organizational Routines was 9.57 completely rewritten result The observed (out of 168), with a standard deviation of 2.77. from the real data (109 out of 168) was over 35 standard deviations higher than the mean. Analysis of unarammatical patterns. grammar might be answer better or this question, it is useful to analyze the residual patterns that incompletely rewritten calls) disparity in the test was sample (appearing never taken back by DBI's product support group. had implicitly whether some other do not in fit the hypothesized twenty four of the the presence of a single "US" without any corresponding This pattern would imply that responsibility for the specialist of course, more parsimonious than our hypothesized grammar. To help grammar. The most common "PS." The question remains, In was call nearly all transferred to the user and of these instances, taken responsibility by continuing to work on the other action, but had neglected to indicate this in the database. a DBI The existence of this pattern makes sense, because the high workload encourages support people to give problems take them back only necessary. if reduces the apparent fit of the of calls completely rewritten The remaining few seemed to officially Many people simply attended more fundamental departures from the grammar. In to require additional work. had been transferred to For these cases, problems that Thirteen of the calls had to the customer for further data which Without customer pressure on these problems, the support more pressing matters. These exceptional patterns are in the data had reflected this move, then the percentage closed (or deferred or declared inactive), although work had apparently apparently never materialized. ways If had been closed and then reopened. of these calls simple structure that to would exceed 79%. have been solved turned out never been ceased. calls away and This omission on the part of the DBI staff significantly grammar. calls reflected example, seven of the some call or taking theoretically important we have hypothesized is because they indicate not always attained which the hypothesized grammar might be improved. 28 in that practice. One might want even the They also suggest to add a rule, for Organizational Routines example, that allows support. One user episodes that do not include returning responsibility for based on to over 90% and then re-opened. These of the test sample, but they insights from the fieldwork, of Furthermore, there keep adding rules which to exclude is until every possibility essentially a is we do rules would improve the coverage is judgment model; overfitting the covered. call. of the would also contradict our normative sense, what constitutes a complete, competent always the danger of against a null hypothesis, different product might also want to add a rule that allows patterns that have no closing constituent, or patterns that are closed grammar to The decision Although not yet have tests to we can compare in principle, call. one can simply of which rules to include test and a hypothetical grammar the relative performance of grammars (Simon, 1992). DISCUSSION This grammatical analysis reveals a great deal of regularity superficially to shown in captured be quite unstructured. figure 3) in appear to The observed it Even though the software support performances (as We embodies a regularity arises from the structural features of the situation transferred between in way specialists, information on. the work must be If in routines are often associated with specialized repetitive is organized These assets define a is (e. g., division of labor), of the participants. solicited Calls must be from the user, calls must be our dataset allowed us to code the process at a would emerge. which routines are embedded specific assets." can be repetitive, functionally similar pattern of action. finer level of detail, additional regularities way that are forced to conclude that although this work and the cognitive models opened before they can be closed, and so of the data that would appear be quite diverse, they share an underlying structure the rules of a grammar. apparently non-routine, in It organizations. human could hardly be otherwise, because As Winter (1986: 168) or physical capital that he calls "routine- set of possible actions which give rise, patterns. 29 notes, in practice, to Organizational Routines In the DBI software support group, for example, specialists have a limited lexicon of At any point with which to accomplish their work. how to proceed with solving a problem, as reflected creativity and diligence applied toward next are quite limited. example, once a Further, there in the lexicon. this goal, but the a user, it variety; to be identical. What we these patterns They present. will emerge whether or not arise ' jm tue way affordances of the situation. the work of among some of these moves. For fall far short of constraining all can best be described as constrained find, instead, performances that are functionally There may be a great deal must be transferred back. These dependencies give additional structure to the performances, but they performances few choices about generic choices about what can be done a logical dependence is call is transferred to time, there are only a in moves similar, but not necessarily the same. Finally, the preconditions for purely scripted behavior are is organized, as reflected This interpretation in the constraints quite consistent with Nelson is and and Winter's (1982) ideas about routine-specific assets, routines as truce, and other contextual features that enable and constrain they allow the agents in action. Cognitive structures are also important, of course, insofar as the situation to employ the appropriate assets, observe the truce, and thereby produce the desired pattern of action. Conceptualizing routines grammatically draws attention to the relationship between an organization's structure and the sequences of action that occur there. structure defines the set of possibilities, but not the particular becomes action. natural to ask For example, might a developers To even begin make up to How in new way alter the pattern of patterns of work? that how changes work? In this sequences we observe. structural features might alter the of framework, Thus, observed pattern it of communicating and sharing information with users and How would a persistent are routines in different division of labor affect the the face of changing institutional structures? address such questions, one must be able to summarize the patterns of action the hypothesized routines. 30 Organizational Routines Grammatical models have for this purpose. distinct advantages over other kinds of models (e.g., Markov models) they describe a set of possible patterns of action from which First, This formulation has theoretical value, because enact particular patterns. it accommodates apparently conflicting perspectives on routines as fixed patterns and routines as accomplishments. It has methodological value, as well, because members the effortful real routines are unlikely to Rather, they display a variety of functionally similar patterns that display "fixed" patterns. can be concisely summarized using the kind of formalisms described here. Second, phrase way structure rules provide a convenient routines") that expanded into to describe the layers of constituents ("sub- can be rearranged and combined to form new Each constituent can be patterns. a family of functionally similar patterns that represent alternatives ways of accomplishing that part of the routine. Third, as illustrated in the case of the user and developer episodes, grammatical models are well suited to representing connections between events that way may be widely separated in a sequence. to distinguish normatively correct instances of illustrated in the tendency of the DBI support Fourth, grammatical models provide a clear a routine from other instances, as staff to avoid claiming responsibility for certain calls. Last but not least, the use of a grammatical model allows us to operationalize the definition of an organizational routine as a set of repetitive, functionally similar patterns of action with a degree is of precision that a "pattern of action" grammar has not been possible before. if it In particular, a sequence of coded symbols can be parsed by a grammar. The patterns of action within the are established as "functionally similar" by their inclusion within a ends semantic domain (Spradley, 1979). That particular "end." instances of it And within to interpretation finally, some is, a pattern of action each pattern is "repetitive" in if the grammar one can time period of practical or theoretical interest. and refinement in common meansis a "means" collect multiple While subject still oarticular settings, these operationalizations help to the concept of organizational routines more explicit counts as a routine and what does not. 31 by defining empirical to a criteria for make what Organizational Routines A New Comparative Framework If we carry the analogy to language forward, comparative framework similarities between patterns. in many and so on. A the there are grammatical may have the is of activity into concise, high-level allows linguists to identify families of that share certain syntactic characteristics recruiting, a grammatical approach of The different. such as sales, purchasing, advantage different. structural each organization may be quite in potentially powerful same way grammar kind of words are completely organizations summarize innumerable diverse patterns In new human language, true of other kinds of processes, as well, auditing, planning, ability to In different languages, although the actual although the specific actions taken same may be suggests the possibility of for organizational analysis. By analogy, the customer service process similarities, it human languages (Newmeyer, 1983), a grammar of routines should allow organizational scholars to identify families of organizational processes and to chart their changes over time. The grammatical framework becomes profusion of lexical items, such as nouns. grammar As a facilitates when especially valuable By creating categories there is a of functional equivalence, comparisons across sets of apparently diverse instances. practical matter, grammatical models seem most appropriate sequential records are kept or can be easily generated. especially for processes that are distributed in for situations Observational data time and space. is Fortunately, where expensive, many kinds of business processes generate archival data as a byproduct of normal operations which could be analyzed using these methods. Bill collectors, for could potentially be useful (Sutton, 1991). all example, keep records of With the increased use of information systems kinds of service and manufacturing organizations, the necessary data more and more Assuming their activities that is likely to in become available. that data are available, the success of this approach formulate an appropriate lexicon and syntactic constituents. abstraction are analogous to the constructs one finds 32 in will depend on our These intermediate traditional ability to levels of nomological research, except Organizational Routines that instead of referring to the variable properties of objects, they refer to actions or By introducing abstract constituents collections of actions. "closing") to represent parts of a routine, compared in terms of their constituents. it allows routines that differ In linguistics, it is in In is the existence of syntactic all subsequent the arrangement of these constituents that characterizes the much more so structure of a language, surface structure. It than the details of the vocabulary or other aspects of the case of software support, the simple three-part structure for calls (opening, search for solution, closing) suggests the possibility of comparing the different organizations be their details to constituents such as noun phrases and verb phrases that forms the basis for theorizing (Cook, 1988). and "opening", "search" (e.g., ways accomplish openings, searches, or closings, independent of that how the other parts of the overall .out me are accomplished. Assessing Models Alternative Although organizational routines might be expected to have a great deal of regularity, there always the possibility of exception, producing contingencies. fit an hypothesized model. It breakdown, seems unreasonable It creativity, shirking, error, to expect that would be highly desirable to all and other variety- instances of a routine would have a systematic way of ascertaining whether an hypothesized model could be improved upon by adding or deleting rules. (1992) notes that the best available test for the adequacy of rule based models heuristic that the is model should explain many more cases than the number is Simon the general of rules. Using this rule of thumb, our hypothesized model fares reasonably well by explaining 109 cases (plus a similar number in the training sample) with only 13 rules. between the complexity currently have no of the grammar and statistical test for the number of There cases it is clearly a trade-off here can describe, but we assessing the relative contribution of individual rules. 33 t Organizational Routines Some Assumptions Issues and The key assumption of this is that organizational This assumption syntactic constituents. survey responses mean and aggregated. While is method same the is no thing to different respondents The advantage best employ routine strings of summary measures approach is is in we observe us from the object to distance that unlike traditional linear models, assumes that it is meaningful are always just the tip of to isolate the which at reduced to synchronic structure of routines As Fabian (1990) they are created by diachronic in it. We would argue, however, order to achieve a degree of moment in time. Finally, there is always the that in of existing at any moves), but the source of these to the model. risk that way Rules are resources in a determine action (Zimmerman, 1970; Giddens, 1984). can embody physical or grammar describing patterns of social action with understanding of the nature of the social world. such a we observe This framework can be used to describe lexical evolution (new life of helpful to bracket off the of action misinterpreted as an attempt to objectify social An example repetition of which they occur just as is it about the patterns clarity moves) and syntactic evolution (new patterns changes would be exogenous notes, the performances that an iceberg that consists of the rehearsal and these patterns over time; these performances create the context rules it any methodology that involves a that the experiences of the participants are from their diachronic context (Barley, 1990). particular Blumer, 1968), coded symbols. This method also much as than the assumption that of the overall routine, the essential sequential structure of the The disadvantage preserved. is of this meaningful and can therefore be compared phenomenon, the representation tends representation of a complex into criticized (Cicourel, 1964; more commonly treated as completely unproblematic. As of our inquiry. can be coded different, in principle, assumption has been this life logical constraints rule (from Salancik & on the is for action, is but they do not a sense possibilities for action in is that be inconsistent with our best Nonetheless, there Leblebici, 1988), 34 that will in which a situation. one cannot eat a meal Organizational Routines before it has been prepared. Note that has been prepared, deterministic, but it it this rule does not force one to eat a meal just simply rules out the possibility of reversing this sequence. does constrain the set methodology makes no attempt to of possibilities. In is not is It it recognition of these issues, our uncover any rules that people rules to describe patterns of action, this because Although "really use." we use merely a convenient way of summarizing sequential patterns of action that allows comparison and generalization across settings. For this reason, the models presented here are rather different symbolic or computational models that have been used theory. Unlike in processes when confronted that, reproduces a particular pattern of behavior. an ant carrying a crumb, for example, is members and organization a reductionist model of that the circuitous path of While people have more extensive cognitive do work in constrained environments that Grammatical models of organizational routines provide a convenient way to summarize those possibilities and render them available analysis, but they leave the this respect, enactment of particular patterns open to unfold for inspection anew each and time. In grammatical models capture the essence of the phenomenon, as eloquently expressed by one of the anonymous road. of the with a set of situational constraints, Simon (1969) argues of organizations limit their possibilities for action. to create many the product of the ant's simple rules for navigation applied to the constraints of the environment. capabilities than ants, than cognitive science Simon (1969, 1992), we are not attempting individual cognitive in spirit ASQ reviewers: "Routines are like ruts They do not exactly determine where the next wagon describe where past will in go, but neither a well-traveled do they merely wagons have gone." CONCLUSION Imagine a community of scholars color at the center of some yellow, green or blue and no way for of their whom color is a critical concern. These scholars place most important theories, yet they have no words of distinguishing 35 among them. for red, Organizational scholars are not Organizational Routines quite this weak, bad at best. off, of course, but our tools for describing Our theories refer to routines and generalizing about routines are as though they were concrete objects, yet no systematic means of describing these objects. By providing a way structure of routines, grammatical organization theory in the patterns of action ways we that call models create the to represent the sequential possibility of investigating were not previously possible. It 36 we core issues in remains to be seen whether routines merit the theoretical importance without a methodological lens to help us see these patterns, we have we have given them, but might never know. Organizational Routines REFERENCES Andrew Abbott, 1991 "A primer on sequence methods." Organization Science, Alker, Hayward 375-392. R., Jr. 1987 "Fairy Tales, Tragedies, and World Possibilist Ashforth, Blake E. Histories: Towards Interpretive Story Grammars as World Models." Behaviormetrika, 21: 1-28. and Yitzhak Fried 1988 "The Mindlessness Barley, 1: of Organizational Behaviors." Human Relations, 41: 305-329. Stephen R. 1990 "Images of Imaging: Notes on Doing Longitudinal Fieldwork." Organization Science, 1: 220-247. Blumer, Herbert 1969 Symbolic Interactionism: Perspective and Methods, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Chomsky, Noam 1956 "Three models IT-2: Cicouiel, for the description of language." IRE Transactions on Information Theory, 113-124. Aaron 1964 Method and Measurement in Sociology, New York: Free Press. Cohen, Michael D. 1991 "Individual Learning and Organizational Routine: Emerging Connections." Organization Science, 2: 135-139. Colby, Benjamin N., Sayuri Kennedy and Louis Milanesi 1991 "Content Analysis, Cultural Grammars, and Computers." Qualitative Sociology, 14: 373- 387. Cook, V. J. 1988 Chomsky's Universal Grammar, Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell. 37 Organizational Routines Coulmas, Florian 1981 Conversational Routine: Explorations in Standardized Communication Situations and Prepatterned Speech, The Hague: Mouton. Daft, Richard L. and Norman B. Macintosh 1981 "A Tentative Exploration into the Amount and Equivocality of Information Processing Work Organizational Units." Administrative in Science Quarterly, 26: 207-224. Fabian, Johannes 1990 Power and Performance: Ethnographic Explorations through Proverbial Wisdom and Theatre Folger, Joseph in P., Shaba, Zaire, Madison, Wl: University Dean 1984 "Coding Social E. Hewes and Interaction." In of Wisconsin Press. Marshall Scott Poole Brenda Dervin and Melvin Communication Science, Volume IV: J. Voight, (eds.) Progress in 115-161. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Garfinkel, Harold 1967 Studies Ethnomethodology, Englewood in Ewan Gazdar, Gerald, Klein, Geoffrey Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Pullum and Ivan Sag 1985 Generalized Phrase Structure Grammar, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Gersick, Connie J. and J. 1990 "Habitual Routines Richard in Hackman Task-Performing Groups." Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 47: 65-97. Giddens, Anthony 1984 The Constitution of Society, Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Gioia, D. 1984 and P. "Scripts Poole in Organizational Behavior," Academy Management Review, 9, 449-459. Goffman, Erving 1981 Forms of Talk, Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Gonzalez, Rafael C. and Michael G. Thomason 1978 Syntactic Pattern Recognition: An Introduction, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. 38 Organizational Routines Gottman, John M. and Anup K. Roy 1990 Sequential Analysis: T. 1983 "Structural for Behavioral Researchers, Cambridge: Cambridge Press. University Hannan, Michael A Guide and John R. Freeman and organizational change." American Sociological Review, 29: 149- inertia 164. Heritage, John 1984 Garfinkel and Ethnomethodology, Cambridge, England: Polity Press. Hutchins, Edwin 1991 "Organizing Work by Adaptation." Organization Science, 2: 14-39. Leidner, Robin 1993 Fast Food, Fast Talk: Service Work and the Routinization of Everyday Life, Berkeley: University of California Press. Leigh, Thomas W. and Patrick F. McGraw 1989 "Mapping the procedural knowledge of industrial sales personnel: investigation." Journal of Marketing, Levitt, 53: A script-theoretic 16-34. Barbara and James G. March 1988 "Organizational Learning." Annual Review of Sociology: 319-340. Lynch, Beverly P. 1974 "An Empirical Assessment of Perrow's Technology Construct." Administrative Science Quarterly, March, James G and Herbert A. Simon 1958 Organizations, Miclet, 338-356. 19: New York: John Wiley and Sons. Laurent 1986 Structural Methods in Pattern Recognition, New York: Springer- Verlag. Nelson, Sidney G. and Richard R. Winter 1982 An Evolutionary Theory Newmeyer, Frederick of Economic Change, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. J. 39 Organizational Routines 1983 Grammatical Theory: Its Limits and Its Possibilities, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Olson, Gary M., James D. Herbsleb and Henry H. Rueter Forthcoming "Characterizing the Sequential Structure of Interactive Behaviors through Statistical and Grammatical Techniques." Human Computer Interaction. Pentland, Brian T. 1992 "Organizing Moves 37: in Software Support Hot Lines." Administrative Science Quarterly, 527-548. Pentland, Brian T. Forthcoming "Grammatical Models of Organizational Processes." Organization Science. Perrow, Charles 1967 "A Framework for the Review, 32: Comparative Analysis of Organizations." American Sociological 194-208. Prince, Gerald 1973 A Grammar of Stories, The Hague: Mouton. Ryan, Marie-Laure 1979 "Linguistic models Semiotica, 28: in Narratology: From structuralism to Generative Semantics." 127-155. and Husseyin Leblebici Salancik, Gerald R. 1988 "Variety and Form in Organizing Transactions: Nancy DiTomaso and Samuel A Generative B. Bacharach, (eds.) Research Grammar in of Organization." In the Sociology of Organizations: 1-32. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Sandelands, Lloyd E. 1987 "Task Grammar and Attitude." Motivation and Emotion, 11: 121-143. Schank, Roger C. and Robert P. Abelson 1977 Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding: an Inquiry Hillsdale, NJ: Schegloff, Emanuel Lawrence Erlbaum. A. 40 into Human Knowledge Structures, Organizational Routines 1982 "Discourse as an come between interactional achievement: sentences." on Languages and In Some uses Deborah Tannen, (eds.) of 'uh-huh' and other things that Georgetown University Round Table Linguistics 1981: 71-93. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Simon, Herbert A. New 1957 Administrative Behavior, York: Macmillan. Simon, Herbert A. 1969 The sciences of the artificial, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Simon, Herbert A. 1992 "What Spradley, is an "explanation" James of behavior?" Psychological Science, 3: P. 1979 The Ethnographic Interview, Sutton, Robert 150-161. New York: Holt, Rinehart, Winston. I. 1991 "Maintaining norms about expressed emotions - The case of bill collectors." Administrative Science Quarterly, 36: 245-268. Van de Ven, Andrew H. and Marshall 1990 "Methods for Scott Poole Studying Innovation Development Program." Organization Science, 1: in the Minnesota Innovations Research 313-335. Ventola, Eija 1987 The Structure of Social Interaction: A Systematic Approach to the Semiotics of Service Encounters, London: Frances Pinter. Walsh, James P. and Gerardo R. Ungson 1991 "Organizational Memory." Academy of Management Review,16: 57-91. Weick, Karl E. 1979 The Social Psychology of Organizing, Second Edition, New York: Random House. Weiss, Howard M. and Daniel R. Ilgen 1985 "Routinized behavior in organizations." Journal of Behavioral Economics, 14: Winter, Sidney G. 41 57-67. Organizational Routines 1986 "The research program of the behavioral theory of the firm: evolutionary perspective." In B. Gilad and S. Kaish, (eds.) Orthodox Handbook critique and of Behavioral Economics, Volume A: Behavioral Microeconomics: 151-188. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Withey, Michael, Richard 1983 "Measures Scale." L. Daft of Perrow's Academy of and William H. Cooper Work Unit Technology: Management An Empirical Assessment and a New Journal, 26: 45-63. Zimmerman, Donald H. 1970 "The Practicalities of Rule Use." Toward the Reconstruction In Jack Douglas, of Sociological 42 (ed.) Understanding Everyday Life: Knowledge: 221-238. Chicago: Aldine. Organizational Routines Figure 1: Analogy Behavioral between behaviorlal Routine: the set of possible performances for a particular task, described by a grammar. Example: customer service grammatical Grammatical Entity Organizational and in part entitles Counterpart Organizational Routines Figure 2: Coding scheme Code for software support process Organizational Routines Figure 3: 1 Sequence of moves in subset of the call sample Organizational Routines Figure A grammar 4: Grammatical CALL for DBI's software Rule A whole closing. search call, consisting of a opening, search closing. and for solution --> opening process Explanation opening, search for solution, --> support Opening move O. for solution--> any order( work, transfer, user, developer Search of a call. for solution, consisting of any sequence and of working, transferring, as well as user- ). developer- episodes. closing --> tentative done, comment, fix, Final stage of a call, closing --> done, fix --> (F or done (C or --> comment work comment. tentative --> (W, --> I or (E or W, )• which may include a one of several symbols indicating that work is done, and a trailing comment that explains what was done. tentative R or D). blank). T ), fix, work. Mote that there are two kinds of closings (with "tentative fix", and that each constituent in the closing (tentative fix, done, and comment) has its own rule. and without a An episode of work on the call, consisting of a any length. string of "w"s of user user --> --> US, PS. Change US, work, PS. and back to product support, with an optional work episode intervening. responsibility for a call to the user developer -> DV, PS. Escalate a call to a development and then back to product support, with an optional work developer DV, work, PS episode intervening. --> 46 MIT LIBRARIES Organizational Routines 3 Figure Rewrite 5: Rewriting Rule a Applied sample call TOAD ODflEMQfiM S ?!76 Date Due fi£C S Lib-26-67