•*&* m LIBRARY OF THE MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY G PAPER SLOAN SCHOOL OF MANAGEMENT ._.l?l ,FRED P. RESEARCH PROGRAM ON THE ORGANIZATION AND MANAGEMENT OF RESEARCH AND DEVELOPMENT Conflict, and Performance in R & D Organizations: Some Preliminary Findings William Sept. 1965 M. Evan ^ • ^ ^ ..; 1 ^#l%6-^5 RESEARCH PROGRAM ON THE ORGANIZATION AND MANAGEMENT OF RESEARCH AND EEVELOPMENT Conflict and Performance in R & D Organizationsj_ --i' Some Preliminary F indings [ l.n William Sept. 1965 M. Evan " IC.rJ, ''^•; -^___.li.#l^^ Presented at the Second Conference on Research Program Effectiveness Sponsored by the Office of Naval Research Washington, D.C. July 28, 1965 The research described in this paper was supported by Grant No. NsG 235 of the National Aeronautics and Space Administration for research studies on the organization and management of R&D. Revised Sept. I965 CONFLICT AND PERFORMANCE IN R Se D ORGANIZATIONS: SOME PRELIMINARY FDTOINGS* William M. Evan Sloan School of Management Maseachusetts Institute of Technology With relatively few exceptions, social scientists studying organizational phenomena have not focused their attention on conflicts. Some notable exceptions are the studies of Robert Kahn and his colleagues on role conflicts on managerial conflicts and sociometric structure. 2 Clearly, this paucity of research on conflict cannot be due to the scarcity of the phenomena. ication, whether engaged in manufacturing, matter, knows this is not trxie. R&D, and Harrison White Any denizen of any organ- or any other function for that Hence, why the relative inattention to this facet of organizational life? This is indeed a difficult question in the psychology and sociology of research. Suffice it to say that some problems are less complex than others, some are more significant from a theoretical or a practical viewpoint, some are more fashionable or more readily fundable. It may be that conflicts are so omnipresent in organiza- tions, as well as in the world as a whole, that they tend to be taken for granted. In addition, they are difficult to investigate because the parties involved are often not willing to discuss them openly with social scientists. This may be particularly true where conflicts are triggered off by the exercise of power which give rise to a sense of injustice. 3 The purpose of this study is to investigate some causes and consequences of several types of conflicts in two in industry. R&D organizations, one in government and one 2 - THE PROBLEM An underlying assumption of this study Is that conflicts are endemic in organizations because of a lack of consensus as to the expectations and prescriptions for various organizational positions^ or a lack of uniform commitment to organizational objectives. A second assumption of this study is that some types of conflicts are detrimental and some are beneficial from the point of view of both organizational and individual goals. Thirdly, the principle of minimizing conflict, subscribed to by some managers and social scientists, may have some validity for crisis organizations, such as armies, or for so-called routine organizations, such as some manufac- turing organizations, but not for knowledge- and technology-producing organizations, such as those engaged in research and development. In R & D organizations, k as in other types of organizations, various points in the structure: conflicts arise at at top managerial levels in the course of policy making, between various levels of management, and between various departments at a given level in the hierarchy which require "lateral relationships' whose departments are interdependent in function. among managers Another and perhaps distinctive locus of conflict in R & D organizations is the project team. In the conduct of R & D, whether in a university, in an industrial organization, or in a government laboratory, there is a growing trend toward teamwork. This trend has been encouraged by the emergence of inter-disciplinary areas, the growing size of research projects, the increasing cost of equipment, and the increasing complexity of problems which require a systems approach and the complementary skills of several disciplines. This secular shift away from the academic model of the individual scholar to a team effort in research has been documented in several studies, including a recent report by Hagstrom on the incidence of teamwork among the faculty of various disciplines at the University of California, where it ranges from 29^ in mathematics and statistics to 97^ in physics.' Whenever such teamwork occurs, there is a huilt-in opportunity for a variety of conflicts to erupt. The focus of this study is on the project-team level in the organizational structure in which two or more individuals of the same or different specialties collaborate in the solution of a problem. The major problem to be investigated is: What kinds of conflicts at the project-group level are beneficial for the performance of R & D, what kinds are detrimental, and why? A TYPOLOGY OF PROJECT GROUP CONFLICTS The theoretical framework for this research was suggested by some of the concepts developed by Parsons and Bales in analyzing social systems. or social systems, to survive. All groups, organizations, in general, must solve internal and external problems if they are Every social system is expected or seeks to perform some function in relation to its environment. Internally, if the system is to be viable, satisfy the social and psychological needs of its members. Thus, it must one of the dimensions of conflict has to do with the external environment of the project group as distinct from a conflict having to do with social-emotional relations, namely, with inter- personal relations among the project group members. Problems pertaining to the environmental demands of the social system may involve technical or task conflicts; on the other hand, problems concerned with social-emotional factors may involve interpersonal conflicts. Another dimension of social system conflicts deals with the status of the parties involved. Do the conflicts arise among peers or do they arise among one or more of the subordinates and the supervisor of the project group? Since some research suggests that obstacles to communication are fewer among status equals Q than status unequals, we would expect that conflicts arising between subordinates and project group supervisors would have different effects on performance than those conflicts occurring among status equals within the project. . - k - The cross -classification of the two dimensions of conflict, technical versus interpersonal conflict, on the one hand, and peer conflict versus supervisorsubordinate conflict, on the other, yields four types of conflicts (Figure l). In other words, in the performance of project group research, at least four types of Insert Figure 1 about here conflicts may arise: (a) task conflict among peers, i.e., disagreement among pro- ject members as to what the objectives of the project shall be and how to achive them; (b) task conflict between one or more project members and the project super- visor; (c) interpersonal conflict among peers, for example, personal dislikes or mutual distrust among project members; and (d) interpersonal conflict between one We are not, of course, suggest- or more project members and the project supervisor. ing that this typology of project group conflicts exhausts all those that in fact occur. A variety of administrative conflicts, for example, do not clearly fall in any of these four types. Are all four types of conflict dysfunctional for the attainment of the objectives of a project group? a Our principal hypothesis is that technical conflicts have positive effect on performance whereas interpersonal conflicts have a negative effect on the performance of R fis D project teams. In the work of project teams, in the history of science and technology as a whole, versy would arise over the objectives of a as it was anticipated that contro- project as well as the means employed to reach the objectives and, ceteris paribus, that it would have a beneficial effect on the performance of the group. Several decades ago, Edwin Boring, in his presidential address to the American Psychological Association, observed that: "The history of science, like Hegel's view of the history of tho>i,:;ht, is one long series of theses, set off by ardently advocated antitheses, with ultimate syntheses terminating controversy and marking a step forward. This picture, it seems to me, holds not only for speculative philosophical psychology but also for the most rigorously observational work. Controversy has always been part of the method of science. "H Controversy in project team work, or technical conflict in our terminology, can potentially be adjudicated by rational means, and hence could conceivably open the way to alternative perspectives on the part of project group members. Interpersonal conflict, because it does not lend itself as readily to rational management, prove disruptive and interfere with the performance of the project group. it was predicted that technical conflict, may In short, whether among project group peers or between members of the project and the supervisor would be positively correlated with project group performance. tions, particularly in R & D, In view of the fact that performance in organiza- is a complex, multi-determined phenomenon, any one variable is apt to account for only a small amount of the variance. Accordingly, several classes of variables, psychological, social-psychological, and sociological, were built into the research instrument of this study in the expectation that the correlations between types of conflict and performance would be mediated in varying degrees by these variables. 12 DESIGN OF THE STUDY To test the principal hypothesis of the study, the administration of a questionnaire in two and an industrial laboratory. R&D a survey was designed involving organizations, a governmental The governmental laboratory is engaged primarly in basis and applied research in aerodynamics; the industrial laboratory is predomin- antly concerned with development work in the field of electronics. tion, In each organiza- we ascertained the pattern of collaboration among scientists and engineers on official project teams. 13 The identification of the memberships of various project teams proved to be a surprisingly difficult problem to solve. Once this identification was made vith the help of a special questionnaire addressed to the supervisors in the two laboratories^ all project group members were asked to complete a questionnaire which included a variety of questions pertaining not only to types of con- flict but also to hypothetical causes of conflict and several measures of performance. In selecting the project groups from both organizations, we stipulated that the project group should number at least 2 individuals, should have a duration of at least 6 months, and should involve some degree of task interdependence. These stipulations were deemed necessary if we were to observe any conflicts among the group members. The nature of the data, as is already apparent, is cross-sectiorial in character which means that interpretations of the direction of causality are especially diffiAlthough on theoretical grounds we are predicting that some types of con- cult. flict have positive "effects" and others have negative "effects" on performance, what we observe, of course, are only concomitant variations. one hand, and laboratory and field experiments, deficiencies. on the other, A panel study, on the could overcome these Another characteristic of the data of this study is that it involves reports of behavior and attitudes by means of a self-administred questionnaire. This has the advantage of encouraging anonymity and confidentiality and the disad- vantage of affording an opportunity for some respondents to distort their responses at will. Obtaining an accurate measure of response bias is still a challenging problem in survey research. The questions pertaining to our four types of conflict were of nature. Each project group member was asked to rate on a scale of 1 a sociometric to 7 how much he usually disagreed on technical matters with each of his fellow project group members. Similarly, he was asked to rate on a scale of 1 to 7 how much he usually disliked or had difficulty getting along with each of his fellow project group members and with his supervisor. The intricacies of measuring conflict and • - 7 - performance suggested the advisability of obtaining comparable data from three parties: all members of the project groups, first-level supervisors of the project groups, and second-level supervisors. Thus, in the government laboratory we obtained responses from 82 scientists and engineers associated with 27 projects and from 27 first-level and 27 second-level supei-visors. In the industrial laboratory, our survey included 159 scientists end engineers who were associated with 37 project 30 first-level supei^isors, and 30 second-level supervisors (Figxire 2). groups, Insert Figure 2 about here. The total number of respondents in the survey was 355 RESULTS The measurement of performance in R & D organizations still poses a challenge to both administrators and social scientists. were obtained, subjective and objective. In this study, two kinds of meas\ires The subjective measure consisted of a set of rating scales on how productive the project groups are as seen by the group' members themselves, by the first-level supervisors, and by the second-level superA similar subjective measure was obtained of project group creativity. visors. Although subjective rating scales are often not very reliable, the two objective measxires we obtained, free. number of articles and number of patents, are also not trouble- The number of patents received by project-group members is in part a func- tion of the kinds of tasks they are engaged in and the organizational policies in regard to patenting inventive ideas. by project group members policies is affected Similarly, the number of articles published by the type of task and the organizational concerning the public sharing of information. Another objective measure of performance which we sought to obtain was the ratio of budgeted costs to actual costs. Our efforts, however, were in vain because the unit of the accounting systems in these organizations was not the project group but an administrative unit equivalent . 8 to - department or a large-scale project cross-cutting various departments. a The analysis of the data thus far permits an examination of the relationship between types of conflict and productivity. Since the productivity ratings, as is clear from Table significantly intercorrelated in both 1, are, with one exception, Insert Table 1 about here. laboratories, they were averaged, of the findings, in the Interest of simplifying the presentation into a composite productivity rating. each project group, This composite rating, for consists of an average of the ratings of project group members on how productive their groups are, and of the ratings of first-level and second- level supervisors on the productivity of their project group. Table 1, As in the case of the unit of analysis throughout this report is the project group, not the « individual engineer or scientist. The project group means on the four types of conflict and on the composite productivity rating are shown in Table 2. The group means on the four types of Insert Table 2 about here conflict appear to be higher in the industrial than in the governmental laboratory, and roughly equal on the composite productivity rating. is the fact Especially noteworthy that the means for the two types of technical conflict are approximately twice as high as the means for the two types of interpersonal conflict. This find- ing may mirror the actual relative frequency of these types of conflict or else the relative difficulty that project group members have in answering questions about interpersonal conflict as compared with technical conflict. The zero-order correlation between types of conflict and performance are presented in Table 3. Since we are dealing with ordinal data, the Kendall rank order Insert Table 3 about here. " , - 9 - correlation was used rather than the Pearson product -moment correlation. The hypothesis that Interpersonal conflict is negatively correlated with productivity is partially supported by the results in Table 3. Of the four Kendall Taus in the two laboratories involving the relationship between interpersonal conflict and productivity^ three are negative and one reaches the .01 level of significance. The corollary hypothesis that technical conflict would be positively correlated with performance is not supported, at least not for the measure of composite rating of productivity positive, Tfrfiich we have thus far analyzed: one is negative, two of the four Kendall Taus are end one is zero. The noticeably higher correlations among the government project groups than among the industrial project groups may be a function of the difference in organizational climate, of the nature of the missions of the two organizations, the terms of employment of the engineers and scientists. or of These differences may be somewhat clarified by exploring the zero-order correlations by means of partial association. Among the several intervening variables we have so far examined to ascertaih whether they affect the zero-order correlation between conflict and productivity, two seem to be enlightening. In Table k, we have partialled out the effect of project group loyalty at Insert Table k about here two levels of this variable, above and below the median, and correlated conflict with productivity. The scores on project group loyalty are derived from a seven- point rating scale q^uestion on "How important is your association with your project group? Among the project groups in the government laboratory, the original significant correlation (i.e., the zero-order correlation) of -.38 between interpersonal conflict among peers and productivity is markedly affected by the degree of project group loyalty. In groups with a high degree of project group loyalty, the original - 10 - negative correlation is enhanced, but it is diminished when project group loyalty is low. Also noteworthy is the increased negative correlation from .10 to .50 between interpersonal conflict with supervisors and productivity under of low degree of project group loyalty. a condition The negative effect of interpersonal con- flict on performance is especially pronounced when there is an absence of social support from project group members in the form of loyalty as well as a strained relation with the supervisor. The original positive correlation of .ik between technical conflict among peers and productivity among the groups in the government laboratory is also noticeably affected by degree of project group loyalty. Under a condition of low degree of project group loyalty, the correlation becomes .25. And even the vir- tually zero correlation of technical conflict with supervisors and productivity increases to .12. Also suggestive is the difference in magnitude of the correla- tions between low degree and high degree of project group loyalty. Under a condi- tion of high degree of project group loyalty, there is virtually no correlation between technical conflict and productivity. Evidently, for technical controversy to develop and engender higher productivity, group cohesion may well be an impediment. Thus we observe a slight tendency for technical conflict to be positively associated with productivity, as predicted, only under a condition of low project group loyalty. Among the industrial project groups, the mediating effect of project group loyalty on the relationship between conflict and productivity is not at all evident. Notwithstanding the higher incidence of various conflicts in the industrial laboratory which we observed in Table 2, we did not find any appreciable correlations between conflict and productivity for these groups in Table 3, nor do we notice in Table k any consistent effects of project group loyalty on the zero-order correlations for the industrial groups. This difference between the two laboratories 11 may be due to the higher rate of tiirnover of professional personnel in the indusIf this explanation is true, trial laboratory than in the governmental laboratory. we would expect to observe a lower degree of project group loyalty among the Similarly, industrial groups than among the governmental groups. if in fact the professionals in the industrial laboratory have less Job security and are more transient than those in the government laboratory, we might expect them to have a lower degree of loyalty to the organization as a whole, and possibly, as a conse- quence, a higher degree of loyalty to their profession. support for these anticipated differences in Table the project groups on three types of loyalties Insert Table loyalty to the R&D We find some measure of which presents the means of loyalty to the project group. about here. organization, and loyalty to the profession. the governmental groups, loyalty, 5 : 5 ll^ Compared with industrial groups are lower in degree of project group about equal in loyalty to the organization, and higher in professional loyalty. Another group attribute that appears to affect the relationship between conflict and productivity is the size of the project group. In Table 6, we partialled out this variable into project groups with four or less members and five or more members. The smaller the project group, the greater is the negative correlation between interpersonal conflict and productivity. This is true for the govern- mental as well as the industrial project groups. In other words, in smaller project groups interpersonal conflict is more detrimental to productivity than in larger groups. Presumably in smaller groups, one cannot as readily avail oneself of a pattern of avoidance in the event of interpersonal conflict as in the case of larger groups. As a consequence, interpersonal conflict is more likely to exert a negative influence on productivity in smaller than in larger groups. - 12 - The impact of project group size on the relationship between technical conflict and productivity is not consistent. However, the effect of group size in the indus- trial laboratory on the relationship between technical conflict and productivity is noteworthy. a The original correlations of .00 and .Oh become .lU and .12 under condition of smaller group size and -.07 and -.23 under a condition of larger group size. In short, at least among the industrial groups, technical conflict appears to have a slight positive effect on productivity among smaller groups and a negative effect among larger groups. ment project groups, If this finding were also true of the govern- it would be more suggestive than it is. However, among the government project groups, technical conflict among peers apparently has a more Whether positive effect on productivity in larger groups than in smaller groups. the differential impact of project group size on the relationship between technical conflict and productivity in the two laboratories is due to differences in the degree of project group loyalty or to other factors is, at this stage of the analysis, unclear. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION A preliminary analysis of survey data on a sample of project groups in two laboratories provides some measure of confirmation of the hypothesis that inter- personal conflict is negatively associated with performance and technical conflict is positively associated with performance. The evidence is more persuasive for the detrimental effect of interpersonal conflict than for the beneficial effect of technical conflict. The relationship between conflict and performance appears to be partly depen- dent upon at least two variables: group loyalty and group size. Under a high degree of group loyalty, the negative effect of interpersonal conflict on performance seems to be more pronounced than under a low degree of group loyalty. is true, however, This of the project groups in the government laboratory but not in the . - 13 - industrial laboratory. Group size also appears to affect the relationship of conflict and performance in that in smaller project groups, at least in the industrial laboratory, technical conflict is more likely to arise and have a positive effect on performance than in larger project groups where interpersonal conflict is more apt to have a negative effect on performance. Thus management may have to attend to the possible adverse consequences for performance of a high degree of project group loyalty. Apparently, a high degree of project group loyalty, at least in the government laboratory, is not as conducive as a low degree of project group loyalty to the emergence of technical conflict and hence to the positive effect that it may have on performance. With respect to project group size, management seems to be faced with a trade-off between the negative effects of smaller groups, which seem to be conducive to a high degree of interpersonal conflict, and the positive effects of smaller groups in facilitating technical controversy that may be productive If these preliminary findings are confirmed by further analysis of the data of this study, they would underscore the need for management of R & D organizations to consider the consequences of such variables as group size and group loyalty for project group performance and the capabilities of supervisors to manage various types of conflict that arise in the course of team effort. Otherwise put, mana- gerial decisions involved in establishing and dissolving project groups should take account of psychological, social-psychological, and sociological factors affecting group performance. Vfhile we do not as yet have a firm body of knowledge on the factors that lead to optimal group performance, particularly in R So D organizations, some knowledge is accvunulating which could improve managerial decisions pertaining to the composition of project teams. Figure 1 TYPES OP CONFLICT IN PROJECT GROUPS Parties to the Conflict Issues In Conflict Technical Conflict Interpersonal Conflict Peers TC peers SupervisorSubordinates Figure 2 DESIGN OP THE SURVEY Number of Groups Number of Project Group Members Number of First-Level Supervisors Number of Second-Level Supervisors 27 82 27 27 Industrial Laboratory 37 159 30 30 Total 6k 2J«1 57 57 Governmental Laboratory Table 1 INTERCORRELATION OP PERFORMANCE MEASURES FOR PROJECT GROUPS IN A GOVERNMENTAL AND AN INDUSTRIAL LABORATORY (Kendall Taus) Project Groups In a Governmental Laboratory (N Self-Rating of Project Group Productivity (N " 21) Supervisor Rating of Project Group Productivity Plrst-Level Supervisor Rating Project of Project Groups in an Industrial Laboratory 37) Supervisor Self-Rating of Rating of Project Group Project Group Productivity Productivity ._ .^Z »«« .30' Group Productivity Second-Level Supervisor Rating Project of .27» Group Productivity ' •** significant at the ,05 level aignif leant at the ,001 level Ik , .i*l«»« .28* Table 1 INTERCORRELATION OP PERFORMANCE MEASURES FOR PROJECT GROUPS IN A GOVERNMENTAL AND AN INDUSTRIAL LABORATORY (Kendall Taus) Project Groups In a Governmental Laboratory (N Self-Rating of Project Group Productivity (N " 27) Supervisor Rating of Project Group Productivity First-Level Supervisor Rating of Project Group Project Groups in an Industrial Laboratory 37) . Supervisor Self-Rating of Rating of Project Group Project Group Productivity Productivity ._ .13 »•» .30< Productivity Second-Level Supervisor Rating of Project Group .27^ Productivity * *»» significant at the .05 level significant at the .001 level .I'* .i*l««« .28» Table 2 PROJECT GROUP MEANS"*" ON CONFLICT AND PERFORMANCE IN A GOVERNMENTAL AND AN INDUSTRIAL LABORATORY Project Groups Types of Conflict. TC TC IC IC peers ^ sup-sub Project Groups In a in a Governmental Laboratory Industrial Laboratory .32 *35 .37 .38 .17 .21 .15 .17 .69 .70 , 1 peers ^ sup-sub Performance • Composite Rating on Productivity (average of selfrating, supervisor and manager ratings) i" All group means are based on a scale ranging from to 1* . Table 3 RANK ORDER CORRELATIONS BETWEEN TYPES OP CONFLICT AND PERFORMANCE IN GOVERNMENTAL AND INDUSTRIAL PROJECT GROUPS (Kendall Taus) Composite Rating of Productivity Oovernmental Project Groups Types of Conflict IC > (^"37) (N-27) -.38»» Industrial Project Groups . ^02 peers IC -.10 -.13 .1^ 'OO -.02 .04 sup-sub TC peers TC sup-sub ** significant at the .01 level Table ^ RANK ORDER CORRELATIONS BETWEEN TYPES OP CONFLICT AND COMPOSITE RATING OP PRODUCTIVITY IN GOVERNMENTAL AND INDUSTRIAL PROJECT GROUPS, CONTROLLING FOR PROJECT GROUP LOYALTY (Kendall Taus) Composite Rating of Productivity Low Degree of Project Group Loyalty Governmental i Zero-Order Correlations High Degree of Project Group Lovalty Table 5 mean"*" loyalties op project groups in a governmental and an industrial laboratory Governmental Project Groups (N-27) Industrial Project Groups (N-37) Type of Loyalty Loyalty to Project Group Loyalty to the Organization R&D Loyalty to the Profession * ,^1 »3^ .61 *S^ .38 .67 The means computed in this table are based on the means of each of the 6^^ project groups* Table 6 RANK ORDER CORRELATIONS BETWEEN TYPES OF CONFLICT AND COMPOSITE RATING ON PRODUCTIVITY IN GOVERNMENTAL AND INDUSTRIAL PROJECT GROUPS, CONTROLLING FOR PROJECT GROUP SIZE (Kendall Taus) Rating of Productivity C omposite Type o f Con fl ict Project Groups of ^ or less Members Zero-Order Correlations Project Groups of 5 or more Members .. Governmental Project Croups IC IC ..as* [..38»»] -.32 -.21 C-.io] -.06 .I'O .33 C-.02] -.15 peers 8Up-8Ub TC TC .03 C peers ..03 Gup-oub Industrial Project Groups (JiliZl IC IC TC TC * *• peers -.05 C .02] -.02 -.28» C-.13] -.09 .00] -.23 sup-sub .li» peers sup-sub .12 significant at the .05 level significant at the .01 level C -.07 FOOTNOTES * Revised version of e paper presented at the Second ONE Conference on Research Program Effectiveness, Washington, D.C., July 27-29, I965. The author is indebted to a NASA grant to M.I.T. which made this study possible. He is also grateful to James R. Brovm, John Goering, and James R. Miller III for their help in processing the data, 1. Robert L. Kahn et al ., Organizational Stress: Studies in Role Conflict and Ambiguity, New York: Wiley, 1964. 2. Harrison White, "Management Conflict and Sociometric Structure, of Sociology, LXVII (September, 3. Robert L. Kahn, American Journal I961), pp. I85-I99. "Introduction," in Robert L. Kahn and Elise Boulding, eds.. Power and Conflict in Organizations, New York: Basic Books, William M. Evan, Walno W. Suojanen, 196i<-, pp. 1-7; "Superior-Subordinate Conflict in Research Organizations, Administrative Science Quarterly. 10 (June, I965) k. " " PP. 52-64. "Management Theory: Functional and Evolutionary," Journal of the Academy of Management, 6 (March, I963)/ pp. 12-17. 5. See, for example, H. A. Landsberger, "The Horizontal Dimension in a Bureaucracy," Administrative Science Quarterly, 6 (December, I961), pp. 299-332; George Strauss, "Tactics of Lateral Relationships, (September, I962) pp. I6I-I86; " Administrative Science Quarterly, 7 George Strauss, "Work-Flow Frictions, Interfunc- tional Rivalry and Professionalism: A Case Study of Purchasing Agents, Organization, 23 (Summer, 1964), pp. 137-14-9. " Human • -2- 6. See, for example, Lowell H, Mattery and George P. Bush, eds,. Team- work In Research , Washington, D.C.: American University Press, 1953» pp. 171-186; William H. Whyte, Jr., The Organization Man Doubleday Anchor, 1956, pp. 219-222; A. H. . New York: Cottrell, "Scientists: Solo or Concerted," in Bernard Barber and Walter Hirsoh, eds., The Sociology of Science . New York: Free Press, 1962, pp. 388-393* 7. Warren 0, Hagstrom, "Traditional and Modern Forms of Soientific Teamwork," Administrative Science Quarterly . 9 (December, 196^), pp. 241- 263. 8. "General Theory in Sociology," In Robert K, Cf. Talcott Parsons, Merton, Leonard Broom, and Leonard S. Cottrell, Jr., eds.. Sociology. Today . New York: Basic Books, 1959, pp. Theories of Society . '*-l6; Talcott Parsons et al. Vol. 1, New York: Free Press, 1961, pp. « 30-79; Robert P. Bales, Interaction Process Analysis; A Method for the Study of Small Groups 9. . Cambridge: Addison-Wesley , 1951 See, for example, E. P. Torrance, "Some Consequences of Power Dif- ferences on Decision Making in Permanent and Temporary Three-Man Groups," in A. P. Hare, E. P. Borgatta, and R. F. Bales, eds.. Small Groups and L. R. . New York: Knopf, 1955, PP. ^182-1192; N. R. P. Maier Hoffman, "Quality of First and Second Solutions in Group Problem Solving," Journal of Applied Psychology . ^^ (August, I960), pp. 278-283; N. R. F. Maier and L. R. Hoffman, "Organization and Creative Problem Solving," Journal of Applied Psychology . k5 (August, 1961), pp. 277-280. for example, Evan, op. clt 10. See, 11. Edwin G. Boring, History, Psychology and Science; Selected Papers, New York: . John Wiley, I963, p. 68. 12. For an analysis of the methodology of survey research, see, for example, Patricia L. Kendall and Paul F. Lazarsfeld, "Problems of Survey Analysis," in Robert K. Merton and Paul F. Lazarsfeld, eds.. Studies in the Scope and Method of "The American Soldier," Olencoe. 111.: Free Press, 1950, pp. 133-196; Herbert Hyman, Survey Design and Analysis; Principles. and Procedures, Glencoe, 111.: Free Press, 1955- "Organizational Lag," Human Organization, in press. 13. Cf. William M. Evan, Ik. Cf. Robert K. Merton, Social Theory and Social Structure, Glencoe, Cases, 111.; Free Press, 2nd editionj 1957, PP. 225-386; Alvin W. Gouldner, "Cosmopolitans and Locals: Toward an Analysis of Latent Social Roles-I, Administrative Science Quarterly, 2 (December, 1957 )> PP« 28I-306; W. G. Bennis, N. Berkowitz, M. Affinito, end M. Malone, "Reference Groups and Loyalties in Out-Patient Departments,"" Administrative Science Quarterly, 2 (December, 15. See, 1957), PP. '+8I-5OO. for example, D. Cartvright and A. Zander, eds.. Group Dynamics; Research and Theory, 2nd edition, Evanston, 111.: Row Peterson, I96O; Barry E. Collins and Harold Guetzkow, A Social Psychology of Group Processes for Decision-Making, New York; Wiley, 1964; Edwin J. Thomas and Clinton F. Fink, "Effects of Group Size," Psychological Bulletin, 60 (July, I963), PP. 371-38^+; Donald C. Pelz, "Some Social Factors Related to Performance in a Research Organization, " Administrative Science Quarterly, 1 (December, 1956), pp. 310-325; L. R. Hoffman. -1*- and N. R. P. Maler, "Quality and Acceptance of Problem Solutions by Members of Homogeneous and Heterogeneous Groups," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology , 62 (March, 1961), pp. MOl-'iO?. HUV c » 6« Date Due «i)V. |\G ^ 0&M9M' S'89 Lib-26-67 MIT LIBRARIES m'<^ lllilPI 3 DD3 a^T TOfl 5fi7 MIT LIBRARIES 3 TDSD DD3 ATT 5T5 Mir LIBRARIES (o^^ 1751803 DD3 aiT bD3 TDfiD 3 >mM- HD28 ,^)<j-^5^ ' TDflD 3 0D3 bll fiTT M.I.T. Alfred P.Sloan .mUiU School of Management Nos.lU2-65V7orking Papers. Nos. 151-65 MIT LIBRARIES D TOfl D 3 03 b54 a .a 1^-7 ^fcf D03 a^T ^loao 3 t.e'i ri'!-^l„.,„..T TOaO DD3 ^00 153 3 H'^'^^'' SIGN ' 3 "'^"o'a 3 d" Q 3 abT 135 TOaD DD3 TOD 13a at this the card and present it with book CIRCULATION DESK. MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF MIT LIBHARIES '^-^^ TECHNOLOGY LIBRARIES iilllll 3 loao 003 a tiT iba DEWEY LIBRARY