The Function of the Fornix in Syrian Golden Hamsters: A Lesion and Behavioral Study of Papez Circuit by Anthony 0. Okobi B.S. Neurobiology and Behavior Cornell University, 1999 SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF ELECTRICAL ENGINEERING AND COMPUTER SCIENCE IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE IN BIOELECTRICAL ENGINEERING AT THE MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY BARKER MAHUSSITuTE OF TECHNOLOGY FEBRUARY 2002 JUL 3 1 2002 ©2002 Anthony 0. Okobi, All rights reserved. LIBRARIES The author hereby grants to MIT permission to reproduce and to distribute publicly paper and electronic copies of this thesis document in whole or in part. Signature of Author: SgtefA : of Health Sciences and Technology January 7, 2002 Certified by: Gerald E. Schneider 'rofessor of Neuroscience Thesis Co-Supervisor Certified by:_ Louis D. Braida Professor of Electrical Engineering Thesis Co-Supervisor Accepted by: C I'A Professor of Electrical Engineering andCvo De u cn nXL The Function of the Fomix in Syrian Golden Hamsters: A Lesion and Behavioral Study of Papez Circuit by Anthony 0. Okobi Submitted to the Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science On January 7, 2002 in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirement for the Degree of Master of Science in Bioelectrical Engineering ABSTRACT Little is known about the function of the fornix in hamsters, as well as in other animals. It is a part of the Papez circuit. Papez circuit is known to play an important role in the emotional aspects of behavior. Normal, sham surgery and bilateral fornix lesion hamsters were tested in a semi-natural habitat (SNH). The behavior of the dorsal fornix lesion, pre-commissural and post-commissural fornix lesion hamsters were compared to the behavior of the control animals, as well as to the their own behavior prior to surgery. All the behaviors documented involved instinctual responses, not visuo-spatial task learning responses. Statistical analysis was done on the behaviors to determine the deficits caused by fomix lesions. Results support change in motivation state, rather than spatial memory deficit, as the affect caused by lesions of the fornix. Thesis Co-Supervisor: Gerald E. Schneider Title: Professor of Neuroscience Thesis Co-Supervisor: Louis D. Braida Title: Professor of Electrical Engineering 2 Acknowledgements: * I would like to thank both of my thesis supervisors, Prof. Gerald E. Schneider and Prof. Louis D. Braida. The former for teaching me everything I know about the neuroanatomy and behavior of hamsters and the latter for teaching me everything I know about circuits and bode plots. * Thanks to Rutledge Ellis-Behnke for his assistance in everything from capturing images on the computer to coating subslides with gelatin. * Of course I have to thank my foster mother, Barbara Lewis, for repeatedly telling me to worry about finishing my SM thesis and not kill myself with details. * Finally, this thesis could not have been finished in time without the help of the many lab busy or lazy bees, known as MIT UROP students. 3 Table of Content: I Specific Aim of Research 2 Introduction and Background 2.1 Papez Circuit......................................................................7 2.2 Lesion Studies of Components of Papez Circuit............................ 2.3 3 2.2.1 Hippocampal Formation............................................. 2.2.2 Fornix Fibers.............................................................9 2.2.3 Septal Area............................................................ 2.2.4 Hypothalamus and Mammillary Bodies...........................12 8 8 11 14 Syrian Golden Hamster....................................................... 2.3.1 Taxonomy and History..............................................14 2.3.2 Stereotyped Behaviors............................................... 2.3.3 Photoperiodicity and Rhythmicity of Behavior..................16 15 Materials and Experimental Design 3.1 Animal Subjects and Diet........................................................18 3.2 Surgical Procedure............................................................ 3.3 Semi-Natural Habitat..........................................................21 3.3.1 Schneider Experimental Cage......................................21 3.3.2 Hamster Arena.........................................................23 3.4 Behavioral Testing Procedure...................................................27 3.5 Summary of Experiments.................................................... 3.6 Histological Procedure........................................................33 3.7 4. 6 30 3.6.1 Perfusion and Tissue Sectioning.....................33 3.6.2 Staining Methods.................................................... 33 Statistical Analysis........................................................... 35 Results 4.1 19 In-Cage Behavior..................................................................36 4.1.1 Hoarding Behavior.......................................................36 4 4.2 4.1.2 Nesting Behavior.................................................... 37 4.1.3 In-Cage Activity...................................................... 38 4.1.4 Before and After Surgery: In-Cage Behavior Comparisons........41 Arena Behavior....................................................................42 4.2.1 Activity Level........................................................ 42 4.2.2 Grooming..............................................................44 4.2.3 Novelty Response....................................................45 4.2.4 Foraging Behavior................................................... 46 4.2.5 Before and After Surgery: Arena Behavior Comparison......49 4.3 Food Consumption and Weight............................................. 51 5 Conclusion 52 6 Discussion 54 7 Future Work 56 References 57 5 1 Specific Aims of Research The purpose of this research is to determine the effect of a fornix lesion on the behavior of the Syrian Golden hamster (Mesocricetusauratus). Little is known about the function of the fornix in hamsters or in other animals. The fornix is a bundle of axons that originates in the hippocampal formation, loops around the thalamus and terminates in the hypothalamus. It is a part of Papez circuit. Papez circuit is known to play an important role in the emotional aspects of behavior (Bear et al., 1996). Many experiments have been done on the hippocampus, which deals with memory, and on the hypothalamus, which deals with motivational states, including fear, appetite, etc. While these experiments have increased our knowledge of these two structures, little light has been shed on the functions of the fornix, which links them. We hypothesize that a fornix lesion would be expected to cause some deficits in the emotional or motivational aspects of Syrian hamster behavior, such as loss of curiosity. Lesioning as a method of exploration was chosen because lesion studies have allowed scientists to assign certain functions to specific anatomical regions of the brain, as well as help to separate the roles of different regions of the brain. Such lesion studies have revealed that some anatomical regions of the brain play different roles in different species. For example, Schneider (1967, 1969) proposed that in mammals, cortical and subcortical visual structures have distinctly different roles in mediation of rodent and primate visual behavior. Though lesion studies have been done on the fornix, most of them have concentrated on the fornix's relationship with the hippocampus and the effects of a fornix lesion on spatial memory and visuo-spatial encoding. The research discussed in this thesis, on the other hand, was designed to focus on the fornix's relationship with the other limbic structures, such as the septum and hypothalamus, and to determine the emotional and motivational deficits in the natural behavior of Syrian hamsters that might be caused by a fornix lesion. Thus, one of the big challenges in this experiment was the construction of an environment that was natural enough to elicit normal behaviors from hamsters, yet structured in such a way as to allow control of external parameters and easy quantification of the behaviors. 6 2 Introduction and Background 2.1 Papez Circuit The search for the connection between emotions and the physical brain has a long history. But it was not until 1937, that James Papez first proposed the idea that specific brain circuits were the link between the cerebral cortex and the effector systems devoted to emotional experiences and expression (Purves et al., 1997). Recognizing the unique neuroanatomical properties of the limbic system, Papez argued that the limbic system was indeed this link. Papez based his argument on the works of Philip Bard of Johns Hopkins University and Walter Hess, which demonstrated that the hypothalamus had great influence on the expression of emotion. Furthermore, Papez realized that the cortex and hypothalamus were interconnected through a pathway that included many other nuclei in the brain. This pathway became known as Papez circuit (Figure 1). Although there is still some controversy concerning the exact constitutes of the limbic system, the major structures that make up Papez circuit include the cingulated gyrus, the parahippocampal gyrus (paralimbic area), the hippocampal formation, the septal area, and parts of the thalamus and hypothalamus. Today, Papez circuit is the simplest circuit of the limbic system, which is composed of several other neurological structures interconnected by myelinated fiber tracts to form other various circuits (Nauta and Feirtag, 1986b). The most prominent fiber tract in the Papez circuit is the fornix. It is generally accepted that the various circuits of the limbic system, centered around Papez circuit, generate the experience of moods, emotions, and consolidation of short-term memory into long-term memory. Association area ) (neocortex Paralimbic Anterior nucleicnuaecra of thalamus Entorhinal Cinsaexres fMammillarymbodes areas, area Hippocampal forma I Hypothala us ISeptal x (Ach) IF area Figure 1. Papez circuit plus diagram adapted from figure by Gerald E. Schneider: A brain circuit between the neocortex and the effector systems devoted to emotional experiences and expression Fornix fibers, originating from the hippocampal formation, has many targets. 7 2.2 Lesion Studies of Components of Papez Circuit Papez circuit is composed of several neuroanatomical structures, but in this research we are only concerned with the fornix fibers and the structures that are immediately related to the fornix: the hippocampal formation, the septum, the hypothalamus and the mammillary bodies. The latter two neuroanatomical structures were not lesioned nor were they immediately adjacent to the site of the lesion, but they are targeted by the fornix and thus are indirectly influenced. Information on the brain mechanisms underlying hamster behavior regulation is limited, so many of the lesion studies discussed in this section were done on mice, rats and humans, other than the Syrian Golden hamster. 2.2.1 Hippocampal Formation The hippocampus, also known as Ammon's horn, is an elongated bulge just ventral to the cingulated gyrus that resembles the sea horse from which it received its name. Unlike the other parts of the cerebral cortex, that have six cell laminae, the hippocampus, known as the archicortex ("primitive cortex"), has neuronal cell bodies that occupy only one layer (Nauta and Feirtag, 1986a). Archicortex, although regarded as being a very primitive type of cortex, greatly misrepresents the hippocampus in terms of its precisely organized and highly complex patterns of connections. According to Brodal (1981 a), the most impressive cellular elements of the hippocampus are the large pyramidal cells. The efferent fibers of the hippocampus come from these pyramidal cells and leave the hippocampus via the fornix. The hippocampus is a highly specialized cortical structure that plays a very important role in learning and memory, especially in visuo-spatial tasks. Two aspects of the connections of the hippocampus can be considered crucial to the understanding of its function: First, the extensive two-way connections with various cortical association areas and second, the direct connections with other limbic structures such as the cingulated gyrus and the septal nuclei (Brodal, 1998). The surface of the hippocampus facing the ventricle is the deepest layer, and consists of myelinated fibers, which collect on its surface, giving rise to efferent fibers that unite to form the fornix (Brodal, 198 1a). While anatomically, the delimitation of the hippocampus is fairly clear, its functional relationship with neighboring regions is much more obscure. It is thus common to lump the hippocampus with its surrounding region, including the dentate gyrus, subiculum and entorhinal cortex, together and call them the "hippocampal formation" or "hippocampal region" (Brodal, 1981 a). It is quite widely accepted that the hippocampal formation plays a critical role in learning and memory, but a precise characterization of the type of role and the means by which it accomplishes this role still remains debatable. Spatial learning seems to be the most disrupted in animals with hippocampal lesions, especially in the CAl area. Wilson 8 and Tonegawa (1997) found that CA1-knock out mice were severely deficient in the acquisition of spatial memory (learning of the position of the hidden platform by using the relationships among distal cues around the pool). Hippocampal-damaged animals can still learn to respond to an individual stimulus, but they are notably impaired at many tasks involving learning relationships between stimuli, especially in the absence of explicit reinforcement (Gluck and Myers, 1996). Animals with hippocampal damage have also been shown to have deficits in foraging behavior (Osborne and Dodek, 1986). A lesion study conducted by Borer et al. (1979a), using Syrian Golden hamsters, revealed that following hippocampal transactions, food consumption increased 1.3-fold during the seven postoperative weeks. They further concluded that hamsters with hippocampal lesions not only gained weight, but became hypoactive due to reduced motivation to run and not to neurosurgically induced motor impairment nor to a shortage in metabolic fuels available for exercise (Borer et al., 1979a). Whishaw et al. (1994), on the other hand, found that rats with damage to the hippocampal formation were hyperactive. 2.2.2 Fornix Fibers The most prominent fiber tract in the Papez circuit is the fornix. The fornix is a massive bundle of axons. In the human brain, it comprises a million fibers, which is equal, in this respect, to the optic tract (Nauta and Feirtag, 1986a). Projections from the superior anterior portion of the hippocampal formation come together to form the fornix fiber. According to Per Brodal (1998), with the advent of axonal transport of radioactively labeled amino acids, it has been shown that most of the fornix fibers originate in the subiculurn (part of the hippocampal formation) and not from the hippocampus proper. After culminating at the surface of the hippocampus, the fornix then bifurcates and sends one set of fibers, known as the pre-commissural fibers of the fornix, projecting into the septal area. The second set of fornix fibers, known as the postcommissural fibers of the fornix, loop around the thalamus and terminate in the hypothalamus and mammillary bodies. These post-commissural fibers of the fornix are also known as the fornix columns. Most of the afferent fibers to the mammillary nucleus arrive through these fibers (Brodal, 1998). Current evidence suggests that lesions of the fornix in rats lead to visuo-spatial impairment, much like those experienced by rats with complete hippocampal removal (Whishaw et al., 1997). Walker and Olton (1984) were able to show, in experiments using series of maze tasks that varied in spatial working memory requirements, rats with fimbria-fornix lesions performed at chance levels. They concluded that the performance of these fimbria-fornix lesioned rats could have possibly been due to a temporary retrograde amnesia. Experiments, using radial arm mazes, have also supported these results that the poor performance of fimbria-fornix lesioned rats was due to a difficulty in spatial memory and not from an insufficient time to process stimulus information (Walker and Olton, 1979). Whishaw et al. (1997) notes that like hippocampal damaged rats, rats with fornix lesions seem unable to learn any new spatial response in only a few 9 trials. They did however note that the results they obtained might have been task dependent. However, the results obtained from the fimbria-fornix lesions might not be indicative of the function of the fornix because other experiments have indicated that the fornix and the fimbria differ in their involvement in behavior. For example, using a Tmaze and a semi-circular multiple discrimination apparatus to test the effects of selective partial lesions of the fimbria-fornix, M'Harzi et al. (1987) concluded that the fimbria produced a significant deficit in reversal and place learning, whereas lesions of the fornix only disturbed learning based on a reversal procedure. Combined funbria-fornix lesions seemed to result in impairment of the retention of spatial discrimination, when tested in the two mazes (M'Harzi et al., 1987). Many other fornix lesion studies have concentrated on the relationship between the fornix and the hippocampus and the effects of a fornix lesion on spatial memory and visuo-spatial encoding. But very little research has been done on the motivational aspect of animal behavior, such as in foraging. As was stated previously, animals with hippocampal damage have also been shown to have deficits in behavior dealing with food consumption. Many researchers however have argued that the deficits observed in foraging behavior might be due to the fact that the behaviors observed were documented during tasks that required spatial memory. Hippocampal damaged animals have been known to have information processing deficits during foraging tasks (Osborne and Flashman, 1986). Evidence suggests that while visuo-spatial impairments are seen with fornix lesioned animals, there seems to be no affect on the major homeostatic regulatory mechanisms that might be associated with a damaged hypothalamus, although the fornix projects to the hypothalamus (Osborne and Dodek, 1986). Instead what seems to be influenced were the sequencing of behaviors involved in foraging, eating, drinking, etc. However, alteration of the sequence of behaviors is environment dependent. Research by Collier and Rovee-Collier (1981) has shown that the most important constraints on feeding behavior, in a natural setting, is the availability of food and the animals ability to alter their feeding behavior accordingly. Osborne and Flashman (1986) claim though that when spatial components were removed that fornix lesioned rats adjusted just as well to the different foraging situations as the control. Thus they concluded that hippocampal damage, which they assume was the same as a fornix lesion, does not cause differences in responses to the environment, but instead disturbs the micro-regulation of the sequence of foraging behaviors. Osborne and Flashman (1986) do admit that there is a possibility that fornix lesioned rats might be so sensitive to sensory distraction that even miniscule amounts of sensory information were unable to be filtered out. Thus the disruption in the sequence of foraging behavior shown by fornix lesioned rats might be due to some type of attention disorder. The fornix and/or the hippocampus might then be viewed as a structure embodying a brain mechanism which regulates what behavioral sequence is appropriate for a given situation and the continuation of this sequence to its natural termination. 10 2.2.3 Septal Area According to Brodal (198 1a), the septal area develops from the telencephalon and, in humans, consists of a sheet of gray matter, traversed by many fibers, arranged in the vertical plane of the medial wall of the anterior horn of the lateral ventricle, chiefly in front of the anterior commissure. However, there are slight variations among different species of mammals. It seems that the lateral septal nucleus is the main part of the septal area receiving afferents from the hippocampus, while the medial septal nucleus gives rise to most of the septal efferents going to the hippocampal formation (Brodal, 198 1a). The septal afferents from the hippocampus and the adjoining subiculum project through the fimbria and the pre-commissural fornix to the lateral septal nucleus (Swanson, 1977). The septal nuclei are particularly rich in cholinergic neurons and are in fact the most well defined neuroanatomical structure containing these cells (Brodal, 1998). Thus it is not surprising that acetylcholine is particularly prominent in the septal region. Among the cholinergic cell groups, the septal nuclei was the first to attract interest because early lesions and electrical, as well as chemical, stimulation of this region of the basal forebrain demonstrated that they influenced not only autonomic functions, but also emotions and behavioral reactions (Brodal, 1998). Septal lesions, in particular, seem to alter sexual and foraging behaviors. According to Per Brodal (1998), the effects caused by septal lesions can be in part explained by the connections between the septal nuclei and the hypothalamus, as well as the amygdala, because lesions in these neuroanatomical structures also induce similar effects. According to Siegel (1985), Syrian Golden hamsters with septal lesion have displayed increased levels of aggression, decreased hoarding and nest building. Both antero-ventral and posterior septal-anterior thalamic lesioned groups showed deficits in nesting, hoarding, as well as in sexual behavior (Potegal et aL., 198 1a). Such results were also obtained by Shipley and Kolb (1977), when they demonstrated that septal-lesioned hamsters not only displayed increased levels of shock-induced aggression, but also displayed deficits in nest building and hoarding. According to Borer et al. (1977, 1979b), following septal lesions there is a 1.2fold increase in food intake during ten postoperative days and following septohypothalamic cuts, weight gain was five times faster and food intake was 1.1 to 1.2 times greater during the first postoperative month. Borer et al. (1983a) also states that the hypoactivity that accompanies lesions of rostromedial septum, VMH (Borer, 1974), hippocampal transactions (Borer et al., 1979a), and septohypothalamic cuts (Borer et al., 1979b) is related to acceleration in the rate of weight gain rather than to neurosurgically induced motor impairment or to shortages in metabolic fuels available for exercise. Thus when compelled by electric shock, septal-lesioned hamsters will run as fast and as long as neurologically intact hamsters. According to Borer (1985) what distinguished them from neurologically intact hamsters was the reduced motivation to run or the reduced ability of physical activity to mobilize metabolic fuels in a way that would sustain rapid and prolonged bouts of running, which is characteristic of normal hamsters. Their conclusion 11 was that the limbic forebrain circuit that encompasses fibers interconnecting hypothalamus, septum and hippocampus is responsible for linking spontaneous running and weight regulation in a non-homeostatic fashion. 2.2.4 Hypothalamus and Mammillary Bodies Although the research presented in this thesis is not strictly concerned with functions of the hypothalamus or the mammillary bodies, these structures are the final destination of the fornix and thus should be addressed. The hypothalamus is a structurally small, but functionally very significant part of the brain. It is part of the diencephalon, along with the thalamus, and is crucial for the control and regulation of emotions, homeostatic and reproductive functions, as well as being influential in other more primitive behaviors such as fear, appetite, etc (Purves et al., 1997). Structurally and functionally related to the hypothalamus is the pituitary gland, also known as the hypophysis, by means of the infundibulum (or infundibular stalk). The pituitary gland is a critical endocrine organ capable of regulating the functions of other endocrine organs. Posterior to the main area of the hypothalamus, the infundibulum is continuous with a slight bulge in the floor of the third ventricle, the tuber cinereum. Further along the floor of the third ventricle, a larger bulge occurs at the transition of the diencephalon to the mesencephalon, giving rise to the mammillary bodies (Brodal, 198 1a). According to Brodal (198 1a), the mammillary bodies can be found in the interpeduncular fossa, one on each side of the median plane. The fornix leading to the mammillary bodies can be found among the fiber bundles mingled with the gray matter that make up the lateral walls and floor of the third ventricle (Brodal, 198 1a). Fornix fibers can easily be distinguished as one of the larger fiber bundles leading to the mammillary bodies. Very little is known about the function of mammillary bodies. One of the major deficits associated with damaged mammillary bodies is memory loss and or formation. Clinical observations have identified lesions of the mammillary bodies or their connection to the thalamus (mammillothalamic tract) as being the cause of anterograde amnesia, the inability to establish new memories, and retrograde amnesia, inability to retrieve previously formed memories (Brodal, 1998). Furthermore, bilateral lesioning of the fornix leading to the mammillary bodies in epiletic patients has also been reported to cause the same sort of amnesia (Brodal, 1998). It is generally accepted, after numerous experiments, that the function of the hypothalamus serves not only in the regulation of motivational and emotional rewarding system, but also to coordinate endocrine, autonomic, and somatic motor responses to behavior needed to meet immediate goals and necessities of the body, such as feeding, drinking and reproduction (Brodal, 1998). Furthermore, the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN), which is believed to regulate the photoperiodicity and rhythmicity of many of the behaviors exhibited by the Syrian Golden hamster, is a nucleus of the hypothalamus. Hypothalamic nuclei have a rich network of mutual connections, so that a lesion of one nucleus will interfere with the functioning of several others as well (Brodal, 1998). Thus 12 many of the behaviors exhibited by the hamster can be altered by changing the nature of the signals received by the hypothalamus. Many of these signals received by the hypothalamus come via the fornix. Lesioning of the fornix should inevitably alter the output of the hypothalamus and thus the emotional state and behavior of the lesioned animal. 13 2.3 Syrian Golden Hamster 2.3.1 Taxonomy and History Syrian Golden hamsters are members of the rodent family and are one of many species of hamsters. Originally the Syrian Golden hamster was named Cricetus auratus, based on the skin and skull of one dead female specimen, by George Robert Waterhouse in 1839 (Clark, 1987). Then in 1902 A. Nehring renamed the Syrian Golden hamster Mesocricetus auratus. It carries this name to this day. Currently the taxonomic position of the Syrian Golden hamster is as follows: order, Rodentia; family, Cricetidae; subfamily, Cricetinae; genus, Mesocricetus; and species, auratus(Honacki et al., 1982). Syrian Golden hamsters, also known as Syrian hamsters, Golden hamsters, or Golden Syrian hamsters, are small furry animals, with short legs and a very short tail (Figure 2). ~A Figure 2. Syrian Golden Hamster, Mesocricetus auratus. In the wild, the Syrian Golden hamster is mainly prey to flying predators, such as hawks and owls. So they must be constantly alert and vigilant while in open fields for moving objects in the sky. Hamsters themselves are primarily granivores, eating mostly domesticated grains and wild seeds, but they have been known to also eat the green parts of plants, as well as the roots, fruits, and even insects. A normal hamster does not eat its food out in the open field were it forages, but instead takes it back to the safety of its home before commencing to eat (Schneider, unpublished). Having long sharp claws and teeth, Syrian hamsters are adept diggers in the dry, rocky steppes and brushy slopes that make up their natural habitat. They inhabit underground multi-chambered burrows, in which they hoard the food they foraged from the fields during the twilight and night (Billingham and Silvers, 1963). Each chamber of a multi-chambered burrow usually serves a special function, such as for hoarding food, depositing fecal bulbous or sleeping. In general, Syrian hamsters live alone and will readily fight off other hamsters who enter their burrow (Nowak and Paradiso, 1983; Anderson and Jones, 1984). 14 It appears that most of the Syrian Golden hamsters used as laboratory animals or pets are descended from three or four hamsters captured by Professor Isreal Aharoni of the Department of Zoology of the Hebrew University in 1930 (Clark, 1987). Such a homogenous population of animals might be considered good for research because little variation would be expected to be found from one hamster to another, either behaviorally or anatomically. A second population of Syrian Golden hamsters was introduced to the United States, by Michael R. Murphy, in 1971. Murphy was a graduate student in Department of Psychology (now the Department Brain and Cognitive Sciences) at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT) when he made the second capture of live Syrian Golden hamsters (Murphy, 1985). While at MIT, observations on the natural history and comparisons of these hamsters with the original domesticated stock were made. Descendents of the hamsters captured by Murphy are maintained at the National Institute of Health, Bethesda, Maryland. Syrian hamsters have become established, along with the mouse, rat, guinea pig, rabbit, cat and dog, as a major asset among the important animals commonly used for experimental work in biology and medicine, as well as in other fields. They have many unique characteristics that make them exceptionally well suited as laboratory animals. A unique trait possessed by the Syrian hamster is the paired eversible cheek pouches, which hamsters use to carry food and other small objects, like pebbles. Another unique characteristic of the Syrian hamster deals with its atypical immuno-genetic tolerance to many diseases and trauma caused during surgery in lesion studies (Fulton, 1968). 2.3.2 Stereotyped Behaviors Many of the behaviors displayed by Syrian hamsters are stereotyped and are quite predictable. One such normal behavior exhibited by both male and female hamsters is that of nest building. If given suitable materials, such as cotton, a Syrian hamster will build a nest that is enclosed, much like the burrows they dig. Although the behaviors of hamsters are rarely as stereotyped in the sense that many of the behaviors displayed by birds or reptiles, which are nearly invariant in their form, timing, rhythm, or repetition rate, Syrian hamsters do perform a few actions that can easily be distinguished and seem to serve as communicative functions (Johnston, 1985). For instance hamsters can be observed to groom right before executing a action, such as exiting cage or approaching another hamster, or after being attacked by another hamster, scared, or encountering a novel object. Thus grooming, other than serving its main function as a means of keeping the animal clean, is also an indication of indecision and or disturbance (Schoenfeld and Leonard, 1985). Syrian hamsters show rudimentary grooming-like movements very early in life. Dieterlen (1959) called these early grooming-like actions "movements of defense," because they are displayed in response to irritative stimulation about the snout, as when a pup attempts to wipe its snout immediately before and after sneezing. 15 2.3.3 Photoperiodicity and Rhythmicity of Behavior Syrian hamsters are primarily twilight animals, becoming active only when the sun is setting. They then remain active throughout most of the night. During daylight hours they can be found sleeping or organizing their nests, in the burrows they constructed or stole from another hamster. They have a very precise internal biological clock or circadian rhythm that has been extensively studied. This biological clock seems to control many of the Syrian hamster's behavior, including activity level, food consumption, etc. (Zucker and Stephen, 1973). A true "circadian rhythm" would be expected to have a 24 hour period or cycle, that is maintained with no external inputs. The hamster does not have a "true circadian rhythm", but instead has one that must be kept in synchrony with the earth's 24 hour day. For hamsters, the only documented external oscillator or Zeitgeber is the light-dark (LD) photoperiod (Morin, 1985). Photoperiodic control of the Syrian hamster's circadian rhythm has been well established in many experiments. Many of the research done in this area of hamster nocturnal behavior have utilized specialized rotating wheels to monitor their activity level. Syrian hamsters are sufficiently nocturnal animals that under a LD 12:12 cycle, 99% of all wheel revolutions occur in the dark phase (Zucker and Stephan, 1973). Furthermore, Bruce (1960) demonstrated that a 24-hour light cycle can entrain the hamster's locomotor rhythmicity. The cycle of the Syrian hamsters circadian rhythm varies slightly with the earth's position in orbit around the sun, but on average it is around 23 hours 56 minutes and 3.4 seconds (Morin, 1985). The anatomical relationship of the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) to the visual system has been demonstrated to play an important role in the regulation of circadian rhythms, yet no neuroanatomical correlate has been discovered that might actually generate the periodicity (Moore, 1983; Pickard and Silverman, 1981). Syrian hamster feeding also seems to have a component of rhythmicity to it. Hamsters eat about 15 times spread over 24 hours (Borer et al., 1980; Rowland, 1982; Morin, 1981). Individual meals do not entrain to the LD cycle, nor are they affected by light. Furthermore, feeding does not free run in synchrony with the wheel-running rhythm, but instead persists with a periodicity in the ultradian range. According to Borer (1985), hamster feeding gives the appearance of a centrally programmed ultradian rhythm of standard-sized meals, the spacing of which may be imposed in part by a strong peripheral negative feedback. She believes that this negative feedback is composed of mechanical, chemical, or hormonal signals associated with meal processing and that these signals are dissipated more rapidly in circumstances where increased meal frequency and food consumption are induced. Thus mechanisms controlling feeding behavior seem to be integrated with mechanisms controlling energy storage. According to Granneman and Wade (1982), changes in energy utilization may be more important than changes in energy intake in the adaptation of Syrian hamsters to food restrictions. 16 Hamster feeding also seems to be coordinated by internal clocks that remain unaffected during food deprivation and over-feeding (Morin, 1981b). After being starved for 12 hours or longer, normal hamsters were unable to increase their food consumption, given the same diet, and were unable to change their feeding patterns (Granneman and Wade, 1982; Borer et al., 1979). According to Rowland (1982), measures of the gastric fill and plasma metabolites indicate that filling and emptying of the fore-stomach may occur with a periodicity similar to that of spontaneous meals. The feeding periodicity is clearly distinguishable from the running, gnawing, and hoarding rhythms of hamsters. For instance, hamsters will hoard 45 mg Noyes pellets in a LD rhythm, with approximately ten times more being hoarded at night (Toates, 1978). According to Zucker and Stephan (1973) the rhythmicity of feeding causes males to eat about 55% of their food during the 12-hr dark phase. Thus a certain component of Syrian hamster feeding pattern appears to be a circadian rhythm. According to Morin (1985), it is possible that the circadian rhythm in the feeding pattern of Syrian hamsters is the result of time competition between mutually exclusive behaviors, such as general locomotion or drinking. 17 3 Materials and Experimental Design 3.1 Animal Subjects and Diet We chose Syrian hamsters as subjects in this study because hamsters have some advantages over other animals for use in surgical experiments. Hamsters exhibit many fixed action patterns, known as instincts, that are constant from one animal to another. These instincts are clear-cut and easily quantified. Lastly, Professor Gerald E. Schneider and members of his laboratory have been doing research on Syrian Golden hamster behavior for decades, and thus have a firm grasp of the behavioral patterns of Syrian Golden hamsters. Sexual dimorphic differences between male and female Syrian Golden hamsters in brain structure and behavior meant that we had to use one or the other, but not both sexes. Because males tend to be single-minded in their pursuit of females and were consistently trying to escape, we chose to use only female Syrian hamsters in this research. The food given to the subjects in this research was in the form of pellets that were on average 1" x / 2" x %/". Each pellet was composed of no less than 22% crude protein, 5% crude fat, 5% crude fiber, 6% ash, and no less than 2.5% added minerals. These food pellets were produced by LabDiet@ under the title SP00 Prolab@ RMH3000. The ingredients used to make the pellets are as follows: ground wheat, dehulled soybean meal, wheat middlings, ground corn, fish meal, animal fat preserved with BHA, dehydrated alfalfa meal, calcium carbonate, brewers dried yeast, soybean oil, salt, dicalcium phosphate, DL-methionine, L-lysine, magnesium oxide, ferrous sulfate, zinc oxide, manganous oxide, copper sulfate, calciumiodate, cobalt carbonate, vitamin A acetate, cyanocobalamin, riboflavin, nicotinic acid, calcium pantothenate, menadione dimethylpyrimidinal bisulfite (source of vitamin K), folic acid, pyridoxine hydrochloride, thiamin mononitrate, biotin, and choline chloride. 18 3.2 Surgical Procedure In this research several types of fornix lesion surgeries, as well as sham or exposure control surgeries, were done on various subjects in order to determine the exact function of the fornix. Each subject was anesthetized during the surgery using a combination of valium and pentobarbital. The amount of anesthesia given was determined by that subject's weight, at 0.08cc/IO0g for pentobarbital and 0.16cc/IOOg for valium. After the injected anesthetic had taken hold, the skull of the animal was exposed and an opening was made at the site of surgery using the lambda and bregma landmarks on the skull. Gentle suction was used to remove the neocortical area and expose the hippocampal formation and fornix. This was the point where the control exposure surgeries ended. For fornix lesion surgeries, a stainless steel micro-surgical knife made of tungsten wire was used to cut the designated fornix fibers. After successful surgery, gel-foam soaked in saline was placed into the site of surgery and the skin above the site of surgery was sutured closed using surgical staples. The subject was closely monitored until she awakened and moved around adequately enough to obtain food and water. Approximately two weeks of recuperation was allowed before initiation of the behavioral tests. All surgeries were bilateral (Figure 3). This was done so that the fornix bundle in one hemisphere does not compensate for the loss of the other, which would result in close-to-normal behavior. Although the fornix was cut in these surgeries, the location along the bundle's trajectory from the hippocampus to the hypothalamus where the lesion was administered differed for the different surgeries. Bilateral dorsal fornix lesions were made to disrupt all fornix connections between the hippocampal formation and the hypothalamus and the manmillary bodies, as well as bilateral lesions of the postcommissural fornix fibers (fornix-column lesions), which directly connected the hippocampal formation with the other two structures. Since some of the axons from the fornix fiber reached the hypothalamus via the septum, lesions were made of the precommissural fornix connection (fornix-septum lesions). These surgeries were done to determine which of the trajectories of the fornix were most crucial in maintaining the normal hamster behaviors observed. 19 --- P.ost-comme sural rarnr T _j tiernalIle -u- - Pre-coanmssural rarnix Hippocampal Formnillon T-R 4-1 Figure 3. A visual table of the fornix surgeries performed. All original sketches were done by Dr. Gerald E. Schneider. (1) A top view of the hippocampal formation and the fornix fibers as they would be situated in a hamster brain. (2) A side view of the hippocampal formation and the fornix fibers, along with the location of the hypothalamus and mammillary body. (3) A sketch of a dorsal fornix lesion. (4) A sketch of a fornixcolumn lesion. (5) A sketch of a fornix-septum lesion. (Note: the fact that all surgeries were bilateral is not represented in this figure) All surgeries in this research were done in accordance with the Massachusetts Institute of Technology's Division of Comparative Medicine guideline for animal surgery and the approved protocol for animal surgery, protocol number 97-043-3, in Gerald E. Schneider's Laboratory. 20 3.3 Semi-Natural Habitat 3.3.1 Schneider Experimental Cage The experimental cages, which served as the subjects' living facilities during the experimental periods, were designed by Dr. Gerald E. Schneider. Each cage is 21" x 9.75" x 13.75", and is divided into a front-section and a back-section (Figure 4). The walls of the experimental cage were made of transparent plexi-glass, except for the top level of the front-section of the cage, which was made of chicken wire mesh. Covering the top of the cage was a lid with chicken wire mesh on the interior and stainless steel borders bent 90 degrees to make the lid. Located on the middle of the left, right, front and back of the cage lid were hooks where latches on the experimental cages, located across from the hooks, could be fastened. The back-section of the experimental cage was composed of two levels, top and bottom. Situated on the top level of the back-section were two quartz stones. These stones were placed there for the subjects to use to file down their ever-growing teeth. Connecting the top level of the back-section with the bottom level of the back-section was a tunnel made of smooth polished black plexi-glass. The bottom back-section, which was separated from the bottom and middle levels of the front-section by the same black plexi-glass, formed an enclosed compartment referred to as the den. The den of each experimental cage was filled completely with a mixture of the heat-treated pine shavings and 100% pure cotton balls. The mixture of pine shavings and cotton formed a composite that could be burrowed through by the hamster and used to build specialized compartments, referred to as nests, in the den. 21 Side View Back Front TopLll View Back Front- Figure 4. A transparent computerized model of a Schneider Experimental Cage. The side view shows the front-section of the cage, divided into the top, middle and bottom levels, as well as the back-section with its top and bottom (den) levels. Subjects were housed in these cages during the experimental period. The front part of the cage had three levels instead of two: top, middle, and bottom. The top and bottom levels of the front-section each had a door that lead into the habitat. These two doors, like the walls making up the experimental cage, were made of seethrough plexi-glass. A 6" ramp connected the bottom level of the front-section to the middle level, in of each cage. The middle level was in turn connected to the den by a tunnel. Sticking into the bottom level of the front-section of the cage and opposite the bottom level door was a metal tube, which was attached to a glass water bottle, that served as the hamster's source of water. This water bottle sits outside of the experimental cage on a clear plexi-glass platform attached to the front of the cage. The floor of each cage was made up of rows of metal rods approximately (1 cm) in diameter and spaced so that feces and urine can be collected in a 20" x 10" stainless steel tray, located below each cage. Covering left, right and back walls of the den and the top level of the backsection of each experimental cage, are 10" x 9" sheets of black plexi-glass cover-doors that can be opened. Each cover-door is fastened to the body of the cage by a latch that could be un-hinged, allowing the cover-door to be opened and the investigator to view the inside of the cage through the clear plexi-glass making up the walls of the den and back-portion of the cage. These cages were designed to mimic an underground dwelling, as well as the immediate surroundings, a hamster might inhabit in the rocky hills of a Syrian landscape. Thus Schneider Experimental Cages created a more natural habitat for the subjects 22 compared to the animal Division of Comparative Medicine's standard hamster cages, while maintaining the sanitary, food and water dispensary requirements, as well as ventilation needed to sustain a laboratory animal such as the Syrian Golden Hamster. Furthermore, the transparent plexi-glass walls, as well as the sanitation tray of each cage, provided ways for the investigator to monitor and track subjects inside their experimental cages. 3.3.2 Hamster Arena All behavioral tests were conducted in an apparatus known as the Semi-Natural Habitat (Figure 5), which was composed of the Schneider Experimental Cages situated on top of the Hamster Arena. The floor of the Hamster Arena was constructed out of four rectangular 3/8" birch plywood. Each of these rectangular plywood pieces was approximately 6' long and 2' wide. Long pieces of plywood, with front-end dimensions of 2" x 4", were screwed to the bottom edges of the four Arena floor pieces, so that these four rectangular pieces now stood approximately 4 and 3/8" high. Three holes were drilled into the now 4 and 3/8" sides of the Arena floor pieces along the 6' length, except on the sides that would form the ends of the Arena. Special 4" long pegs were constructed to fit into these holes in such a way that they would rest in holes of one Hamster Arena floor piece and insert into the holes of an opposing floor piece if the two pieces were pushed together. Therefore, when all the Hamster Arena floor pieces were placed together, width-wise so that the pegs on one plywood piece inserts into the matching holes in the opposing piece, an Arena floor that was approximately 8' long, 6' wide, and 4 and 3/8" high was created. All components of the Hamster Arena floor were coated with water-based semi-gloss polyurethane to protect them from the rotting affects of moisture. 23 Caige 1, Cage 2 Cage 5 I(aue 6 Cage 4 Cage 3 CB) ____ ( JCC) F~N / C® / /1 / / ~.1'~~ Figure 5. Semi-Natural Habitat (Hamster Arena and the Schneider Experimental Cages). Six subjects, housed in the cages labeled Cage I through Cage 6, could be tested during an experiment. The circles around the letters A, B, C and P represent the foraging stations with the visual cues Apple, Bell, Cone and Pumpkin, respectively. Represented on the bottom left is the Lake Area. The Novelty Area is the triangular area on the bottom right. The six experimental cages (only four cages in the first experiment) rested side by side along the length of one of the end rectangular Hamster Arena floor pieces. This end of the Arena was designated as the top of the Arena. The other rectangular end floor piece was designated the bottom of the Arena and was extensively modified. An irregular hexagonal portion of the bottom left end of the Arena, which could be approximated by an area 34" long and 30" wide, was lowered 2" below the rest of the Arena. This left bottom region of the Hamster Arena is known as the Lake Area. Additional modifications were made to the Lake Area. Two perpendicular 11" x 6.5" areas were carved out of the Lake Area and two 2" tall clear plastic containers, of the exact same area, were placed into these areas, so that they were flush with the surface of the Lake Area. The left-most plastic container was lined with multi-colored pebbles and filled with sterile autoclaved water. This was the lake. The right-most container was also filled with multi-colored pebbles and was known as the rock bin. Located on the bottomright end of the Arena, was another region known as the Novelty Area. The Novelty 24 Area was designated by an isosceles triangular region made by a 38" long divider that sat in slots on the right and bottom wall of the Arena. Thus the sides of the Novelty Area were 38" x 27" x 27". The divider, which was supposed to characterize a hilly region in a Syrian landscape, was 12" at its highest point and 5.5" at its lowest. A 3.5" x 3.33" opening was carved out of the divider which allowed the subjects to enter and exit the Novelty Area. A new novelty object was placed in the Novelty Area the next time the same subject was behaviorally tested in the Semi-Natural Habitat (SNH). The reactions of the subjects to the novel objects were observed and recorded. Circular holes, slightly larger than 8.5" in diameter, were pseudo-randomly drilled into the floor of the Hamster Arena. Positioned into these holes were four bowls, specially designed by Gerald E. Schneider for food retrieval, known as foraging stations (Figure 6). Each of these stations was a 2.5" deep bowl, with a diameter of 8.5". All the bowls were made of black plexi-glass tubing and had smooth rounded rims. They sat snuggly inside their holes and were held up by a support platform under the floor of the Arena, such that the rims of the bowls were flush with the floor of the Hamster Arena. Located at the center of each foraging station, and raised approximately 2.5" from the bottom of the bowl, was 1/2" thick black plexi-glass circular platform with a diameter of 5". Holding up this platform at its center was a dowel, 8" long with a diameter of 3/8", made of stainless steel and painted black to match the rest of the foraging station. Since the diameter of the platform was smaller than that of the bowl, the subjects could climb into the foraging stations. The purpose of the platform was to hide the food pellets, foraged by the subjects, from view. Dowel 8"5 2.52 F Bow 2.75" 8.5" Figure 6. A sketch of a foraging station. The foraging station was designed to show that the subject could not see the food without either entering the station or coming very near to it. Four visual cues, made of cardboard and painted black, were mounted on top of each dowel. The visual cues were a black Apple to designate foraging station A (also known as Bin A), a black Bell designated station B (also known as Bin B), a black Cone 25 for station C (Bin C), and a black Pumpkin to designate foraging station P (Bin P). Since the foraging station bowls were set into the Hamster Arena, such that they were almost flush with the floor, only the dowels, the visual cues, and a small portion of each station's platform were visible to the subjects. Thus the subjects could not see nor remove food pellets from the foraging stations without crawling into them. The visual cues were used by the investigator to distinguish the foraging stations for observational and data analysis purposes. It was possible that the subjects also used them as landmarks. Enclosing the Hamster Arena on the two sides and at the bottom were three removable walls made of the 3/8" birch plywood. The two sidewalls were both approximately 8' long and 16" tall, while the bottom end wall was approximately 6' long and 16" tall. Insertion slots, 1" deep and 10" wide, were screwed to the left and right sides, as well as the bottom side of the Arena floor, for the walls to be placed in and held firmly. The left and right sides of the Arena had three insertion slots, while the bottom side had two insertion slots. Two wedge-like slots were nailed to the right wall and the bottom wall to hold the Novelty Area divider. Lastly, blockers and covers were constructed to prevent the subjects from escaping during behavioral testing. The blockers were made out of the same 3/8" birch plywood as most of the Arena and were constructed to be inserted in between the experimental cages and between the end cages and the sidewalls. Three covers were constructed out of transparent plexi-glass. Two 35" x 31" covers were used to cover the experimental cages with the front ends extended over the Arena so that the subjects could not climb up the cages and escape during behavioral testing. The third cover was triangular, 45" x 32" x 32", and was placed over the Novelty Area to prevent escape from that avenue. All three walls and the blockers were treated with water-based polyurethane to protect them from the moisture deposited by subjects as they vaginal scent mark and urinate during their foraging of the Hamster Arena. In order to make the SNH resemble the Syrian Golden hamster's natural habitat, mountainous ranges, distant trees, and bushes were painted on the walls of the Hamster Arena. Natural objects that would be found in the Syrian landscape, such as wood, rocks, etc., were placed in the Arena. For example, four polished sections of a birch branch, approximately 1.5" in diameter and 6" long, were attached together using Velcro to simulate a pile of wood debris. A miniature representation of a tree was constructed out of a polished oak branch inserted into a pine log. Three red bricks were used to simulate a rock formation that could provide temporary shelter and protection for the subjects during foraging. It was the hope of the investigator that the more natural environment of the SNH, would help trigger latent instincts and induce the subjects to behave in a manner similar to that of a wild Syrian Golden hamster in the natural habitat of the Syrian country-side. 26 3.4 Behavioral Testing Procedure Although there were slight modifications in the behavioral testing procedure from experiment to experiment, in general the same procedure was followed for all the experiments conducted in this research. Subjects were generally tested over a course of approximately one week to one month, depending on the experiment and survival of the subject (See Summary of Experiments, p. 3 1). The order in which the subjects were tested was pseudo-random, that is to say that all possible sequences were exhausted before repeating the same order of testing. By dropping the food pellets, through the front-section bottom level door of the experimental cage, onto the metal rods, an audible clanking noise was made, that was used to condition the hamsters to come out of their den. During actual behavioral testing, this noise was used to bring the subjects out of their dens before starting the test. Though the subject was expected to forage in the Hamster Arena during the behavioral tests, for additional food, each subject was supplied with a constant source of water and two pellets a day. All observations were done between 2:00PM and 7:00PM. This time was important because hamsters are twilight animals. A behavioral testing period, also known as an observational period, usually ranged between 10 to 30 minutes, depending on the experiment and the subject's behavior. During this behavioral testing period, each subject was allowed to exit her experimental cage and enter the open area of the SNH. The behavior of the each subject was recorded using a Sony 72X Digital Zoom hand-held video camera, during the first three experiments, and using a Sony DCR-TRV900 Digital hand-held video camera, during the last two experiments. The difference between the two cameras is that the latter allowed the digitally recorded video to be directly captured and displayed on a personal microcomputer. The video camera used during the observational period was mounted on a tripod and positioned approximately 7.5' above the SNH (Figure 7). Not only was a camera useful in recording the behavior of the subjects, but it was also useful because they displayed the time of day, and could be used to calculate the length of an observation as well as the length of various behavioral activities. Both types of digital video cameras used in this research had night vision capability. 27 Camera Angle Diagram Video camera Tripod Semi-Natural Habitat 6' Weght BaIance UL Figure 7. A diagram of the position of the video camera during behavioral testing. The camera used during the observational period was mounted on a tripod and positioned approximately 7.5' above the Semi-natural habitat. Before the start of each observation period, the water in the lake area was replaced with freshly sterilized water and a new object placed within the novelty area. Two food pellets, approximately 1" x 'A" x " each, were placed into each foraging stations, if no food pellets remained from the previous behavioral testing period. Care was taken to make sure that each pellet was out of sight and the visual range of the subject until she was within 6" from the edge of the foraging station. The video camera, already mounted and positioned in place above the SNH, was then turned on using a remote control. The bottom plexi-glass door to one of the subject's experimental cage was opened and remained open for the entire duration of that subject's testing. Only the door to the experimental cage of the subject who was being tested was open during that observational period. During the observational period, auditory notes were made about the subject's behavior and any other events occurring in or near the SNH. After the allotted time had elapsed or when the subject had foraged all the foraging stations and had extensively explored the Arena, she was then coaxed back into her experimental cage. Food pellets removed from the foraging stations by the subject that was tested were replaced with new pellets and the door to another subject's experimental cage was opened. The number of subjects tested per day depended on the number of subjects, the experiment, and the time of day the testing began. In the first three experiments, the subjects were kept on a constant day/night cycle of 16 hours light and 8 hours of darkness. Total darkness, the time when no lights were 28 on, occurred at 6:00PM. All observations of these female subjects were done between 3:30PM and 7:00PM, over the week-long period. The time between 3:30PM and 7:00PM is divided into three periods. First of the time periods was between 3:30PM and 5:00PM and was referred to as Day, because all the lights were on in the Behavioral Room, which contained both the SNH and the Schneider laboratory hamster colony. The time period between 5:00PM and 6:00PM was referred to as Twilight because the main light sources were off, but a second smaller and much dimmer light source was still on. Two overhead lights were the sources of the main light. These two overhead lights were on a timer system and the source of the day/night cycle. The smaller light source, which was suppose to represent the weak light from a sunset, was a regular desk lamp, with standard 60 watt Phillips@ incandescent light bulb. This desk lamp was on a different timer than the main lights and was on for only one hour. Since the desk lamp went off at 6:00PM, the period between 6:00PM and 7:00PM was referred to as the Night period. Only the normal subjects of the first experiment were tested in all three Periods. All other subjects used in this research were tested during the "Day" period. In the last two experiments of this research, the subjects were kept under a constant day/night cycle of 14 hours and 10 hours. Total darkness occurred at 7:00PM. A special data sheet was used to record the quantitative aspects, as well as the qualitative aspects, of the target behaviors exhibited by the subjects (Figure 8). Regions of the SNH where behaviors of interest were exhibited were then recorded onto the data sheet, along with the time and length of the behaviors. At the completion of an experiment the video-recorded behavior of each hamster was also reviewed using a VCR and television. Furthermore, the stainless steel tray, underneath the individual experimental cages, was used to determine the overall organization of the den, the bottom level of the back-section of the cage. Because the nests built by the subjects were usually located in the interior of the den, the number, organization, and functions of these nests could not be determined directly. Since the Tray Analysis was done at the conclusion of an experiment, the information extracted from them was the averaged sum of all the events that occurred in the experimental cages, with the most recent events being somewhat weighted greater. 29 3.5 Summary of Experiments Normal control subjects, who never underwent surgery, were indicated by a normal hamster (NH) number (i.e. NH-7). A hamster fornix experiment (Hfx) number (i.e. Hfx-1) was used to identify any subject who had undergone surgery, including sham (exposure) surgery. A "B" in front of the Hfx number (i.e. BHfx-1) was used to identify subjects before they underwent surgery. At the completion of each of the following experiments, all surviving subjects were sacrificed, perfused and their brains preserved in 2% paraformaldehyde. In the first experiment, the control experiment, a group composed of four pseudorandomly selected female Syrian Golden hamsters (NH-1, NH-2, NH-3 and NH-4) were used to determine and quantify normal hamster behavior in the newly designed seminatural habitat (SNH). All four subjects were approximately eight months old and were bred in Gerald E. Schneider's hamster colony. Therefore their parentage and lineage were known. The subjects were allowed 30 days to habituate to the novel environment of the SNH. The behavioral testing transpired over a period of one week, during which time each subject was tested six times. A second experiment was done using six female Syrian Golden hamsters of similar age, three of which were normal and three experimentals. All the experimentals had undergone surgery. The subjects were then randomly placed in one of the six Schneider experimental cages. All the subjects were approximately 8 months old. The subjects were then allowed one week to habituate to the SNH. Both the hamster in the third cage, Hfx-12, and the one in the fourth cage, Hfx- 11, died during this period and could not be behaviorally tested. Behavioral testing transpired over a period of 9 days. The subjects, NH-5 and NH-6, were tested 4 times. Hfx-10 and Hfx-16 were also observed 4 times. NH-5 and NH-6 were normal controls and Hfx-10 was a bilateral dorsal fornix exposure control. Hfx-16 had undergone a different type of surgery then the ones presented in this thesis and thus will not be discussed or used in any of the calculations or conclusions made. In the third experiment, six normal female hamsters, approximately five months old at the beginning of the experiment, were used to determine the effect of a bilateral fornix lesion on Syrian Golden hamster behavior in the SNH. Thus behavioral observations were made of these hamsters before and after they underwent surgery. None of these subjects were closely related. Five of the six subjects were moved into the experimental cages and allowed approximately three days to habituate to the SNH. The sixth subject, who went into the fourth experimental cage, was introduced late into the SNH and but also had three days to habituate to them before being tested. Pre-surgical behavioral testing transpired over a four-week period. The subject in cage 1, BHfx-22, was tested 12 times and the subject in cage 2, BHfx-23, was tested 10 times. BHfx-20, in cage 3, was also tested 10 times, while NH-7a, in cage 4, was observed 9 times. The subject in cage 5, BHfx-19, was observed 10 times and BHfx-18, in cage 6, was tested 12 times. 30 Surgery was done on all six subjects -after the pre-surgery behavioral testing. They were then allowed a four-week recovery period before being placed back into the SNH. It was during this period of time that NH-7a died and was replaced by a normal hamster of comparable age and size, NH-7b. An additional five days were allowed for the operated subjects and the new subject to re-habituate to the experimental cages and the Arena, before commencement of the post-surgery behavioral testing. The postsurgery behavioral observations transpired over a period of three weeks. Hfx-22, in cage 1, was tested 11 times, while Hfx-20, the subject in cage 2, was only observed 4 times. The subject in cage 3, Hfx-23, was observed 5 times and NH-7b, the subject in cage 4, was tested 12 times. Lastly, the subject in cage 5, Hfx-19, was tested 10 times and cage 6 subject, Hfx-18, was tested 12 times. NH-7b was a normal control. Hfx-19 and Hfx-22 had undergone bilateral dorsal fornix exposure surgeries, while Hfx- 18 and Hfx-23 had undergone bilateral dorsal fornix lesions. Hfx-20 will not be discussed or used in any of the calculations or conclusions made, because she underwent a different type of surgery then the ones covered by this thesis. The fourth experiment, like the second, was composed of two groups of hamsters, experimentals and controls. Six female hamsters of approximately eight months old were used in this experiment. Two of them were controls, NH-8 and Hfx-28 (in cages 4 and 5, respectively), while the other four were experimentals. The subject in cage 3, Hfx24, and the one in experimental cage 6, Hfx-26, had undergone fornix-column lesion surgeries, while Hfx-27 (in cage 1) and Hfx-25 (in cage 2) underwent fornix-septum fiber lesions. All subjects were allowed approximately one week to habituate to the SNH before behavioral testing began. During this period of habituation, Hfx-25 died and therefore was not behaviorally tested. For the rest of the subjects, behavioral testing transpired over a period of five weeks. Both Hfx-26 and Hfx-27 were tested 13 times. NH-8 and Hfx-28 were both observed 12 times, while Hfx-26 was only tested 7 times because she died prematurely. The fifth experiment was composed of six female Golden Syrian hamsters, each of which was approximately 4 months old. These hamsters were not bred in Gerald E. Schneider's hamster colony, but were instead purchased from the Charles River Laboratory and shipped in cardboard containers from the Charles River Laboratory to MIT, where they were quarantined for approximately one week before being introduced to the Schneider hamster colony. The subjects used in this experiment had exposure to many more alien environments, than the subjects used in the previous experiments. After approximately three days of habituation to the environment of the Schneider hamster colony, these six subjects were placed in their individual experimental cages. They were then allowed approximately one week to adapt to the SNIH. Pre-surgical testing began on the sixth day after introduction to the experimental cages for subjects BHfx-36, BHfx-37 and NH-10 (in cages 1, 2 and 3, respectively), and on the seventh day for subjects BHfx35, NH-9 and BHfx-34 (in cages 4, 5 and 6, respectively). The pre-surgical behavioral testing transpired over a period of two weeks. Three of the six subjects were tested each day, giving a total of seven observations per subject over the two weeks. 31 At the end of the two-week observation period, the subjects were removed from their experimental cages and surgery was done on them. NH-9 and NH- 10 did not survive the surgical procedure and were replaced by Hfx-38 and Hfx-39. Hfx-34 and Hfx-35 underwent bilateral dorsal fornix lesion surgeries. Hfx-36 and Hfx-38 had undergone bilateral fornix-column lesions, while Hfx-37 and Hfx-39 underwent bilateral fornix-septum lesion surgeries. All the subjects were allowed one full week of recovery before they were re-introduced to the experimental cages. After allowing for another week of full recovery and re-habituation to the experimental cages, the subjects underwent post-surgical behavioral testing. The post-surgical testing also transpired over a period of two weeks, during which time each subject was observed seven times. 32 3.6 Histological Procedure 3.6.1 Perfusion and Tissue Sectioning At the completion of each experiment, the animals used in the experiment were sacrificed. An overdose of pentobarbital was injected into the liver. After the animal became deeply sedated, it was then perfused. First the thorax was opened, by removing the rib cages, and the heart exposed. A perfusion needle, connected through a rubber tube to a container of phosphate-buffered saline solution (PBS), was inserted into the left ventricle of the heart. The superior vena cava was cut and a motorized pump was started that pumped PBS through the circulatory system of the animal, flushing out all the blood. After it was assured that the circulatory system was completely flushed, the PBS was exchanged for 2% paraformaldehyde, which was pumped through the system using the same apparatus. After the animal was properly perfused, the brain of that subject was removed and placed in a container of 2% paraformaldehyde to be preserved. The brain was labeled and left in the refrigerator for at least four days, before being transferred from the paraformaldehyde into a container with 30% sucrose solution. At least three days, or until the brain sank to the bottom of the sucrose solution, was allowed before sectioning the brain. After removing the brains from the 30% sucrose solution, it was placed on a water-resistant treated block of wood. The brain was blocked so that the area of interest could be sectioned. The brain sections were cut on a Bright Microtome Cryostat 5030, OTF Series. First the brain was blocked (i.e. frozen) in an O.C.T. 4583 Compound (OCT), an embedding medium for frozen tissue specimens made by Tissue-Tek@, and frozen at approximately -30 degrees Fahrenheit in the Cryostat Quick Freezer. The brain was then mounted onto a sectioning platform and sections 30 microns thick were cut. The sections were place immediately from the microtome blade to the gelatin-coated subslide. Three series of sections (A, B, and C) were made for each brain. The sectioned brain piece on a subslide of the A series was 30 microns apart from the next sectioned brain piece on a subslide of the B series, which was 30 microns apart from the following section on the subslide of the C series and so on. The A series was used in cresyl violet staining and the B series for myelin staining. The C series served as a spare for the A and B series. 3.6.2 Staining Methods After sectioning the brain, the A series subslides containing the regions of interest were stained using cresyl violet and Loyez stain to determine the exact location and extent of lesion. A modified protocol, from Gerald Schneider, was used to stain for the Nissl substance in the cell bodies of the neurons in the tissue of the mounted sections using cresyl violet. Neurons contain Nissl substance, which is primarily composed of rough endoplasmic reticulum, with the amount, form, and distribution varying in different types of neurons. Because of the RNA content, Nissl substance is very basophilic and will be very sharply stained with basic aniline (cationic) dyes, such as with cresyl violet. 33 Slides containing cresyl violet stained sections were used to identify areas with damaged cell bodies, caused by the surgery. The staining solution used to label the Nissl substances was composed of cresyl violet, glacial acetic acid and distilled water. Before staining, the mounted brain sections were first allowed to dry overnight and then allowed to sit for one more night in 70% ethanol. The next day, these mounted sections were put through a series of distilled water to hydrate them, and then stained with cresyl violet. Four changes of distilled water were used to wash off the excess cresyl violet from the tissues. They were placed momentarily into chloroform and then into the differentiator. The differentiator was made of 95% ethanol and drops of glacial acidic acid. All the mounted sections were left in the differentiator until the background tissue appeared almost white. They were then put through a series of ethanol washes, meant to dehydrate the mounted tissue, and then through a series of xylene solutions. Coverslips were adhered to the mounted sections on the subslides, using Permount@. The B series sections were stained for degenerated axons using Loyez staining method for myelinated axons in frozen sections. Since the fornix fiber is a column composed of myelinated axons, myelin stained sections allowed us to identify whether the fornix, as well as the septum which also contains myelinated axons, was transected by the lesion. Myelin stained section also allowed us to determine if the lesion was complete or there was some sparing. The Loyez staining solution used to stain these sections was composed of haematoxylin, dissolved in absolute ethanol, distilled water, and saturated aqueous lithium carbonate. The protocol used for the Loyez staining was modified from an older protocol used to Loyez stain unmounted floating sections. After cutting, the mounted sections were washed first in 3 solutions of PBS and then again in distilled water. The sections were then placed into a solution of ferric ammonium sulphate for hours, while being gently rocked. They were then washed with distilled water and then placed into the Loyez staining solution for about another hour or two on the rocker. Afterwards, these mounted sections were washed again thoroughly in distilled water. Next, they were placed into the differentiator, until the background tissue appeared almost white. The sections were then washed several times with distilled water, until the differentiator was completely washed off. They were then put through a series of ethanol washes, meant to -dehydrate the mounted tissue, and then through a series of xylene solutions. Coverslips were adhered to the mounted sections using Permount®. 34 3.7 Statistical Analysis Statistical analysis was done on the behavioral testing results obtained from the experiments. The quantitative data, obtained directly from the testing results or calculated from them, were inputted into a statistics software package, STATA@, to conduct all the statistical tests. All statistical results presented were from two-tailed t-test analysis. The graphs were produced using Microsoft Excel@. 35 4 Results 4.1 In-Cage Behavior After the completion of the behavioral tests for each Experiment, the subjects were removed from their experimental cages. The experimental cages were then meticulously examined. Sketch, notes, and videotapes were taken on the organization of the subjects in den. Overall cleanliness of each cage, as well as the location of hoards, was also noted. The sanitary tray underneath each experimental cage was also used to verify the location, function, and organization of the nests, found in the den. However, there were indications that the subjects occasionally reorganized their dens. Thus the location and number of the nests and hoards recorded were the result of the cumulative sum of the subject's activity while in their den. 4.1.1 Hoarding Behavior All normal hamster subjects hoarded food pellets, whether they were given the pellet (all subjects were given two pellets a day) or obtained it from foraging in the Hamster Arena. The number of hoards maintained by normal hamsters ranged from 1 to 4 hoards. Individual number of hoards maintained by the bilateral dorsal fornix exposure subjects also ranged from I to 4. Comparison of the number of hoards maintained by normal hamsters and dorsal fornix exposure subjects revealed that the difference in means was not significant (p= 0.4146). Although given two food pellets a day, none of the bilateral dorsal fornix-lesion subjectss or the bilateral fornix-septum lesion subjects established or maintained a food hoard. Both gave statistically significant differences of p= 0.0027, when compared to the normal hamster subjects. One bilateral fornix-column lesion subject, Hfx-24, maintained one food hoard. Comparison of the number of hoards maintained by fornix-column lesion subjects and normal hamsters gave a statistically significant difference of p= 0.0096. 36 Food Hoard 2.5 2 1.5 0 S1 0.5 0 NH DF DE FC FS Subject Group Figure 8. The mean number of food hoards maintained by the five groups of subjects tested. Statistical difference from normal group: DE p= 0.4146; DF p= 0.0027; FC p= 0.0096; FS p= 0.0027. (NH=normal hamster (n=20), DE=bilateral dorsal fornix exposure (n=4), DF=bilateral dorsal fornix lesion (n=4), FC=bilateral fornix-column lesion (n=4), FS=bilateral fornix-septum lesion (n=4)). 4.1.2 Nesting Behavior When given suitable materials, such as the cotton and pine shaving bedding inside the den of the Schneider experimental cages, normal hamsters would burrow into the bedding and build a nest that was enclosed, much like the burrows they would dig underground in nature. All normal hamster subjects tested built nests. The number of nests built by normal hamsters and dorsal fornix exposure subjects varied from 3 to 5 nests. There was no difference in nesting between normal and dorsal exposure hamsters (p> 0.9999). The mean number of nests built by the bilateral dorsal fornix-lesion subjects also had a variance of 1 nest, but the difference between the normal hamsters and dorsal fornix-lesion subjects in the number of nests built was statistically significant (p< 0.0001). The number of nests built by fornix-column lesion subjects, variance of 1 nest, and the number of nests built by fornix-septum lesion subjects, variance of 0.5, were also significantly different from that of normal hamsters (p= 0.0001 and p= 0.0003, respectively). Not only did the fornix-lesion subjects build lower numbers of nests, but the quality of the nests were also worse. Most of the nests built by fornix-lesion subjects were not enclosed, but instead were found on top of the bedding inside the den. 37 Nesting 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 2 a 1.5 0.5 0 NH DF DE FC FS Subject Group Figure 9. The mean number of nests maintained by the five groups of subjects tested. Statistical difference from normal group: DE p> 0.9999; DF p< 0.000 1; FC p= 0.000 1; FS p= 0.0003. (NH=normal hamster (n=20), DE=bilateral dorsal fornix exposure (n=4), DF=bilateral dorsal fornix lesion (n=4), FC=bilateral fornix-column lesion (n=4), FS=bilateral fornix-septum lesion (n=4)). 4.1.3 In-Cage Activity Analysis of the sanitary tray of each subject, as well as visual observation, revealed that normal hamsters and bilateral dorsal fornix exposure subjects spent the majority of their active and inactive times "underground" inside their den. The majority of their feeding, feces depositing and urination occurred in the nests located in the den of their experimental cages. Although fornix-lesion subjects did build nests in their den, they did not utilize them nearly to the extent that the control subjects did. Furthermore, the fornix lesion groups were in general less active than the control groups and were often found sleeping outside of their den, a behavior that was not observed with any of the normal hamsters. Key: = feces / = pebbles = urine /J)= pellet dust Figure 10. The key to the illustrations used in the sanitary tray analysis figures. 38 Normal Hamsters Back I IiOJ NJ 1- 1 Nil-I trot Back 7J NI-2 HNf-2.3 B~Hfx NI Frlxnl I ~ck 1 Frot| 4i~ 7 , J3Uif-34 Back - NII-1 e NH-1. Rack ___ r NH-I -. = rt c~ r I r H 5ck] tRackj Fra*mi i Fck N IHf CA-3 N V_ I F-x-gJ Figure 11. Summary of the tray analysis of all the normal hamsters. Notice that most of the activity occurs in the den, back bottom portion of the experimental cage. 39 4~ Bilateral Dorsal Fornix Exposure t a ackl FrH -2a8 FrxtBc] Back' I raJ Hfx.8 Hf-'c 1 -1(k Bilateral Dorsal Fornix Lesion Fr~IBick flfN I! t Ji]~ t1 18 Bas ] rji Rack 111V34 2j V T i 71:471) Bilateral Fornix-Column iber Lesion Fruj "Back,! Back Fui MAC-,% Ill' I f-24 ii Ilk J ~t4 r Bilateral Fornix-Septum Fiber Lesion Bc Fr-Ml r_ Al Fr IBA I '13 rI~UI HIxM' jjft j4. ~x~4 r.~ ~, -I Figure 12. Summazy of the tray analysis of all the subjects who underwent surgery. Notice that most of the activity for the bilateral dorsal fornix exposure subjects occurs in the den, the back bottom portion of the experimental cage, while most of the activity for all the fornix-lesion subjects occurs in the front portion of the experimental cage, except for two bilateral fornix-column lesion subjects (Hfx-24 and Hfx-26). 40 4.1.4 Before and After Surgerv: In-Cage Behavior Comparisons Statistical analysis could not be done for the before and after surgery comparisons because the experimental cages of the subjects were only analyzed once, after the completion of the SNH behavioral tests. Bilateral Dorsal Fornix Exposure Both Hfx-19 and Hfx-22 were behaviorally tested before and after undergoing bilateral dorsal fornix exposure surgery. The number of hoards maintained by Hfx- 19 went from 2, before surgery to 3, after surgery, while the number of hoards maintained by HFx-22 remained the same. Likewise, the number and quality of the nests maintained Hfx-22 also remained the same, while Hfx-19 maintained one less nest. Both subjects, in general, behaved and were active in cage the same before the dorsal exposure surgery as afterwards (See Figure 11 and 12). Bilateral Dorsal Fornix Lesion Hfx-18, Hfx-23, Hfx-34 and Hfx-35 were behaviorally tested before and after undergoing bilateral dorsal fornix lesion surgery. All of the dorsal fornix-lesion subjects hoarded before undergoing surgery and none of them hoarded after the surgery. The number of nests maintained decreased for all the subjects after surgery by 2 or more. Furthermore, the in cage activity for all the dorsal fornix-lesion subjects shifted from being mostly inside the den to being mostly outside of the den (See Figure 11 and 12). Bilateral Fornix-Column Lesion The only bilateral fomix-column lesion subject that was behaviorally tested before and after undergoing the surgery was Hfx-36. Although, Hfx-36 maintained a hoard before undergoing lesioning of the fornix columns, she did not maintain a hoard afterward. Like the dorsal fornix-lesion subjects, the number of nests maintained by Hfx36 decreased, from 5 to 3. Furthermore, the in cage activity for this bilateral fornixcolumn lesion subject shifted from being mostly inside the den to being mostly outside of the den (See Figure 11 and 12). Bilateral Fornix-Septum Lesion The only bilateral fornix-septum lesion subject to be behaviorally tested before and after undergoing the surgery was Hfx-37. Although, Hfx-37 maintained a hoard before undergoing lesioning of the fornix-septum fibers, she did not maintain a hoard afterward. Like Hfx-36, the number of nests maintained by Hfx-36 decreased after the surgery, from 4 to 3. Furthermore, the in cage activity for this bilateral fornix-septum lesion subject shifted from being mostly inside the den to being mostly outside of the den (See Figure 11 and 12). 41 4.2 Arena Behavior Syrian Golden hamster behavior was documented during the behavioral tests. The primary behaviors observed were activity level, grooming, responses to novelty objects, and foraging behavior. Videocassettes recorded during the behavioral test for the first Experiment were lost, but information documented in writing during the behavioral tests and summary reports were used to compile the results presented for Experiment 1 below whenever possible. The rest of the behaviors reported below were also documented during behavioral tests in the Hamster Arena. Any uncertainty in the documentation was double checked on the videocassette of the behavioral tests. 4.2.1 Activity Level Activity level for all the subjects was determined by counting the number of times each subject passed through regions of the Arena having the coordinates (3,1), (3,2) and (3,3), during a behavioral test, except for the first Experiment where the regions having the coordinates (1,3) and (3,3) were used. The number of passes through the regions of interest was divided by the duration of the respective behavioral test and results from each member of a subject group were combined and plotted against the time of day. Cage4 Cage2 Cage3 Cagel Cage6 Cage5 7 6 A B 5 C 4 3 2 1 1 2 3 4 5 X-axis Figure 13. Regions of the Hamster Arena were assigned Cartesian coordinates. The number of times a subject passed through the regions (3,1) and (3,2), region (3,3) for the first Experiment, was used in determining its activity level. 42 Results obtained for normal hamster subjects suggest that activity level increases slowly during the Day time period and peaks in the beginning of the Twilight time period. It then plateaus off at a relatively high level (Figure 14-A). Most of the higher levels of activity were due to the subjects from Experiment 1. In general, the bilateral dorsal fornix exposure subjects displayed the same pattern of increase in overall activity level as the normal hamster subjects (Figure 14-B). Both the bilateral dorsal fornixlesion subjects and the bilateral fornix-septum lesion subjects, on the other hand, showed no change in activity level as the time of day progressed (Figure 14-C and E, ). Their activity level was consistently low during any given time of day. The bilateral fornixcolumn lesion subjects also showed an abnormal pattern of activity compared to the normal hamsters and the dorsal exposure subjects. Although their activity level was in general higher than those of the dorsal fornix and fornix-septum lesion subjects, the activity pattern remained relatively unchanged and seemed to decrease during the Twilight time period (Figure 14-D). Controls B A Nmnsd Huwwer 13:55 1:43 1619 2 13:55 2W The (A4r) 14 16:19 TM (A1r) 21W Fornih Lesions D C DTardFumix~mden 2- 2- 01 13-55 1619 194 1355 2117 16:19 14 2111) T= (Wr) The(24r) E 1omx-hnumLe in 2- 1.5 F- 013 :55 1619 1843 21W Tzz(24r) Figure 14. Plots of the activity level of the five subject groups. Normal and dorsal exposure subjects (A and B, respectively) show similar patterns of increasing activity as time progresses. The activity level of the dorsal fornix lesion and fornix-septum lesion subjects (C and E, respectively) stay low and do not change with progressing time, while that of the fornixcolumn lesion subjects (D) decreases slightly with time. 43 4.2.2 Grooming The grooming habits displayed by the subjects were documented and quantified. Grooming in hamsters is known to be an indication of internal conflict. Grooming was only documented if the investigator could see the subject grooming. Thus the grooming reported in this thesis is an under-representation of the number of times the subject actually did groom, especially concerning the grooming that occurred inside the dens of the experimental cages where access was not easy for the investigator. All the subject groups groomed more frequently while in the Arena than while in their experimental cages. The length of each groom, on the other hand, was on average longer for grooming that occurred inside the subject's cage (Figure 15) compared to those that occurred while the subject was outside and away from her cage (Figure 16). This was consistent across subject groups. No statistically significant difference was found between the normal hamster and dorsal exposure controls and the other fornix-lesion subjects for in-cage grooming. While outside of the experimental cages, dorsal fornixlesion subjects groomed significantly longer (p=0.0085) than normal hamsters and fomix-septum lesion subjects groomed significantly longer (p=0.0425) compared to the combined control groups (normal and dorsal exposure subjects). In-Cage Grooming 5oo- 0 I.- Nil DE DF FC FS Subject Group Figure 15. A box-plot of the average length of grooming inside the experimental cages. Statistical difference compared to normal group: DE p-=0.3297; DF p=0.5861; FC p= 0.5484; FS p= 0.2367. (NH=normal hamster (n=16), DE=bilateral dorsal fornix exposure (n=4), DF=bilateral dorsal fornix lesion (n=4), FC=bilateral fornix-column lesion (n=4), FS=bilateral fornix-septum lesion (n=3)). 44 500 Outside-Cage Grooming - 0- Nil DF DE FC FS Subject Group Figure 16. A box-plot of the average length of grooming outside the experimental cages. Statistical difference from normal group: DE p=0. 6 6 8 2 ; DF p=0.0085; FC p= 0.3197; FS p= 0.0528, when compared to controls (NH + DE) FS p=0.0425*. (NH=normal hamster (n=16), DE=bilateral dorsal fornix exposure (n=4), DF=bilateral dorsal fornix lesion (n=4), FC= bilateral fornix-column lesion (n=4), FS=bilateral fornix-septum lesion (n=3)). 4.2.3 Novelty Response Exploration of the novel object was documented when the subjects sniffed the bit it, attempted to pouch it, or scent marked the object either vaginally or with object, their flank glands (Figure 17). There were no statistical significant differences between the groups in their exploration of the novelty object (NO), although comparison between normal hamsters and the dorsal fornix-lesion subjects gave a p= 0.0668 value. Many of the subjects especially those from the fornix-lesion groups, never entered the novelty area (NA) and almost 30% of all the subjects entered the NA less than twice during multiple times they were tested. 45 Entering NA vs. Exploring NO 1.2 z 0.8 W 0 0.6 z e0 0.4 -u0.2 0 NH DF DE FC FS Subject Group Figure 17. Histogram of the average number of times members of each group explored the Novelty Object (NO) after entering the Novelty Area (NA). Statistical difference from normal group: DE p= 0.9915; DF p= 0.0668; FC p= 0.8781; FS p= --. (NH=normal hamster (n=19), DE=bilateral dorsal fornix exposure (n=4), DF=bilateral dorsal fornix lesion (n=3), FC=bilateral fornix-column lesion (n=2), FS=bilateral fornix-septum lesion (n=1)). 4.2.4 Foraging Behavior A subject was considered to pouch a food pellet if she stuffed the pellet into the pouches in her cheeks or carried it in her mouth. Hoarding was defined as the subject taking the food pellet, that she had previously pouched in the Arena, from the Arena into her experimental cage. The water in the lake area, though occasionally explored by the subjects, was in general ignored. There was no statistically significant difference between the normal hamster subjects and the bilateral dorsal exposure subjects in their foraging behavior of pouching food pellets from the foraging stations (p= 0.9213). There was also no statistically significant difference between the normal hamster subjects and the bilateral fornixcolumn lesion subjects in their pouching of food pellets (p= 0.2732). However, highly significant differences in pouching behavior were found between normal hamsters and dorsal fornix-lesion subjects (p= 0.0077), as well as between normal subjects and fornixseptum lesion subjects (p= 0.0276). The average number of times food was pouched from the foraging stations for each subject group is shown in Figure 18. 46 Foraging Behavior: Pouching 0.7- - S 0.6 - U.* U 0.5 - 93 0 U. 0.2 o 0.1 0NH DE DF FC FS Subject Group Figure 18. The average number of times food was pouched from the foraging stations for each subject group. Statistical difference from normal group: DE p= 0.9213; DF p= 0.0077; FC p= 0.2732; FS p= 0.0276. (NH=normal hamster (n-=19), DE=bilateral dorsal fornix exposure (n=4), DF=bilateral dorsal fornix lesion (n=4), FC=bilateral fornix-column lesion (n=4), FS=bilateral fornix-septum lesion (n=3)). All controls, normal hamsters and bilateral dorsal exposure subjects, eventually hoarded the food pellets they pouched from the foraging stations in their experimental cages. Rarely were members of these two subject groups observed to eat food pellets outside of their cages (Figure 19). There was no statistically significant difference between the normal and the dorsal exposure subjects in their open field (outside of cage) feeding behavior (p= 0.1477). Members of all three of the fornix lesion groups were often observed to feed outside of the safety of their cages (Figure 19). The difference between bilateral dorsal fornix-lesion subjects and normal subjects in their open field feeding behavior was highly significant (p< 0.0001). The behavioral difference between normal and fornix-column lesion subjects, as well as the difference between normal hamsters and fornix-septum lesion subjects, were also highly significant (p= 0.00 18 and p= 0.0006, respectively). 47 Foraging Behavior: Outside-Cage Feeding 0.07 0.06 0.05 0.04 0 0.03 0 . 0.02 (U 0.01 0 NH DF DE FC FS Subject Group Figure 19. The mean number of times pellets were eaten out side of the experimental cages, while the subject was foraging in the SNH, for each subject group. Experiment 1 subjects were not included. Statistical difference from normal group: DE p=O.1 4 7 7 ; DF p<0.0001; FC p=0.00 18; FS p=0.0006. (NH = normal hamster (n=16), DE = bilateral dorsal fornix exposure (n=4), DF = bilateral dorsal fornix lesion (n=4), FC= bilateral fornix-column lesion (n=4), FS=bilateral fornix-septum lesion (n=3)). Normal hamsters and dorsal exposure subjects on average explored all four of the foraging stations, as well as pouched food pellets from them. The most explored foraging station was either A or B, depending on which was closest to the subjects cage. The least explored foraging station was P, which was located out in the open, in the center of the Arena. In contrast, subjects with fornix lesions did not explore the Arena to the extent of the controls and they usually concentrated on one or two foraging stations. The mean number of foraging stations explored by the dorsal fornix, fornix-column, and fornixseptum lesion subjects were all significantly lower than that of the normal subjects (p= 0.0001, p=0.0312 and p= 0.0089, respectively). Furthermore, members from all the fornix lesion groups were observed to fall asleep during testing in the open-field of the Arena. 48 Foraging Station Exploration 4.5 4 .e 3.5 3 2.5 2 E 1.5 1 0.5 0 NH DE DF FC FS Subject Group Figure 20. The mean number of foraging stations explored by each subject group. Statistical difference from normal group: DE p=0.4109; DF p<0.0001; FC p=0.0312; FS p=0.008 9 . (NH=normal hamster (n=19), DE=bilateral dorsal fornix exposure (n=4), DF=bilateral dorsal fornix lesion (n=4), FC = bilateral fornix-column lesion (n=4), FS= bilateral fornixseptum lesion (n=3)). 4.2.5 Before and After Surgerv: Arena Behavior Comparisons Bilateral Dorsal Fornix Exposure Both Hfx- 19 and Hfx-22 were behaviorally tested before and after undergoing bilateral dorsal fornix exposure surgery. Before and after comparison of activity level in the Arena revealed no significant difference for either of the dorsal fornix exposure subjects. The same was true for the rest of the Arena behaviors observed for these two subjects. Bilateral Dorsal Fornix Lesion Hfx-18, Hfx-23, Hfx-34 and Hfx-35 were behaviorally tested before and after undergoing bilateral dorsal fornix lesion surgery. Activity level in the Arena decreased after surgery for all the dorsal fornix-lesion subjects, except for Hfx-34 whose activity level score was zero for before and after surgery. The change in activity level was significant for Hfx-l 8 and Hfx-23 (p< 0.0001 and p= 0.0168, respectively), but not for Hfx-35 (p= 0.1174). Changes in cage grooming and outside of cage grooming varied among the subjects, but none of the changes were significant. The same non-significant changes were found for their response to novel objects (NO) in the Novelty Area (NA). 49 Though most of the subjects showed a decrease in their food pellet pouching behavior after the lesioning of the dorsal fornix bundle, only the decrease demonstrated by Hfx-34 was significant (p< 0.0001). On the other hand, Hfx-18 and Hfx-23 showed significant increases in the number of times they ate pellets while foraging in the Arena (p= 0.0029 and p= 0.0130, respectively). Hfx-34 never foraged before surgery and Hfx-35 never foraged for food pellets after surgery. Bilateral Fornix-Column Lesion The only bilateral fornix-column lesion subject that was behaviorally tested before and after undergoing the surgery was Hfx-36, Arena activity level, as well as the in cage and outside of cage length of grooming, decreased after the fornix columns of Hfx-36 were lesioned, but none of the decreases were statistically significant. Hfx-36 never entered the Novelty Area so before and after comparisons could not be made. However Hfx-36 did forage both before and after undergoing surgery and the number of times she pouched after entering a foraging station decreased to zero after the surgery (p< 0.0001). Hfx-36 neither ate pellets before nor after undergoing the fornix column lesions. Bilateral Fornix-Septum Lesion The only bilateral fornix-septum lesion subject that was behaviorally tested before and after undergoing the surgery was Hfx-37. Arena activity level for Hfx-37 significantly decreased after surgery (p= 0.00 10), as well as the average length of her incage grooming (p= 0.0006). Although the average length of her outside of experimental cage grooming did increase, the increase was not statistically significant. And like most of the subjects tested in Experiment 5, Hfx-37 did not enter the Novelty Area. Even though Hfx-37 pouched more often before surgery, she also explored the foraging stations, without pouching, more often. Thus the change in the number of times she pouched, on entering a foraging station, before and after surgery was not statistically significant, although the increase in her outside of cage pellet consumption after surgery was significant (p=0.00 4 3 ). 50 4.3 Food Consumption and Weight Both the control and the fornix lesion groups were given two food pellets a day in their experimental cages, as well as being allowed to forage for additional pellets in the Hamster Arena. Although the control subjects pouched and hoarded significantly more pellets than their lesioned counterparts, the data obtained from weighing the subjects before and after surgery, did not indicate a trend of weight gain or weight loss amongst any of the surgery groups (Table 1). It must be noted that documentation of the weight of the subjects were made more to determine the amount of anesthesia to give each subject, before surgery and before sacrificing, rather than as an end in itself. Weight Change Group Subject # Before-Surgery-After Change in Weight Dorsal Fornix Exposure Hfx-1 0* Hfx-22 11Og 140g 120g 130g + Dorsal Fornix Lesion Hfx-34 Hfx-35 125g 125g 140g 120g + Fornix-Column Lesion Hfx-36 Hfx-38 130g 125g 125g 125g Fomix-Septum Lesion Hfx-37 Hfx-39 115g 11Og 120g 11Og none + none Table 1. Subjects that underwent surgery were weighed before surgery, but after pre-surgical behavioral testing, and again at the conclusion of the experiment, after post-surgical behavioral testing. *Hfx-10 did not undergo pre-surgical behavioral testing. 51 5 Conclusion All in-cage behaviors were significantly altered by the fornix lesions. Significant alterations were also observed in the foraging, exploratory, activity level, and grooming behaviors of the Syrian hamster, but not in the novelty response behavior. Of the behaviors examined, those dealing with cage maintenance and activity, as well as the behaviors dealing with food acquisition and consumption, were the most affected by the lesion of the fornix fiber bundles. Fornix-lesioned subjects displayed drastic shifting of in-cage activity from the "underground" den region of the experimental cages to the "above-ground" front region. Furthermore fornix-lesion groups also showed reduction of the number of nests built, as well as the lack or drastic reduction in the number of food hoards maintained. These results are consistent with the deficits in nest building and hoarding obtained by Shipley and Kolb (1977) and Siegel (1985) in their observation of septal-lesioned hamsters, although these deficits were observed with fornix-column lesion hamsters as well in this study. Although the cause of these results are not entirely clear, a reduction in the level of motivation of the lesioned subjects would be in agreement with Borer et al. (1985) documented decrease in the in-cage wheel-running activity of hippocampal lesioned Syrian Golden hamsters, which Borer et aL (1985) also concluded was due to a reduced motivation to run and not to motor impairment nor lack of food. All fornix lesioned groups on average pouched food pellets much less consistently than the control groups. Instead, the fornix lesion groups were found to be significantly more likely to eat the food pellets out in the open Arena rather than pouch and hoard them in their cages. These results are consistent with foraging behavior deficits documented in rats with hippocampal lesion (Osborne and Dodek, 1986) and with septal lesion (Brodal, 1998). Fornix-lesion subjects were consistently unwilling to pouch, although they had no motor impairment hindering them. They were also observed to occasionally fall asleep while feeding on a food pellet in one of the foraging station in the exposed Arena. Although they could smell the food pellets in the other foraging stations, fornix-lesioned hamsters explored significantly fewer numbers of foraging stations than their control counterparts. The activity levels of the fornix lesion groups, while in the Arena, indicated that overall these subjects were hypoactive compared to the control subjects. Although there were insistances when fornix-lesioned subjects displayed hyperactivity, overall their activity level remained relatively unchanged throughout the day and was consistently low. These results are somewhat consistent with those obtained using Syrian hamsters with hippocampal and septal damage by Borer et al. (1979a and 1983a, respectively), but they disagree with Whishaw et aL. (1994) finding that rats with damage to the hippocampal formation, in open-field, were consistently hyperactive. This disagreement might however simply be due to species difference. Once again the higher activity level observed for the fornix-column lesion subjects was probably due to some sparing of the post-commissural fornix fibers in one or two of the subjects, Hfx-24 and Hfx-26. 52 Increase in weight would have been predicted with the hypoactivity of the fornixlesion subjects and was indeed observed by Borer et al. using hippocampal-damaged hamsters (1979a) and septal damaged hamsters (1983a). However, in this study no such change in weight was observed. One possible reason might be that in this study each subject was given a fixed minimum of two food pellets a day and thus did not have access to unlimited amount of food. Furthermore, fomix-lesion subjects did not pouch and hoard the eight additional food pellets in the foraging stations, which they could then consume at a later time. Normal hamsters, maintained at two pellets a day, displayed normal activity patterns, so lack of additional food consumption was not the cause of the hypoactivity. The results obtained for Syrian hamster response to novelty was also inconclusive, since almost 1/3 of the subjects, both control and fornix lesion, entered the Novelty Area less than twice. The lack of statistical difference between the controls and the fornix-lesion subjects in their in-cage grooming behavior was probably due to the under-representation of the number of times that each subject actually groomed because of visual obstruction and inaccessibility posed by the experimental setup. This is likely to be true because statistical differences were found in grooming behavior out-side of the cage. It is uncertain what the significance of the increase in length of grooming by fornix-lesion subjects might indicate. One possibility is that while the fornix-lesion subjects were in the Arena, they experienced higher levels of anxiety and conflicting emotions over foraging for food pellets, exploring the Arena, or just doing nothing. They often did the latter. This conclusion is not too different from Osborne and Dodek's (1986) hypothesis that fornix lesions disrupt the sequencing of behaviors involved in foraging, eating and drinking. Osborne and Dodek's hypothesis is discussed further in the Discussion Section. Once again, the lack of difference between the fornix-column group and the control subjects in grooming was probably due to some sparing of the post-commissural fornix fibers. 53 6 Discussion All the behaviors analyzed in this study involved instinctual responses, not visuospatial learning tasks. Surgery was done to some of the subjects after they had been familiarized with the SNH, thus the results obtained could not have been due to a simple spatial learning deficit. The possibility of the results being due to either anterograde or retrograde amnesia is also unlikely since the fornix-lesion subjects seemed to remember the location of the few foraging stations they did forage, but were not motivated enough to explore further for additional foraging stations. Likewise, behaviors such as activity level would be predicted to increase, not decrease, if the behavioral deficits exhibited were solely due to amnesia. Increase in food consumption, shown by some studies to occur after hippocampal or fornix lesions (Borer et al., 1979a; Osborne and Dodek, 1986) might account for the lack of hoarding, but it cannot adequately account for the other abnormal foraging behaviors displayed by the fornix-lesion subjects. For instance, why did fornix-lesion subjects not first pouch the food pellets, carry them to the safety of their cage, and then consume the food? Furthermore, fornix-lesion subjects did not show an increase in weight after undergoing the surgery, which would be expected with increased food consumption. Although disruption of the circadian rhythms controlling food consumption and activity level, as a possible explanation of some of the deficits demonstrated by the fornix-lesion subjects, cannot be completely ruled out by the results obtained in this study, it does not adequately explain the deficit in nesting building or the abnormal disorganization of their living quarter. Neither of these in-cage behaviors have been documented to be entrained by an internal biological clock mechanism. Furthermore, disruption of the circadian rhythms cannot explain the difference between the fornixlesion subjects and controls in the length of their grooming, a behavior not entrained by an internal biological clock.. Osborne and Dodek's (1986) hypothesized that the behavioral deficits displayed by the fornix-lesion subjects were the result of disrupted micro-regulation of the sequences of behaviors involved in foraging, eating, drinking, etc. Thus fornix fiber bundles carry information used in determining which behavior in a sequence is appropriate for a given situation and the continuation of this sequence to its natural termination. This hypothesis would offer a possible explanation for the results obtained for grooming, but would not explain or predict the overall hypoactivity of fornix-lesioned subjects, unless the micro-regulator is assumed to be the motivational level of the animal. Since the fornix is a component of Papez circuit, it is likely that disruption of the fornix would alter either the expression or the experience of emotional states. Lesions of another component of Papez circuit, the septum, has been demonstrated in other studies to alter emotional and behavioral responses (Brodal, 1998), as well as foraging behavioral deficits, like those displayed by the fornix-lesion subjects (Borer et al. 1977, 1979b; Potegal et al. 1981 a). According to Borer et al. (1985) the difference between septohypothalamic tract lesion hamsters and neurologically intact hamsters was 54 reduced motivation. A reduction in the level of motivation of fornix-lesioned subjects would not only account for the deficits in foraging behavior, but would also predict the hypoactivity, deficit in nest building behavior and poorer quality of the nests, as well as to some degree the higher mortality rate exhibited by the fornix-lesion groups. Many of the results obtained in this study support this hypothesis. We speculate that the fornix fiber bundles carry information essential for the hypothalamus, the center for emotional expression, to properly coordinate emotions necessary for the survival of the hamster. This study was unable to determine which of the fornix trajectories to the hypothalamus was the most crucial for this process. 55 7 Future Work There are portions of this study which were not completed and need to be in order to better understand the importance and consequences of the results obtained. One such portion is the determination of the extent of the neuroanatomical damage caused by the fomix lesion surgeries. Furthermore, it must also be determined if there is sparing of post-commissural fibers in the fornix-column lesion subjects, Hfx-24 and Hfx-26, and whether the sparing can account for the discrepancies in the results obtained for the fornix-column subjects and the other fornix-lesion groups. Analysis of the neuroanatomical structures will require histological staining, using cresyl violet and loyez myelin stains, of all the hamster brains sectioned in this study and a thorough reconstruction of the brain slices. Documentation of scent marking and other behavioral results obtained during this study must also be analyzed to determine whether these behaviors are also altered by the lesioning of the fornix fiber bundles. A new paradigm needs to be devised to more accurately determine and quantify differences in the response of fornix-lesion and normal Syrian Golden hamsters to novel objects and environments. The response of these two groups to predatory or aggressive cues might also be of interest, given that some studies have reported decreased fear and increased levels of aggression (Shipley and Kolb, 1977; Siegel, 1985). The experiments conducted in this study might also be repeated using male Syrian Golden hamsters to determine whether sexual dimorphic differences exist in the functioning of fornix-fibers. Attempts might also be made to recover the normal behaviors by regenerating the severed fomix axonal fibers, via peripheral nerve grafts. Functional recovery of normal behaviors by fornix lesion animals would be a good indication of the effectiveness of various CNS regenerative techniques. 56 References 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. Ackroff, K. and A. Sclafani, Flavorpreferences conditionedby sugars:rats learn to prefer glucose overfructose. Physiol Behav, 1991. 50(4): p. 815-24. Alantar, A., et al., Potentiationoffentanyl suppressionof the jaw-opening reflex by transcranialelectricalstimulation. Brain Res, 1997. 763(1): p. 14-20. Ammassari-Teule, M. and C. Maho, Choice behavior offornix-damagedrats in radialmaze error-freesituationsand subsequent learning.Physiol Behav, 1992. 51(3): p. 563-7. Anderson, S. and Jones, J., Jr., Order and Families ofRecent Mammals ofthe World. 1984. Wiley, New York. Appenrodt, E. and H. Schwarzberg, Septal vasopressin modulates motility and passive avoidance inpinealectomizedrats. Physiol Behav, 1999. 66(5): p. 757-61. Asher, R.J., Morphologicaldiversity of anatomicalstrepsirrhinismand the evolution of the lemuriform toothcomb. Am J Phys Anthropol, 1998. 105(3): p. 355-67. Baldi, B., et al., Effects of combined medial septal area,fimbria-fornix and entorhinalcortex tetrodotoxin inactivationson passive avoidance response consolidationin the rat.Brain Res, 1999. 821(2): p. 503-10. Balse, E., et al., Intrahippocampalgrafts containingcholinergicand serotonergic fetal neurons amelioratespatialreference but not working memory in rats with fimbria-fornix/cingularbundle lesions. Brain Res Bull, 1999. 49(4): p. 263-72. Bartness, T.J., N.F. Ruby, and G.N. Wade, Dietaryobesity in exercisingor coldexposed Syrian hamsters. Physiol Behav, 1984. 32(1): p. 85-90. Bauman, T.R., R.R. Anderson, and C.W. Turner, Thyroid hormone secretion rates andfood consumption of the hamster (Mesocricetusauratus)at 25.5 degree and 4.5 degree C. Gen Comp Endocrinol, 1968. 10(1): p. 92-8. Bear, M.F,, Connors, B.W., Paradiso, M.A., (Eds.) Neuroscience Exploringthe Brain. 1996. Academic Press, Boston. Bernardis, L.L. and L.A. Frohman, Effects ofhypothalamic lesions at different loci on development of hyperinsulinemiaand obesity in the weanling rat. J Comp Neurol, 1971. 141(1): p. 107-15. Billingham, R. and Silvers, W. (1963). Skin transplants and the hamster. Sci. Am. 208, 118-127. Borer, K.T., Absence of weight regulation in exercising hamsters. Physiol Behav, 1974. 12(4): p. 589-97. Borer, K.T., et al., The role ofthe septal area in the neuroendocrinecontrol of growth in the adult golden hamster. Neuroendocrinology, 1977. 23(3): p. 133-50. Borer, K.T., et al., Increasedserum growth hormone and somatic growth in adult hamsters with hippocampaltransections.Neuroendocrinology, 1979a. 29(1): p. 22-33. Borer, K.T., et al., Contributionof growth,fatness, andactivity to weight disturbanceafter septohypothalamiccuts in adult hamsters. J Comp Physiol Psychol, 1979b. 93(5): p. 907-18. 57 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. Borer, K.T., et al., Physiologicaland behavioralresponses to starvationin the golden hamster. Am J Physiol, 1979b. 236(2): p. E105-12. Borer, K.T., M.E. Trulson, and L.R. Kuhns, Role oflimbic system in the control of hamster growth. Brain Res Bull, 1979a. 4(2): p. 239-47. Borer, K.T., C.D. Potter, and N. Fileccia, Basisfor the hypoactivity that accompanies rapidweight gain in hamsters. Physiol Behav, 1983c. 30(3): p. 38997. Borer, K.T., et al., Exercise reversesphotoperiodicanestrusin golden hamsters. Biol Reprod, 1983b. 29(1): p. 38-47. Borer, K.T., B. Shapiro, and A.I. Vinik, A rolefor somatostatinin the control of hamster growth. Brain Res Bull, 1983a. 11(6): p. 663-9. Borer, K.T., et al., Recoveryfrom energy deficit in golden hamsters. Am J Physiol, 1985. 248(4 Pt 2): p. R439-46. Borer, K.T., Regulation of Energy Balance in the Golden Hamster. The Hamster: Reproduction and Behavior, 1985. pp. 3-19. H.I. Siegel (Ed.). Plenum Press, New York. Bouton, M.E., Context, time, andmemory retrievalin the interferenceparadigms ofPavlovian learning.Psychol Bull, 1993. 114(1): p. 80-99. Brandner, C. and F. Schenk, Septal lesions impairthe acquisitionof a cued place navigation task: attentionalor memory deficit? Neurobiol Learn Mem, 1998. 69(2): p. 106-25. Brito, G.N. and G.J. Thomas, T-maze alternation,responsepatterning,and septohippocampalcircuitryin rats. Behav Brain Res, 1981. 3(3): p. 319-40. Brito, G.N. and L.S. Brito, Lesions in the septo-hippocampalsystem orprelimbic cortex ofrats do not alter the performance of a T-maze visual discriminationtask but disruptthe performance of a T-maze alternationtask Braz J Med Biol Res, 1987. 20(3-4): p. 461-5. Brito, G.N. and L.S. Brito, Septohippocampalsystem and the prelimbicsector of frontal cortex: a neuropsychologicalbattery analysis in the rat.Behav Brain Res, 1990. 36(1-2): p. 127-46. Brodal, A., The Olfactory Pathway. The Amygdala. The Hippocampus. The "Limbic System ". Neurological Anatomy: In Relation to Clinical Medicine (3 rd Ed.), 1981. pp. 640-697. Oxford University Press, New York. Brodal, A., The Autonomic Nervous System. The Hypothalamus. Neurological Anatomy: In Relation to Clinical Medicine (3 rd Ed.), 1981. pp. 698-787. Oxford University Press, New York Brodal, P., The Cerebral Cortex and Limbic Structures. The Central Nervous System: Structure and Function (2 nd Ed.), 1998. pp. 553-620. Oxford University Press, New York. Brooks, A.I., et al., Repeated acquisitionandperformance chamberfor mice: a paradigmfor assessment of spatiallearningand memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 2000. 74(3): p. 241-58. Buhusi, C.V. and N.A. Schmajuk, Attention, configuration,and hippocampal function. Hippocampus, 1996. 6(6): p. 621-42. Bunnell, B.N., F.J. Sodetz, Jr., and D.I. Shalloway, Amygdaloid lesions and social behavior in the golden hamster.Physiol Behav, 1970. 5(2): p. 153-61. 58 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. Burton, S., et al., Combined lesions of hippocampus and subiculum Do not produce deficits in a nonspatialsocial olfactory memory task J Neurosci, 2000. 20(14): p. 5468-75. Bussey, T.J., et al., Distinctpatterns of behaviouralimpairmentsresultingfrom fornix transectionor neurotoxic lesions ofthe perirhinalandpostrhinalcortices in the rat. Behav Brain Res, 2000. 111(1-2): p. 187-202. Buzsaki, G., et al., Spatial mapping, working memory, and thefimbria-fornix system. J Comp Physiol Psychol, 1982. 96(1): p. 26-34. Calabrese, P., et al., Fornix damage and memory. A case report.Cortex, 1995. 31(3): p. 555-64. Canguilhem, B., et al., [Feedingbehavior, circannualbody weight and hibernationrhythms in Europeanhamsters lesioned in the noradrenergic ascendingbundles (author'stransl)]. Physiol Behav, 1977. 18(6): p. 1067-74. Capone, F., et al., Oxotremorine-inducedmodifications of the behavioraland neuroendocrineresponses toformalin pain in male rats. Brain Res, 1999. 830(2): p. 292-300. Cassaday, H.J., et al., 5,7-Dihydroxytryptaminelesions in thefornix-fimbria attenuate latent inhibition.Behav Neural Biol, 1993. 59(3): p. 194-207. Cassaday, H.J. and J.N. Rawlins, Fornix-fimbriasection and working memory deficits in rats: stimulus complexity and stimulus size. Behav Neurosci, 1995. 109(4): p. 594-606. Cassel, J.C., et al., The fimbria-fornix/cingularbundle pathways: a review of neurochemicaland behaviouralapproachesusing lesions and transplantation techniques. Prog Neurobiol, 1997. 51(6): p. 663-716. Cassel, J.C., et al., Fimbria-fornixvs selective hippocampallesions in rats: effects on locomotor activity and spatiallearningand memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 1998. 69(1): p. 22-45. Chen, F. and A.J. Lawrence, Effect of chronic ethanol and withdrawalon the muopioidreceptor-and 5-Hydroxytryptamine(A) receptor-stimulatedbinding of [(35)S]Guanosine-5'-O-(3-thio)triphosphatein the fawn-hooded rat brain:A quantitative autoradiographystudy. J Pharmacol Exp Ther, 2000. 293(1): p. 15965. Chozick, B.S., The behavioraleffects of lesions ofthe septum: a review. Int J Neurosci, 1985. 26(3-4): p. 197-217. Chudasama, Y. and J.L. Muir, A behaviouralanalysis of the delayed nonmatching to position task: the effects of scopolamine, lesions ofthe fornix andof the prelimbic region on mediating behaviours by rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 1997. 134(1): p. 73-82. Clark, J.D., HistoricalPerspectivesand Taxonomy. Laboratory Hamsters, 1987. pp. 3-10. G.L.V Hossier and C.W. McPherson (Eds.). Academic Press, Boston. Clemens, L.G. and Witcher, J.A., Sexual Differentiationand Development. The Hamster: Reproduction and Behavior, 1985. pp. 3-19. H.I. Siegel (Ed.). Plenum Press, New York. Collier, G.H., and Rovee-Collier, C.K., A comparativeanalysis ofoptimal foraging behavior: Laboratorysimulation. Foraging Behavior, 1981. pp. 39-76. A.C. Kamil and T.D. Sargent (Eds.). Garland STPM Press, New York. 59 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. Coolen, L.M. and R.I. Wood, Bidirectionalconnections of the medial amygdaloid nucleus in the Syrian hamster brain: simultaneous anterogradeand retrograde tract tracing.J Comp Neurol, 1998. 399(2): p. 189-209. Coutureau, E., et al., Selective lesions of the entorhinalcortex, the hippocampus, or thefimbria-fornix in rats: a comparison of effects on spontaneous and amphetamine-inducedlocomotion. Exp Brain Res, 2000. 131(3): p. 381-92. Dalrymple-Alford, J.C., Behavioraleffects of basalforebraingrafts after dorsal septo- hippocampalpathway lesions. Brain Res, 1994. 661(1-2): p. 243-58. Day, C.S.D. and Galef, B.G., Jr., Pup cannibalism: One aspect of maternal behavior in golden hamsters. J. Comp. Psychol, 1977. 91: p. 1179-1189. Decker, M.W., P. Curzon, and J.D. Brioni, Influence ofseparate and combined septal and amygdala lesions on memory, acoustic startle, anxiety, and locomotor activity in rats.Neurobiol Learn Mem, 1995. 64(2): p. 156-68. Delbende, C., et al., [Role of alpha-MSHand relatedpeptides in the central nervous system]. Rev Neurol, 1985. 141(6-7): p. 429-39. Devinsky, 0., M.J. Morrell, and B.A. Vogt, Contributionsofanteriorcingulate cortex to behaviour.Brain, 1995. 118(Pt 1): p. 279-306. Devor, M. and G.E. Schneider, Attraction to home-cage odor in hamsterpups: specificity andchanges with age. Behav Biol, 1974. 10(2): p. 211-21. Dieterlen, F., Das Verhalten des syrischen Goldhamsters (Mesocricetusauratus Waterhouse). Z. Tierpsychol., 1959. 16: p. 47-103. Duncan, G.E., D.J. Knapp, and G.R. Breese, Neuroanatomicalcharacterizationof Fos induction in rat behavioralmodels ofanxiety. Brain Res, 1996. 713(1-2): p. 79-91. Ennaceur, A. and J.P. Aggleton, Spontaneous recognition ofobject configurations in rats: effects offornix lesions. Exp Brain Res, 1994. 100(1): p. 85-92. Ennaceur, A., N. Neave, and J.P. Aggleton, Spontaneous object recognition and object location memory in rats: the effects of lesions in the cingulate cortices,the medialprefrontal cortex, the cingulum bundle and the fornix. Exp Brain Res, 1997. 113(3): p. 509-19. Flynn, F.W., et al., The relation offeeding and activityfollowing septal lesions in rats. Behav Neurosci, 1986. 100(3): p. 416-21. Francis, D.D., F.C. Champagne, and M.J. Meaney, Variationsin maternal behaviourare associatedwith differences in oxytocin receptorlevels in the rat. J Neuroendocrinol, 2000. 12(12): p. 1145-8. Freeman, J.H., Jr., et al., Lesions of the entorhinalcortex disrupt behavioraland neuronal responses to context change during extinction of discriminative avoidance behavior. Exp Brain Res, 1997. 115(3): p. 445-57. Freo, U., et al., Dose-dependent effects of buspirone on behavior and cerebral glucose metabolism in rats. Brain Res, 1995. 677(2): p. 213-20. Frost, P., et al., Biodynamic studies of hamsterflank organgrowth: hormonal influences. J Invest Dermatol, 1973. 61(3): p. 159-67. Fulton, G.P., The Golden Hamster in Biomedical Research. The Golden Hamster its biology and use in medical research, 1968. pp.3-13. R.A. Hoffman, P.F. Robinson, and H. Magalhaes (Eds.). The Iowa State University Press, Ames, Iowa. 60 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 84. 85. 86. Gabriel, M., et al., Mamillothalamictract transection blocks anteriorthalamic training-induced neuronalplasticityand impairsdiscriminativeoffidance behavior in rabbits. J Neurosci, 1995. 15(2): p. 1437-45. Gaffan, D. and A. Parker, Interactionofperirhinalcortex with thefornix-fimbria: memory for objects and "object-in-place"memory. J Neurosci, 1996. 16(18): p. 5864-9. Gaffan, E.A. and M.J. Eacott, Spatial memory impairment in rats with fornix transectionis not accompaniedby a simple encoding deficitfor directions of objects in visual space. Behav Neurosci, 1997. 111(5): p. 937-54. Garofalo, P., et al., CR 2249: a new putative memory enhancer.Behavioural studies on learningand memory in rats and mice. J Pharm Pharmacol, 1996. 48(12): p. 1290-7. Garritano, J., et al., The output of the hippocampus is inhibited duringsocial behavior in the male rat.Exp Brain Res, 1996. 111(1): p. 35-40. Georges, F. and G. Aston-Jones, Potentregulation of midbrain dopamine neurons by the bed nucleus ofthe stria terminalis.J Neurosci, 2001. 21(16): p. RC160. Gluck, M.A. and C.E, Myers, Integratingbehavioralandphysiologicalmodels of hippocampalfunction.Hippocampus, 1996. 6(6): p. 643-53. Goldman, L. and H. Swanson, Populationcontrol in confined colonies ofgolden hamsters (Mesocricetusauratus Waterhouse). Z Tierpsychol, 1975. 37(3): p. 22536. Goldman, L. and H.H. Swanson, Developmental changes in pre-adultbehavior in confined colonies ofgolden hamsters. Dev Psychobiol, 1975. 8(2): p. 137-50. Granneman, J.G. and G.N. Wade, Effects ofphotoperiodand castrationon postfastfood intake and body weight gain in golden hamsters.Physiol Behav, 1982. 28(5): p. 847-50. Gray, T.S. and J.E. Morley, Neuropeptide Y: anatomicaldistributionandpossible function in mammalian nervous system. Life Sci, 1986. 38(5): p. 389-401. Greenough, W.T., et al., Sex differences in dentriticpatterns in hamsterpreoptic area.Brain Res, 1977. 126(1): p. 63-72. Harper, D.N., A.P. McLean, and J.C. Dalrymple-Alford, Forgettingin rats following medial septum or mammillary body damage. Behav Neurosci, 1994. 108(4): p. 691-702. Hart, M., A. Poremba, and M. Gabriel, The nomadic engram: overtraining eliminates the impairment of discriminativeavoidance behaviorproduced by limbic thalamic lesions. Behav Brain Res, 1997. 82(2): p. 169-77. Hofferer, E., et al., A comparisonof behaviouraleffects and morphological features of grafts rich in cholinergicneurons placed in two sites ofthe denervated rat hippocampus.Exp Brain Res, 1996. 111(2): p. 187-207. Hoffman, R.A., Robinson, P.F., and Magalhaes, H. (Eds.), The Golden Hamster its biology and use in medical research. 1968. The Iowa State University Press, Ames, Iowa. Hoffman, R.A., K. Davidson, and K. Steinberg, Influence ofphotoperiodand temperatureon weight gain,food consumption,fat pads and thyroxine in male golden hamsters. Growth, 1982. 46(2): p. 150-62. 61 87. 88. 89. 90. 91. 92. 93. 94. 95. 96. 97. 98. 99. 100. 101. 102. 103. 104. 105. Honacki, J., Kinman, K., and Koeppl, J., Mammal Species of the World. 1982. Allen Press, Inc. and The Assoc. of Systematic Collections, Lawrence, Kansas. Homer, P.J. and F.H. Gage, Regeneratingthe damagedcentral nervous system. Nature, 2000. 407(6807): p. 963-70. Insel, T.R. and L.E. Shapiro, Oxytocin receptordistributionreflects social organizationin monogamous andpolygamous voles. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 1992. 89(13): p. 5981-5. Irvin, R.W., et al., Vasopressin in the septal areaof the golden hamster controls scent marking and grooming. Physiol Behav, 1990. 48(5): p. 693-9. Janis, L.S., T.W. Bishop, and G.L. Dunbar, Medial septal lesions in ratsproduce permanent deficitsfor strategy selection in a spatialmemory task Behav Neurosci, 1994. 108(5): p. 892-8. Janzen, W.B. and B.N. Bunnell, Septal lesions and the recovery offunction in the juvenile hamster. Physiol Behav, 1976. 16(4): p. 445-52. Jeltsch, H., et al., The effects of intrahippocampalraphe and/orseptal grafts in rats withfimbria-fornixlesions depend on the origin ofthe grafted tissue andthe behaviouraltask used Neuroscience, 1994. 63(1): p. 19-39. Johnston, R.E., Scent marking by male Golden Hamsters (Mesocricetusauratus) III. Behavior in a seminaturalenvironment. Z Tierpsychol, 1975. 37(2): p. 21321. Johnston, R.E., Scent marking by male golden hamsters (Mesocricetusauratus). II The role of the flank gland scent in the causation of marking. Z Tierpsychol, 1975. 37(2): p. 138-44. Johnston, R.E., Scent marking by male golden hamsters (Mesocricetusauratus)I. Effects ofodors and social encounters. Z Tierpsychol, 1975. 37(1): p. 75-98. Johnston, R.E. and T. Schmidt, Responses ofhamsters to scent marks ofdifferent ages. Behav Neural Biol, 1979. 26(1): p. 64-75. Johnston, R.E., Communication. The Hamster: Reproduction and Behavior, 1985. pp. 3-19. H.I. Siegel (Ed.). Plenum Press, New York. Kalil, K. and G.E. Schneider, Retrograde corticalaandaxonal changesfollowing lesions ofthe pyramidal tract. Brain Res, 1975. 89(1): p. 15-27. Kelsey, J.E. and S.P. Grossman, Influence of central cholinergicpathways on performance on free- operantavoidance and DRL schedules. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 1975. 3(6): p. 1043-50. Kelsey, J.E. and H. Vargas, Medial septal lesions disruptspatial,but not nonspatial,working memory in rats. Behav Neurosci, 1993. 107(4): p. 565-74. Kempermann, G. and F.H. Gage, New nerve cellsfor the adult brain. Sci Am, 1999. 280(5): p. 48-53. Kirkpatrick, B., J.W. Kim, and T.R. Insel, Limbic systemfos expression associatedwith paternalbehavior. Brain Res, 1994. 658(1-2): p. 112-8. Kordower, J.H. and M.S. Fiandaca, Response ofthe monkey cholinergic septohippocampalsystem tofornix transection: a histochemicaland cytochemical analysis.J Comp Neurol, 1990.298(4): p. 443-57. Kornecook, T.J., T.E. Kippin, and J.P. Pinel, Basalforebraindamage and objectrecognitionin rats. Behav Brain Res, 1999. 98(1): p. 67-76. 62 106. 107. 108. 109. 110. 111. 112. 113. 114. 115. 116. 117. 118. 119. 120. 121. Kumar, S. and S.K. Kulkarni, Influence of antidepressantdrugs on learningand memory paradigmsin mice. Indian J Exp Biol, 1996. 34(5): p. 431-5. Kyrkouli, S.E., B.G. Stanley, and S.F. Leibowitz, Bombesin-inducedanorexia: sites ofaction in the rat brain.Peptides, 1987. 8(2): p. 237-41. Laurent-Demir, C. and R. Jaffard, Paradoxicalfacilitatoryeffect offornix lesions on acquisitionof contextualfear conditioningin mice. Behav Brain Res, 2000. 107(1-2): p. 85-91. Le Moal, M. and B. Cardo, Rhythmic slow wave activity recorded in the ventral mesencephalic tegmentum in the rat.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 1975. 38(2): p. 139-47. Leutgeb, S. and S.J. Mizumori, Excitotoxic septal lesions result in spatialmemory deficits and alteredflexibilityof hippocampalsingle-unitrepresentations.J Neurosci, 1999. 19(15): p. 6661-72. Lonstein, J.S., et al., Forebrainexpression of c-fos due to active maternal behaviour in lactatingrats.Neuroscience, 1998. 82(1): p. 267-8 1. Lowndes, M. and D.C. Davies, The effect ofarchistriatallesions on 'openfield' andfear/avoidancebehaviour in the domestic chick. Behav Brain Res, 1995. 72(1-2): p. 25-32. M'Harzi, M., et al., Effects ofselective lesions offimbria-fornix on learningset in the rat. Physiol Behav, 1987. 40(2): p. 181-8. M'Harzi, M. and L.E. Jarrard, Strategyselection in a task with spatialand nonspatialcomponents: effects offimbria-fornix lesions in rats. Behav Neural Biol, 1992. 58(3): p. 171-9. Maeda, N., N. Matsuoka, and I. Yamaguchi, Septohippocampalcholinergic pathway andpenile erections induced by dopaminergicand cholinergic stimulants. Brain Res, 1990. 537(1-2): p. 163-8. Maeda, N., N. Matsuoka, and I. Yamaguchi, Role ofthe dopaminergic, serotonergicand cholinergic link in the expression ofpenile erection in rats.Jpn J Pharmacol, 1994. 66(1): p. 59-66. Maeda, N., N. Matsuoka, and I. Yamaguchi, Possible involvement of the septohippocampalcholinergicand raphe-hippocampalserotonergicactivationsin the penile erection induced byfenfluramine in rats. Brain Res, 1994. 652(2): p. 1819. Maeda, N., et al., Involvement of raphe-hippocampalserotonergicand septohippocampalcholinergicmechanisms in the penile erection induced by FR121196, aputative cognitive enhancer.Jpn J Pharmacol, 1995. 68(1): p. 85-94. Malan, A. and G. Hildwein, [Thermoregulationin ambiant heat ofa hibernant, the European hamster (Cricetuscricetus). Comparisonwith the white rat].Arch Sci Physiol, 1969. 23(2): p. 153-81. Malsbury, C.W., L.M. Kow, and D.W. Pfaff, Effects ofmedial hypothalamic lesions on the lordosis response and other behaviors in remale golden hamsters. Physiol Behav, 1977. 19(2): p. 223-37. Malsbury, C.W., D. Strull, and J. Daood, Half-cylinder cuts antero-lateralto the ventromedialnucleus reduce sexual receptivity in female golden hamsters. Physiol Behav, 1978. 21(1): p. 79-87. 63 122. 123. 124. 125. 126. 127. 128. 129. 130. 131. 132. 133. 134. 135. 136. 137. 138. 139. 140. Malsbury, C.W., D.M. Marques, and J.T. Daood, Sagittal knife cuts in the farlateralhypothalamus reduce sexual receptivity infemale hamsters. Brain Res Bull, 1979. 4(6): p. 833-42. Maren, S. and M.S. Fanselow, Electrolytic lesions ofthe fimbria/fornix, dorsal hippocampus, or entorhinalcortex produce anterogradedeficits in contextual fear conditioningin rats.Neurobiol Learn Mem, 1997. 67(2): p. 142-9. Maren, S., Long-term potentiationin the amygdala: a mechanismfor emotional learningandmemory. Trends Neurosci, 1999. 22(12): p. 561-7. McHugh, T.J., Blum K.I., Tsien, J.Z., Tonegawa, S., and Wilson, M.A., Impaired hippocampalrepresentationof space in CAI-specific NMDARJ knockout mice. Cell, 1996. 87(7): p. 1147-8. Mehta, M.R., Quirk, M.C., and Wilson, M.A., Experience-dependentasymmetric shape of hippocampalreceptivefields. Neuron, 2000. 25(3): p. 504-6. Mesulam, M., Brain, mind, and the evolution of connectivity. Brain Cogn, 2000. 42(1): p. 4-6. Morin, L.P., BiologicalRhythms. The Hamster: Reproduction and Behavior, 1985. pp. 3-19. H.I. Siegel (Ed.). Plenum Press, New York. Morin, L.P. and Wood, R.I. A StereotaxicAtlas of The Golden Hamster Brain. 2000. Academic Press, Boston. Murphy, M.R., History of the Captureand Domesticationof the Syrian Golden Hamster (Mesocricetusauratus Waterhouse). The Hamster: Reproduction and Behavior, 1985. pp. 3-19. H.I. Siegel (Ed.). Plenum Press, New York. Nauta, W.J.H. and Feirtag, M., Anatomical Divisions. Fundamental Neuroanatomy, 1986. pp. 39-49. Freeman, New York. Nauta, W.J.H. and Feirtag, M., Affect and Motivation; The Limbic System. Fundamental Neuroanatomy, 1986. pp. 120-131. Freeman, New York. Neill, D.B., J.F. Ross, and S.P. Grossman, Comparison ofthe effects offrontal, striatal,and septal lesions in paradigms thought to measure incentive motivation or behavioral inhibition.Physiol Behav, 1974. 13(2): p. 297-305. Northmore, D.P., Levine, E.S., and G.E. Schneider, Behavior evoked by electrical stimulation ofthe hamster superiorcolliculus. Exp Brain Res, 1988. 73(3): p. 595-605. Nosofsky, R.M., Choice, similarity, and the context theory of classification.J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 1984. 10(1): p. 104-14. Nowak, R.M., and Paradiso, J.L., Walker's Mammals ofthe World 4* ed. 1983. Johns Hopkins Univ. Press, Baltimore, Maryland. Osborne, B. and A.B. Dodek, Disruptedpatternsofconsummatory behavior in rats withfornix transections.Behav Neural Biol, 1986. 45(2): p. 212-22. Osborne, B. and L.A. Flashman, Mealpatternsfollowingchanges in procurement costfor rats with fornix transection.Behav Neural Biol, 1986. 46(2): p. 123-36. Peinado-Manzano, M.A., The role of the amygdala and the hippocampusin working memory for spatialand non-spatialinformation. Behav Brain Res, 1990. 38(2): p. 117-34. Piazza, P.V. and M. Le Moal, The role ofstress in drug self-administration. Trends Pharmacol Sci, 1998. 19(2): p. 67-74. 64 141. 142. 143. 144. 145. 146. 147. 148. 149. 150. 151. 152. 153. 154. 155. 156. 157. 158. Polsky, R.H., Conspecific defeat, isolation/grouping,andpredatorybehavior in golden hamsters. Psychol Rep, 1976. 38(2): p. 571-7. Popik, P. and J.M. van Ree, Neurohypophysealpeptides and social recognitionin rats. Prog Brain Res, 1998. 119: p. 415-36. Poplawsky, A. and R.L. Isaacson, Changes in open-field behaviorsfollowing septal lesions in rats.Behav Neural Biol, 1983. 38(1): p. 61-9. Poremba, A. and M. Gabriel, Amygdala neurons mediate acquisitionbut not maintenanceof instrumentalavoidance behavior in rabbits.J Neurosci, 1999. 19(21): p. 9635-41. Poucet, B. and T. Herrmann, Septum and medialfrontal cortex contributionto spatialproblem- solving. Behav Brain Res, 1990. 37(3): p. 269-80. Pouzet, B., et al., The effects ofradiofrequency lesion or transectionof the fimbria-fornix on latent inhibition in the rat.Neuroscience, 1999. 91(4): p. 135568. Purves, D., Augustine, G.J., Fitzpatrick, D., Katz, L.C., LaMantia, A.S., McNamara, J.O., Ed., Neuroscience. 1997. Sinauer Associates, Inc., Sunderland, MA. . Quillfeldt, J., et al., Biochemical and behavioraleffects of intraseptal microinjectionoffasciculin, an irreversibleacetylcholinesteraseinhibitor. Braz J Med Biol Res, 1991. 24(5): p. 499-507. Rawlins, J.N., T.J. Maxwell, and J.D. Sinden, The effects offornix section on winstay/lose-shift andwin-shift/lose- stay performance in the rat. Behav Brain Res, 1988. 31(1): p. 17-28. Richards, M.P.M., Effects of estrogen andprogesteroneon nest buildingin the golden hamster. Anim. Behav, 1969. 17: p. 356-361. Richards, M.P.M., Activity measured by running wheels and observationduring the estrous cycle, pregnancy, andpseudopregnancy in the golden hamster. Anim. Behav, 1966. 14: p. 450-458. Roeling, T.A., et al., Efferent connections of the hypothalamic "groomingarea"in the rat. Neuroscience, 1993. 56(1): p. 199-225. Roeling, T.A., et al., Efferent connections ofthe hypothalamic "aggressionarea" in the rat.Neuroscience, 1994. 59(4): p. 1001-24. Roeser, C., J.C. Cassel, and C. Kelche, Behavioraleffects ofGM] ganglioside treatment and intrahippocampalseptal grafts in rats withfimbria-fornix lesions. Exp Brain Res, 1997. 115(3): p. 520-30. Ross, J.F. and S.P. Grossman, Septal influences on operantresponding in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol, 1975. 89(6): p. 523-36. Rowland, N., Failureby deprivedhamsters to increasefood intake: some behavioralandphysiologicaldeterminants.J Comp Physiol Psychol, 1982. 96(4): p. 591-603. Rowland, N., Physiologicaland behavioralresponses to glucoprivationin the golden hamster.Physiol Behav, 1983. 30(5): p. 743-7. Sandner, G., et al., What brain structuresare active during emotions? Effects of brain stimulation elicitedaversion on c-fos immunoreactivityand behavior. Behav Brain Res, 1993. 58(1-2): p. 9-18. 65 159. 160. 161. 162. 163. 164. 165. 166. 167. 168. 169. 170. 171. 172. 173. 174. 175. 176. 177. Scheff, S.W., et al., Morris water maze deficits in ratsfollowingtraumaticbrain injury: lateralcontrolledcortical impact. J Neurotrauma, 1997. 14(9): p. 615-27. Schmajuk, N.A. and J.J. DiCarlo, Stimulus configuration,classicalconditioning, and hippocampalfunction.Psychol Rev, 1992. 99(2): p. 268-305. Schneider, G.E., Contrastingvisuomotorfunctions of tectum and cortex in the golden hamster. Psychol Forsch, 1967.31(1): p. 52-62. Schneider, G.E., Two visual systems. Science, 1969. 163(870): p. 895-902. Schneider, G.E. (Unpublished). Four hours in the life of a Syrian Hamster. Cambridge, MA. Schoenfeld, T.A. and Leonard, C.M., BehavioralDevelopment in the Syrian Golden Hamster.The Hamster: Reproduction and Behavior, 1985. pp. 3-19. H.I. Siegel (Ed.). Plenum Press, New York. Schwarting, R.K. and J.P. Huston, Behavioralconcomitantsofregional changes in the brain's biogenic amines after apomorphineand amphetamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 1992. 41(4): p. 675-82. Siegel, H.I., Aggressive Behavior. The Hamster: Reproduction and Behavior, 1985. pp. 3-19. H.I. Siegel (Ed.). Plenum Press, New York. Siegel, H.I., ParentalBehavior. The Hamster: Reproduction and Behavior, 1985. pp. 3-19. H.I. Siegel (Ed.). Plenum Press, New York. Sodetz, F.J. and B.N. Bunnell, Septal ablation and the social behaviorofthe golden hamster.Physiol Behav, 1970. 5(1): p. 79-88. Sutherland, R.J. and A.J. Rodriguez, The role ofthefornix/fimbriaand some relatedsubcorticalstructures in place learningand memory. Behav Brain Res, 1989. 32(3): p. 265-77. Swanson, L.W., The anatomicalorganizationof septo-hippocampalprojections. Ciba Found Symp, 1977. 58: p. 25-48. Swanson, L.W. and W.M. Cowan, An autoradiographicstudy ofthe organization of the efferent connections ofthe hippocampalformation in the rat. J Comp Neurol, 1977. 172(1): p. 49-84. Swanson, L.W. and R.W. Lind, Neuralprojectionssubserving the initiationofa specific motivated behavior in the rat: new projectionsfrom the subfornical organ. Brain Res, 1986. 379(2): p. 399-403. Taylor, G., et al., Adult ontogeny of rat working memory of social interactions.J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 1999. 54(3): p. M145-51. Tejani-Butt, S.M., W.P. Pare, and J. Yang, Effect ofrepeatednovel stressorson depressive behavior and brain norepinephrinereceptorsystem in SpragueDawley and Wistar Kyoto (WKY) rats. Brain Res, 1994. 649(1-2): p. 27-35. Thiel, C.M., J.P. Huston, and R.K. Schwarting, Hippocampalacetylcholine and habituationlearning.Neuroscience, 1998. 85(4): p. 1253-62. Thinus-Blanc, C., et al., The effects of reversible inactivationsofthe hippocampus on exploratoryactivity and spatialmemory. Hippocampus, 1991. 1(4): p. 365-71. Thomas, G.J. and P.S. Spafford, Deficitsfor representationalmemory inducedby septal and corticallesions (singly and combined) in rats. Behav Neurosci, 1984. 98(3): p. 394-404. 66 178. Thomas, G.J. and D.M. Gash, Differentialeffects ofposteriorseptal lesions on dispositionaland representationalmemory. Behav Neurosci, 1986. 100(5): p. 712-9. 179. Treit, D. and C. Pesold, Septal lesions inhibitfear reactionsin two animal models ofanxiolytic drug action. Physiol Behav, 1990. 47(2): p. 365-71. 180. Truong, D.D., et al., Glycine involvement in DDT-induced myoclonus. Mov Disord, 1988. 3(1): p. 77-87. 181. van Rijzingen, I.M., W.H. Gispen, and B.M. Spruijt, Postoperativeenvironmental enrichment attenuatesfimbria-fornixlesion- induced impairments in Morris maze performance.Neurobiol Learn Mem, 1997. 67(1): p. 21-8. 182. Vandenbergh, J.G., Effects ofgonadal hormones on theflank gland of the golden hamster. Horm. Res., 1973. 4: p. 28-33. 183. Vandenbergh, J.G., Reproductive coordinationin the golden hamster: Female influences on the male. Horm. Res., 1977. 9: p. 264-275. 184. Wade, G.N., Obesity without overeatingin golden hamsters. Physiol Behav, 1982. 29(4): p. 701-7. 185. Walker, J.A. and D.S. Olton, Spatial memory deficitfollowingfimbria-fornix lesions: independent oftime for stimulus processing.Physiol Behav, 1979. 23(1): p. 11-5. 186. Walker, J.A. and D.S. Olton, Fimbria-fornixlesions impair spatialworking memory but not cognitive mapping.Behav Neurosci, 1984. 98(2): p. 226-42. 187. Wang, Z., C.F. Ferris, and G.J. De Vries, Role ofseptal vasopressin innervation in paternalbehaviorin prairievoles (Microtusochrogaster).Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 1994. 91(1): p. 400-4. 188. Warburton, E.C. and J.P. Aggleton, Differentialdeficits in the Morris water maze following cytotoxic lesions of the anteriorthalamus andfornix transection.Behav Brain Res, 1999. 98(1): p. 27-38. 189. Weiner, I., et al., Fimbria-fornixcut affects spontaneousactivity, two-way avoidance and delayed non matching to sample, but not latent inhibition.Behav Brain Res, 1998. 96(1-2): p. 59-70. 190. Whishaw, I.Q., et al., "Short-stops" in rats withfimbria-fornix lesions: evidence for change in the mobility gradient.Hippocampus, 1994. 4(5): p. 577-82. 191. Whishaw, I.Q. and J.A. Tomie, Rats withfimbria-fornix lesions can acquire and retaina visual- tactile transwitching(configural)task Behav Neurosci, 1995. 109(4): p. 607-12. 192. Whishaw, I.Q. and J.A. Tomie, Pilotingand dead reckoning dissociatedby fimbria-fornix lesions in a ratfoodcarryingtask Behav Brain Res, 1997. 89(1 2): p. 87-97. 193. Wible, C.G., J.R. Shiber, and D.S. Olton, Hippocampus,fimbria-fornix, amygdala, and memory: object discriminationsin rats.Behav Neurosci, 1992. 106(5): p. 751-61. 194. Wiig, K.A., L.N. Cooper, and M.F. Bear, Temporally gradedretrogradeamnesia following separateand combined lesions of the perirhinalcortex andfornix in the rat. Learn Mem, 1996. 3(4): p. 313-25. 67 195. 196. 197. 198. 199. 200. 201. 202. Wilensky, A.E., G.E. Schafe, and J.E. LeDoux, The amygdala modulates memory consolidationoffear-motivated inhibitoryavoidance learningbut not classical fear conditioning.J Neurosci, 2000. 20(18): p. 7059-66. Wilson, M.A. and Tonegawa, S., Synapticplasticity,place cells and spatial memory: study with second generationknockouts. Trends Neurosci, 1997. 20(3): p. 102-6. Yamazaki, M., et al., FK960 N-(4-acetyl-J-piperazinyl)-p-fluorobenzamide monohydrate amelioratesthe memory deficits in rats through a novel mechanism of action. J Pharmacol Exp Ther, 1996. 279(3): p. 1157-73. Yerganian, G., History and cytogenetics ofhamsters. Prog Exp Tumor Res, 1972. 16: p. 2-34. Yoganarasimha, D. and B.L. Meti, Amelioration offornix lesion induced learning deficits by self-stimulation rewardingexperience. Brain Res, 1999. 845(2): p. 246-51. Zafar, H.M., W.P. Pare, and S.M. Tejani-Butt, Effect ofacute or repeatedstress on behaviorand brainnorepinephrinesystem in Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rats. Brain Res Bull, 1997. 44(3): p. 289-95. Zucker, I., G.N. Wade, and R. Ziegler, Sexual and hormonal influences on eating, taste preferences,and body weight ofhamsters. Physiol Behav, 1972. 8(1): p. 101-11. Zucker, I. and F.K. Stephan, Light-dark rhythms in hamster eating, drinking and locomotor behaviors. Physiol Behav, 1973. 11(2): p. 239-50. 68