ALFRED P. WORKING PAPER SLOAN SCHOOL OF MANAGEMENT BUDGETARY PARTICIPATION, MOTIVATION AND MANAGERIAL PERFORMANCE by Peter Brownell and Mclnnes Morris J. WP 1389-83 January 1983 MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY 50 MEMORIAL DRIVE CAMBRIDGE, MASSACHUSETTS 02139 BUDGETARY PARTICIPATION, MOTIVATION AND MANAGERIAL PERFORMANCE by Peter Brownell and J. Morris Mclnnes WP 1389-83 January 1983 Abstract The paper proposes an "indirect-effects" model linking participation in budget-setting with managerial performance. Motivation was selected as an intervening variable to explore the proposed model in a field study A significant positive association between participation and setting. performance was observed. However, the indirect path linking participation and performance through motivation was found to explain only a small amount In the study, motivation was measured using an of the relationship. Further analysis revealed that greater participation in expectancy model. budget-setting was positively associated with the extrinsic components of motivation, but negatively with the intrinsic components. Contrary to wide-held beliefs, the results seemed to suggest that participation reinforces the contractual nature of the budget at the expense of the personal satisfactions from derived goal-directed and behavior The managerial implications of the "indirect-effects" goal-accomplishment. model, and of the findings of the study, are discussed in the final section of the paper. Budgetary Participation, Motivation And Managerial Performance Early theorists (e.g., Argyris, 1952; Becker and Green, 1962) tended to the view that participation in the budget-setting process was a major means available with consequent improvement in performance. managers of organizations to gain the commitment to budgets, to summarized this Hopwood (1976) earlier sentiment when he suggested that participation was viewed by many as "...a panacea: a cure for all the many ills Dudgetary traditional systems" which have been associated with even Nonetheless, 74). (p. in the disagreement non-empirical realm in which much of this debate was conducted The reply to Becker and Green by Stedry (1964), together with was evident. Becker and Green's (19o4) rejoinder, illustrates the controversial nature of the debate. As subsequent empirical evidence accumulated, it became clear that these l^ile evidence emerged of positive earlier disagreements were well founded, relationships (Cherrington motivation towards between participation and criteria and Cherrington, (Hofstede, the budget 1967; system 1973), attitude Searfoss (Collins, such as towards and Monczka, 1973; Kenis, satisfaction job job (Milani, 1973), 1979), and no 1975), attitude consistent relationship with performance was found. A possible explanation for the lack of an observed association between participation variables, and that is, affect performance. performance revolves around the role of moderating variables which interact with participation jointly to This view gained formal recognition in the literature with the emergence of the contingency framework for control system design (see, for example, Waterhouse and Tiessen, 1978; Otley, 1980). Specifically with regard to participation, Brownell (1982a) has reviewed and synthesized the literature within a framework which explicitly provides for interactive effects of participation with organizational, Recent variables. work empirical interpersonal, (Brownell and individual has 1982b) 1981, produced results consistent with this "moderated-ef f ects" framework. A possible second literature from emerges and explanation, one the organizational in pursued in this In behavior. paper, literature in the area of goal setting and routine task performance, and Schweiger could (1979) find no evidence between participation and performance. on to that propose performance, extent the to may participation of Locke, is it et. al. found to p. went 138) with associated be this influencing be (1981, Locke association systematic a of review a through the joint mechanisms of inducing higher goals and enhanced goal-commitment on the part in which these authors imply an "indirect-effects" model In other words, of workers. other some aroused condition, participation, by positively is associated with performance. The difference between and "moderated-ef f ects" the "indirect-effects" frameworks is significant, and deserves brief elaboration. effects" model participation neither posits directly variable, To existence affecting illustrate, variables which may of performance; affect to participation. the internal/external locus (1981) found control of participation to affect performance. Internals were perform high better, preferred, and under conditions performed of low effects of the "indirect-effects" latter model on performance posits the in interacted to turn, as by individual the found prefer, while with and externals conditions. themselves unrelated, and contrast, the which are found. existence associated with participation and which, -2- were affected participation However, participation and locus of control were no , with viewed are that participation under better, interact being nor performance, Brownell variables these but "moderated- The of By variables affect performance. In this framework, it between participation and statistically significant associations that possible is intervening variable some on the and hand, one between the intervening variable and performance on the other, could combine in such a way as to yield an insignificant association between participation and performance. this study, In motivation was chosen as an intervening variable empirical investigation of indirect-effects the intuitive plausibility of motivation as an model. 2 Apart in an from intervening variable, the there is This is dealt with in ample justification in the literature for its choice. the next section of the paper. Review of Previous Literature Hofstede (1967), in his seminal study of budgeting, all variables studied, participation is the one with the budget on all measures effect motivation" of observed that, 181-182). (pp. "Of strongest Hofstede used four measures of motivation, dealing with the perceived relevance of the budget and the existence of favorable attitudes towards the budget on the part of budgeted (1973) managers. who studied Similar the were results effects on found by Searfoss motivation of participation and Monczka by general foremen in the development of their budgets; motivation was measured through the use of subordinates' ratings of the level of effort expended by the foremen in goal-directing and evaluative activities based on the budget. In both these studies the concept of motivation was operationalized in terms' of acceptance of, and effort expended to achieve the budget. In this study motivation is operationazlied by use of the expectancy model (see, for example, Galbraith Livingstone [1975]). general in its and Cummings [1967], House [1971], and Ronen and The model, shown in Equation (1), is considerably more interpretation of motivation Hofstede and by Searfoss and Monczka. -3- than the constructs used by It posits that individuals will exert Equation 1 Expectancy Model of Motivation: n M = IV, + b Where: P, 1 (IV a +1 . , 1=1 P„. EV.) 1 2i M is motivation Ivjj the is intrinsic goal-directed behavior with associated valence the is or probability, instrumentality, that Px goal-directed behavior will result in goal accomplishment intrinsic IV^ is the plishment associated with goal accom- is the expectation, or probability, that goal plishment will result in the ith extrinsic outcome, accom- valence P2i i = 1, ,n. EVi is the valence of the ith extrinsic outcome, n. i = 1, , effort towards goal achievement to likely to extent that be the extent instrumental in achieving positively valent the (desirable) that goal, outcomes such effort is perceived additionally, and are perceived the to likely to result from goal-directed behavior and from goal-achievement. The intrinsic valences, or rewards, arise from within individual - the essentially the personal satisfactions derived from goal-directed behavior and goal-accomplishment. external to the The extrinsic individual, such valences as the are proferred organization (e.g. by some agent additional pay, promotion, etc.), peers, subordinates, or parties outside the organization. Some possible effects of expectancy model have been suggested by Ronen and Livingstone speculate, for example, the budgetary participation on the elements of that ego-involvement -4- associated (1975). with They pursuing a self-set goal will enhance IV Kruglanski extent the may EV's the Staw, example, for (see, To . this arise, and [1980] 11978]), which posits that extrinsic and intrinsic rewards are state some theory attribution from following IV, for conseqences adverse attendant however, true, is also and possibly , balance psychological of respect in individual's an to in In other words, a strengthening of the attribution of effort self-perception. to the receipt of intrinsic rewards is a possible consequence the removal of or reduction of extrinsic rewards, and vice versa. A relationship of participation with Raia is also likely. P (1965) This suggested that more difficult goals emerge from participative budgeting. translate into reduced perceptions of might to P but , increase might IV^ enhanced internalized value of achieving more difficult goals the has due (Locke et. al., 1981, pp. 127 - 129). How, or whether these opposing tendencies balance out in their effects on LDotivation effects of is suggests participation an however, Overall, unclear. that assessment is This judgment enhancing than for diminishing motivation. the possible opportunities more are there of for consistent with the empirical findings referred to earlier. Turning to a consideration of question this is Nevertheless, behavior. recently been using relatively a with dealt given to in the the largely the issue effects of motivation on performance, within accounting (Ferris, construction simple the organizational of attention some literature 1977; of domain Rockness, motivation, 1977). found has Ferris, inconclusive results regarding the association between motivation and performance. On the more complex support for other hand, Rockness, formulation of the in an experimental expectancy model, study, found and relatively using strong a a positive relationship. In the organizational behavior area, -5- much of the research examining the relationship between motivation and performance stems from the work of Porter and Lawler They (1968). and performance of explanation that posit also, motivation under should contribute conditions, specified an to job to A great deal of subsequent research has been conducted and is satisfaction. summarized in several review articles (Mitchell, 1974; Wahba and House, 1974; Connolly, 1970). llitchell (1979) concludes that most of the research results support a positive relationship between motivation and performance. Method Data for the study were by collected a survey questionnaire from 224 middle-level managers drawn from three separate companies - two in the electronics positions research, industry in and variety a one in the steel including functions, of industry. managers The production, marketing, One selection criterion was used, and administration. held namely that activities of the managers should be controlled, at least in part, via the use of budgeting. Final sample selection was left top to management of each company; thus the sample was not strictly random. The questionnaire measured participation in budget-setting, motivation and performance. Budgetary Participation The participation measures by Milani by Hofstede each item calling for a response from one to seven. The study. The Milani measure previously performed factor analysis of the scale confirmation Cronbach alpha and item in this scale is designed for an additive construction of adequate (1975) six (1967) were both employed Likert type scale, developed of the single reliability coefficient -6- factor the computed overall (Brownell, nature from is a score, 1982b) and a provides of the measure. The its use in this study was 0.76. Hofstede measure The verbal anchoring. an is eight-point, was used in this study simply It scale, single-choice with validate to full Milani the measure, with which it correlated at 0.59 (p<O.Ol), Motivation The questionnaire items were designed to elicit measures on each construct of the model presented in Equation (1). In the following discussion, we first address valences, and then instrumentalities/expectancies. Valences: The approach measurement the to of valences was adapted the from the procedure developed by Lawler and Suttle (1973) in such a way as to distinguish between three the classes of valence (IV,, IV b Seventeen outcomes were used, outcomes) A. one or extrinsic and , EV's). classified a priori as either intrinsic The outcomes are listed outcomes). (nine a (eight in Appendix For each outcome, respondents were twice asked to indicate on a scale from to nine (extremely desirable to their preference for that outcome. extremely undesirable) 3 the strength of First respondents were asked to value each outcome as it might result from "working hard" (goal-directed behavior), and then to value each outcome as it might arise from "meeting or beating budgeted goals" (goal-accomplisl"UDent) . measured was IV, responses to the intrinsic items from the measured by averaging responses the to the first averaging by set responses. of eight same the items, but eight IV was from the second set of responses. The use of the same eight intrinsic outcomes in the measurement IV of IV, and pre-supposes that a given from either goal-directed behavior or goal-accomplishment; outcome may and moreover, arise that the value placed on it might vary depending on how it arises. The assessed valences from associated the second with the nine set of responses -7- extrinsic outcomes (outcomes (EV's) resulting were from • feoal-accomplishment) Instrumentalities/Expectancies: Equation (1) the requires assessment of nine specific instrumentalities associating goal-accomplishment with extrinsic outcome These instrumentalities were assessed by asking respondents (Pn's). to indicate on a scale from one (never) would result or beating budget" the behavior would result each in questionnaire items elicited measures of goal-directed how often "meeting to seven (always), P-, in the , Three outcome. additional perceived probability goal-accomplishment. For that ease of interpretation, the instrumentality responses were converted to probabilities in the range of zero to one. (p P, < were significantly The three assessments of P, A single measure of 0.01) correlated, with a mean correlation of 0.35. was derived by averaging the three responses. measure final The of was motivation obtained then by aggregating the individual constructs following Equation (1). Performance A self-rating version of et.al. (1963, the performance measure developed overall 106-107) rating. indicated that In the Mahoney The measure calls for ratings on a nine-point 1965) was used. scale for each of eight separate dimensions of performance single by developmental eight work, Mahoney , et separate dimensions were together with a al. (1963 conceived of pp. as being independent, and that jointly, the dimensions should explain at least 55 per cent of the overall rating (the remainder being explained by job-specific factors). Each of these claims was tested in the current study. To assess dimensional independence, a rule of thumb suggested by Pindyck and Rubinfeld (1976, p. 68) was used. is likely to be a Multi-collinearity among an independent variable set concern if the sample correlation between two of the independent variables (dimensions) is larger than the correlation of either or -8- both with the dependent variable (in this case, possible comparisons, intercorrelations three only overall rating). the violated second claim, overall the The sub-dimensions. performance rating regressed was regression explained 78 percent of criterion, this indicating that the eight dimensions are reasonably independent. To test the on eight the variance the 28 Of in the overall ratings, implying that only 22 per cent of the functions critical to effective performance were job-specific in the current sample. Approach to Anal ysis Because motivation, Instead, major the of ordinary hypothesized multiple partial regression, benefit relationship of path between regression in the analysis two collinearity cannot spirit that is in variables a of it participation between used be path analysis, allows analysis. was used. decomposition a structural the in model and into the The of the direct effects of one on the other, as well as the indirect effects of the first on the second, via one or more intervening variables. The basic form of the structural model hypothesized earlier is depicted in Figure 1, denoted as coefficients where participation, X, , in motivation the model as exogenous variable the X~, are and performance denoted p. ., and in the X^. as R, model, The denotes is path the unexplained portions of the endogenous variables, motivation and performance. -9- Structural Model Figure 1: The equations to the structural model are as follows; Xo = ^21 1 (2) '^Zu u X. = p^^X + p^„X + p R 32 2 ^3v V 31 1 The solution correlation ^-i^' for p„ , be can directly A path coefficient correlation between zero-order (3) them assessed between two variables will equal circumstances in where (motivation) only on which, in variable model, is conceived of variable the is as being dependent (participation). X, The solutions to p^^ ways. the a the This is true for viewed to be dependent on a single cause and a residual. X computing by One possibility procedure. ^"^"^ Pin ^^^ ^^ obtained in several equivalent (Stokes, This 1974) involves -10- is by means multiplying of an equations instrumental by suitable Suitable instruments are taking expected values, and solving. instruments, variables contained in a particular equation which are uncorrelated with the residual variable of that equation. X^ are suitable, but X-, In equation (3), for example, X and is not. A second approach, and the one used here, involves partial regression. If, as above, it assumed that is variables explanatory in the residuals are uncorrelated with the the equations variables are expressed in standard form, by ordinary squares least in which they then the p. .'s they procedures; standardized partial regression coefficients. are in appear, can fact be and all estimated equal Equations (2) and (3) to the can be rewritten as follows: ^2 = e2iX^ + P2, R, ^3= ^31.2^1^^32. 1^2 (4) +P3v^ ^^^ The residual path coefficients can be calculated using the general form: = / (1 - r^) p. le 2 where e is r coefficient. the square of the appropriate multiple correlation In the present context, we have: P2u = ^ ^^ - ^12^ P3v - •/ (^) ^1 - ^3.12) <7) Finally, the decomposition of the total effect of one variable in the model on another is obtained using the computed path zero-order correlations: ^12 = P2I rj_3 ^8) = P31 + P32ri2 (9) r23 = P32 + P3;l^12 ^l^^ -11- coefficients and The capture on components second indirect the the right hand while effects, side the equations of and (9) components, first (10) path the coefficients themselves, capture the direct effects. Results Of 224 questionnaires distributed, 140 were returned, a response rate of 02.5%. Of questionnaires returned 140 the improper or incomplete responses. The zero and motivation, are performance, reported in Table the shown are based on a 1 were excluded because of The final sample size was therefore 108. correlations among order 32 in participation, variables, three Table two-tailed test, probabilities The 1. all as are other the results presented in the paper. INSERT TABLE 1 HERE The set of procedure the regressing variables is P32* path X are The path coefficients outlined on X in the and X results coefficients, of and the the previous in The standardized. equation of regression overall are INSERT TABLE -12- 2 HERE X is presented regression significant. regression on coefficient approach The section. multiple a following were computed (3) itself, in which in p^, , and Table are involves all on 2. positive X„ Both and The path coefficients for the errors in Figure 1 derived are by the application of the formulae in Equations (6) and (7) to the results obtained: = 0.990 p., 2u P3^ = U.984 the decomposition of Finally, in Figure 1 Equations following performed is total effects within the model and (9) (10), presented and the decomposition is summarized in Table 3. INSERT TABLE 3 HERE The results performance, proportion, indicate the effect that of through the majority appearing total the motivation to be of effects accounts The direct. participation for only lack of a on small explanatory power of motivation as a mediator of the effects of participation was, of course, first hinted at in Table 1, where the zero-order correlation between the two was found be to conclude that the bulk of necessarily insignificant. the Other direct. It is, nonetheless, premature to effects of participation on performance are variables, operating like motivation but unmeasured in this study, could account for some or all of what in the study has been accounted for as an apparent direct effect. Further Analysis and Interpretation In the aggregate analysis, participation significantly associated with one another. and motivation the expectancy The results of the analysis are shown in Table 4. -13- not To examine this finding further, the associations between participation and the components of model were explored. are INSERT TABLE 4 HERE In the column first simple the correlation between coefficients participation and the nine extrinsic valences are shown. Four of the nine There is no a priori reason to suppose are negative and five are positive. that extrinsic valences should be influenced by the degree of participation, and this is in relation column. to out. However, participation; While only five of significant, The borne third namely, the column shows and the products of participation, the largely contribution of the nine the shown are and through effect rewards to on second three are between participation extrinsic valences. the its the high pay and promotion. correlation coefficients ^ j' s in coefficients are positive, probability of pay raise, extrinsic the coefficients the interesting pattern display an P„'s the P^'s, the - motivation seems as It enhances particularly if the those rewards within the official power of the organization to bestow. An interpretation of this is that participation serves to legitimize the budget as a basis for performance evaluation and contingent administration of organizational rewards. Such a conclusion consistent with previous is findings (Brownell, 1982b), to the effect that a "budget-constrained" style of evaluation was budget-setting; appropriate conversely, in circumstances managers were of found high to in participation respond poorly to participation when the budget was not used as a salient basis for evaluation. Column's and the 4 and intrinsic 5 show the correlation coefficients between participation valences, both of these groups, six of the eight eight of -14- IV 's a the and the eight IV, 's. For D coefficients are negative (p < using 0.11, binomial a test); 12 overall, participation and intrinsic valences are negative (p belief the increasing level the to reduction with previous the strengthening extrinsic with rewards [1980] appears to in diminish, the participation of and task the line In Kruglanski and [1978]), and goal-accomplishment between instrumentality the of (Staw suggestions of in goal-accomplishment. from by pursuit the perspective, devaluation significant derived satisfaction the intrinsic an with associated be of from Indeed, manner. appears goal-commitment enhance associated ego-involvement of to likely Contrary to 0.03). < these results suggest that it appears to be acting self-set goals, opposite is participation that between correlations 16 of some degree, intrinsic the valences. The correlations and participation between the three measures of P, (the probability that goal-directed effort will lead to goal accomplishment) Participation correlates with the overall were all found to be positive. measure of 0.18 at P-, explanations Two 0.08). < (p of would this First, perhaps participation acts to strengthen goal commitment, plausible. There effort, and therefore the perceived probability of goal-achievement. is be considerable evidence from organization! the behavior literature (see Locke et al., 1981) that the intrinsic value of goal accomplishment tends to increase as the level of goal difficulty increases. is central study, the notion of to however, intrinsic valences A explanation. opportunity introduced the to by goal-setting and motivation. negative relating second to goal-accomplishment is explanation negotiate easier participation between association that goals; (Schiff and this mechanism Indeed, In the participation is at odds participation may that Lewin, is, budget 1970; Onsi, current the and with this provide an may be slack 1973). This would be consistent with the observed negative association of participation -15- with the IV less since achieving an easier goal would be likely to lead 's, intrinsic satisfaction. Moreover, since participation appears to be to connected with the use of the budget to evaluate performance as a basis for bestowing it rewards, organizational would be perfectly rational for managers to perceive it to be in their self-interest to negotiate as low a budget as possible (Lowe and Shaw; 1968). The preceding interpretation of about questions Nonetheless, the the study benefits of participation indicate does research findings raises important the significant a between participation and the overall measure of budget-setting. in relationship positive performance. To explore this further, each of the eight sub-dimensions of performance was correlated with participation, and the results are shown in Table 3. INSERT TABLE All eight examination correlations of the are pattern 5 positive, among the HERE and significant (p < 0.05). suggests coefficients that An the dimensions of managerial performance which are most strongly associated with participation are precisely those which might be expected to be strengthened by judicious use of a budget as a managerial tool, in particular planning, investigating and evaluating. Limitations and Conclusions The aim the study "direct-effects" model, an and has of performance consequences of the was to "indirect-effects" potential participation. In to the -16- whether, assess add model to our interests of with compared linking a participation understanding parsimony, of a the single variable, motivation, was included as intervening between participation and The results of the study show that the indirect path through performance. motivation contributes relatively little to an explanation of the observed association between participation and performance. is insufficient grounds for abandoning the have suggested been (Porter and Lawler, performance Lawler, 1968; Other For example, ability and role co-determinants, as however, "indirect-effects" model. intervening variables could enter the model. clarity This evidence, with of motivation, Further research is 1971). necessary to resolve this matter. The form of the expectancy model of motivation adopted in the study also requires further research and possible refinement. offers being a for rationale associated with goal-accomplishment. recognizing the goal-directed Moreover, For example, Staw (1977) possibility of extrinsic rather behavior, valences with simply than multiple extrinsic valences enter into the equation additively, whereas both the intrinsic valences enter the equation as single mean values. This has implications for the relative weightings accorded extrinsic and intrinsic valences respectively in the calculation of motivation. Then there is the question of the appropriateness performance in a research study such as this one. of the measure of The criterion variable of ultimate concern is an organization's creation of profit potential and the realization primarily of this concerned organization. through with efficient the operations. regulation In this limited context, of However, managerial the appropriate budgeting is within an work criterion variable of direct interest is managerial performance, even if the connection between managerial concern. performance and organizational effectiveness remains a broader This still leaves the issue of the reliability of self-ratings of performance, compared with superior, peer -17- or subordinate ratings. While self-ratings have been shown to exhibit leniency error, provided the error is not systematic among the respondents in a manner which is collinear with any of the independent variables, the research findings will be unaffected. Furthermore, Kavanagh et al. (1971) have suggested that self-ratings provide greater discrimination among dimensions of performance than do other sources of rating, and may therefore be more reliable in analyzing the components of performance which contribute to the overall assessment. this, Having said however, exhortations (e.g. Steers, 1977) to devote more research effort to clearly better techniques for measuring performance are the development of valid. Finally, there is the among variables effects issue of the model in and causality the analysis The proceed. direction which in done in the study does not permit any inference about the causalities; these can only be constructed perceived from supporting in participation In theory. this study, may budget-setting it cause is implied higher levels motivation, which in turn may cause higher levels of performance. dynamic, for a inter-temporal context a rationale reverse chain of causality - i.e. could be to of in a But, constructed argue high performance leading to high reality) of high is entirely possible that motivation leading to a perception (and indeed perhaps a participation in budget-setting. that For example, it participation might be viewed as a luxury affordable only to organizational units already performing satisfactorily. i^ven recognizing the limitations of the study, the potentially of significance in the design of control systems. is a costly control strategy, findings are Participation since it consumes managerial resources. The may be compensated by improved managerial performance. But, on the other hand, the findings the findings of also the study indicate indicate some that latent the costs -18- control in system cost participation process, arising in First, ways. two sacrificed through the creation of organizational supplanting being intrinsic costless suppression the to could induce on the achievement These latter organization; stated rewards. hidden And budgetary slack. contingent rewards, performance potential some costs rewards even through, of for costly may be viewed as budgets, be being be secondly, could more may their strongly, example, diminished creativity within the organization. It is precisely because of these hidden consequences that "indirect-effects" model is potentially of considerable practical, as theoretical, interest. which participation benefits, as well A greater understanding of the mechanisms through affects performance should provide management basis for designing the participation process in such a way as all the the and to drive out the this study have indicated. -19- latent costs which the to with a capture results of TABLE 1 Zero-Order Correlations Between Variables Participation(Xi)/Performance(X3) Participation(X]^)/Motivation(X2) Motivation(X2)/Perforroance(X3) TABLE , , , Intercept Value p<O.Ol r = 0.139, n.s. r = 0.248, p<O.Ol 2 Calculation of Path Coefficients Coefficient r = 0.369, Standard Error P3-]^ and P32 t TABLE 4 Correlations between Participation and the Elements of the Expectancy Model Outcome* TABLE 5 Correlations between Participation and the Eight Sub-Dimensions of Performance Planning Appendix A List of Outcomes Eight intrinsic and nine extrinsic outcomes were used in this study. Extrinsic Intrinsic 1. Pay Raise 1. Personal growth and development 2. High Pay 2. Setting higher standards for yourself 3. Respect from boss 3. Giving help to others 4. Respect from other employees 4, Time at work passing fast 5. Receiving more compliments 5. Feelings of security 6. Greater chances for independent 6. ' Setting higher standards for others thought and action 7. Fewer chances to make friends 7. Feelings of accomplishment 8. Special reward or recognition 8. Being tired 9. Promotion -23- Appendix B The solution equation to variables. instrumental (3) can be suggested As through derived earlier, X, and suitable instruments since they are assumed uncorrelated with R the X„ use of serve as . Multiplying both sides of (3) by X, and then by X„ gives: '"' hh = hh - ^31^1^2-^ P32^2 ^ P3l4 P32V2 ^ ^ ^'^ ^3v\K ^^^^ P3v^2\ Taking the expected value of each of (i) and (ii) gives r^3 = P31 + P32ri2 ^^23 = and (iii) ^^^^^ ^^^^ P31^12 + P32 follow (iv) from (i) and (ii) since, for standardized variables, ^'^^ (a) E(X.X.) = r. (b) E(X^) = 1. . 1 The terms involving R disappear since E^^i^v) = ^X.R = 01 V (iii) (poi and and Pon)« _ represent (iv) system of two equations Solving (iii) and (iv) simultaneously gives "^ ^^13 P3I ^^12^23 1\ P 32 a "^ ^ ^23 , 1 (v) 2 r^2 ^12^13 (vi) 2 - r^2 -24- in two unknowns Substituting the values from Table 1 for the r's in (v) and (vi) gives p, " 0*201, values identical to those shown in Table 2. = 0.341, and ^^^ 25- Footnotes 1. An internally controlled individual attributes the outcomes of his o\^m actions to himself, while an externally controlled individual tends to attribute these outcomes to chance, luck, or fate. See Rotter [1966] for details of this variable, together with the instrument used to measure it. 2. Of course, we do not intervening variable, inclusion. intend to imply, by its motivation is the that choice as a single for candidate only 3. These raw scores were reversed and rescaled by subtracting five from all scores. As a result of this procedure, "neutral" responses score zero, while responses in the "desirable" direction score positively (one to four) and responses in the "undesirable" direction score negatively (minus one to minus four). 4. The dimensions were planning, investigating, coordinating, supervising, staffing, negotiating and representing. 5. This procedure is also used to test the suitability of more parsimonious In general, the assessment models which exclude one or more linkages. is of on the matrix suitability based whether original of intercorrelations among the variables in the model can be reproduced by a more parsimonious model. For elaboration, see Land (1969). 6. As previously since P2i is 7. The equivalence of the coefficients derived here with those resulting from the instrumental variable procedure is demonstrated in Appendix B. evaluating, mentioned, it was not necessary to solve equation (2) equivalent to r]^2> the zero-order correlation between participation and motivation. -26- References Argyris, C. The Impact of Budgets on People Ithaca, NY: and Public Administration, Cornell University, 1952. , , School of Business Becker, S. W. and D. Green, "Budgeting and Employee Behavior," Journal of Business (October 1962), pp. 392-402. "Budgeting and Employee Behavior: A Rejoinder to 'Reply'," Journal of Business (April 1964), pp. 203-205. , a Brownell, P., "Participation in Budgeting, Locus of Control, and Organizational Effectiveness," The Accounting Review (October 1981), pp. 844-860. "Participation in the Budgeting Process: When it Doesn't," Journal of Accounting Literature pp. 124-153. , , When it Works and (Vol. 1, 1982a), "The Role of Accounting Data in Performance Evaluation, Budgetary Participation and Organizational Effectiveness," Journal of Accounting Research (Spring 1982b), pp. 12-27. , Cherrington, D. J. and J. 0. Cherrington, "Appropriate Reinforcement Contingencies in the Budgeting Process," Empirical Research in Accounting: Selected Studies Supplement to Journal of Accounting Research (1973), pp. 225-253. , Collins, F., "The Interaction of Budget Characteristics and Personality Variables with Budgetary Response Attitudes," The Accounting Review (April 1978), pp. 324-335. Connolly, T., "Some Conceptual and Methodological Issues in Expectancy Models of Work Performance Motivation," Academy of Management Review (No. 1, 1976), pp. 37-47. Ferris, K. R. "A Test of the Expectancy Theory of Motivation in an Accounting Environment," The Accounting Review (July 1977), pp. 605-615. , Galbraith, J. and L. L. Cummings, "An Empirical Investigation of the Motivational Determinants of Task Performance: Interactive Effects between Instrumentality-Valence and Motivation-Ability," Organizational Behavior and Human Performance (No. 2, 1967), pp. 237-257. , Hofstede, G. H., The Game of Budget Control , Hopwood, A. G. Accounting and Human Behavior Prentice-Hall, 1976. , Assen: , Van Gorcum, 1967. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: House, R. J., "A Path-Goal Theory of Leader Effectiveness," Administrative Science Quarterly (September 1971), pp. 321-338. -27- Kavanagh, M. J., A. C. MacKinney and L. Wolins, "Issues in Managerial l Performance: Multitrait-Multimethod Analyses of Ratings," Psycholgica"" 34-49. Bulletin (January 1971), pp. Kenis, I., "Effects of Budgetary Control Characteristics on Managerial Activities and Performance," The Accounting Review (October 1979), pp. 707-721. Kruglanski, A. W., "Issues in Cognitive Social Psychology" in H. R. Lepper and D. Greene (eds.) The Hidden Costs of Reward; New Perspectives on the Psychology of Human Motivation . New York: Wiley and Sons, 1978, pp. 19-29. Land, K. C, "Principles of Path Analysis," in E. Borgatta (ed.). Sociological Methodology San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1969, pp. 3-2 , Lawler, E. E., Motivation in Work Organizations Brooks/Cole, 1973. , 7. Belmont, California: Lawler, E. E. and J. L. Suttle, "Expectancy Theory and Job Behavior," Organizational Behavior and Human Performance (June 1973), pp. 482-503. Locke, E. A. and D. M. Schweiger, "Participation in Decision-Making: One More Look," in B. M, Staw (ed.). Research in Organizational Behavior (Vol. 1), Greenwich, Ct.:, JAI Press, 1979, pp. K, N. Shaw, L. M. Saari and G. P. Latham, "Goal-Setting and Task Performance: 1969-1980," Psychological Bulletin (No. 1, 1981), pp. 125-152. , Lowe, A, E, and R. W, Shaw, "An Analysis of Managerial Biasing: Evidence from a Company's Budgeting Process," Journal of Management Studies (October 1968), pp. 304-315. Mahoney, T. A., T. H. Jerdee and S. J. Carroll, Development of Managerial Performance: A Research Approach Cincinatti: Southwestern Publishing Company, 1963. , , "The Jobs of Management," Industrial Relations (February 1965), pp. 97-110. Milani, K. W. "The Relationship of Participation in Budget-Setting to Industrial Supervisor Performance and Attitudes: A Field Study," The Accounting Review (April 1975), pp. 274-284. , Mitchell, T. R. "Expectancy Models of Job Satisfaction, Occupational Preference and Effort: A Theoretical, Methodological, and Empirical Appraisal," Psychological Bulletin (81, 1974), pp. 1053-1077. , ., "Organizational Behavior," Annual Review of Psychology (1979), pp. 243-281. "Behavioral Variables Affecting Budgetary Slack," The Accounting Review (July 1973), pp. 535-548. Onsi, M. , -28- "The Contingency Theory of Management Accounting: Achievement Otley, D. T. and Prognosis," Accounting, Organizations and Society (1980), pp. 413-428. , Pindyck, R. S. and D. L. Rubinfeld, Econometric Models and Economic Forecasts New York: McGraw-Hill, 1976. , Porter, L. W. and E. E. Lawler, Managerial Attitudes and Performance Homewood, Illinois: Irwin, 1968. , Raia, A. P., "Goal Setting and Self-Control: An Empirical Study," Journal of Management Studies (No. 1, 1965), pp. 34-53, Rockness, H. 0., "Expectancy Theory in a Budgetary Setting: An Experimental Examination," The Accounting Review (October 1977), pp. 893-903. Ronen, J. and J. L. Livingstone, "An Expectancy Theory Approach to the Motivational Impact of Budgets," The Accounting Review (October 1975), pp. 671-685. Rotter, J. B., "Generalized Expectancies for Internal versus External Control of Reinforcement," Psychological Monographs: General and Applied (1966, Whole No. 609), pp. 1-28. Schiff M. and A. Y. Lewin, "The Impact of People on Budgets," The Accounting Review (April 1970), pp. 259-268. , Searfoss, D. and R. Monczka, "Perceived Participation in the Budget Process and Motivation to Achieve the Budget," Academy of Management Journal (December 1973), pp. 541-554. "Motivation in Organizations: Toward Synthesis and Redirection," in B. M. Staw and G. R. Salancik (eds.). New Directions in Organizational Behavior Chicago: St. Clair Press, 1977. Staw, B. M. , , "Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation," in Leavitt, H. J., L. R. Pondy and D. M. Boje, Readings in Manger ial Psychology , University of Chicago Press (1980), pp. 23-61. , Steers, R. M. Organizational Effectiveness: A Behavioral View Monica, Calif.: Goodyear Publishing, 1977. , , Santa Stokes, D. E. "Compound Paths: An Expository Note," American Journal of Political Science (February 1974), pp. 191-214. , Stedry, A. C. "Budgeting and Employee Behavior: Business (April 1964), pp. 195-202. , A Reply,' Journal of Wahba, M. A. and K. J. House, "Expectancy Theory in Work and Motivation Some Logical and Methodological Issues," Human Relations (27, 1974), pp. 121-147. Waterhouse, J. H. and P. Tiesssn, "A Contingency Framework for Management Accounting Systems Research," Accounting, Organizations and Society (1978), pp. 65-76. -29- kSS3 019 v^ MIT LIBRARIES 3 TDflD DD4 S7h 143 "SM^Aobe: i5 oH dA^ (^^^^^sf^