Methods of Frequency Selectivityand Synchronization Measurement in Single Auditory Nerve Fibers: Application to the Alligator Lizard by Christopher Rose B.S.E.E. Massachusetts Institute of Technology (1979) M.S.E.E. Massachusetts Institute of Technology (1981) SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY at the MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY May, 1985 ) Christopher Rose, 1985 Signature of Author %- -q- Department of Electrical Engineering & Computer Science May 17, 1985 Certified by Prof.Thomas F. Weiss ThesjSpervisor __ Accepted by Archives MASSACiUSETTS iNST' E OF TECHNOLOGY JUL 1 9 1985 IDACir.' ___ Prof. Arthur C. Smith Chairman, Department Committee METHODS OF FREQUENCY SE CIICVITYMEASUREMENT IN SINGLE AUDITORY NERVE FIBERS: APPLICATION TO THE ALLIGATOR LIZARD by CHRISTOPHER ROSE Submitted to the Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science on May 17, 1985 in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Electrical Enginee ABSTRACT This work is composed of two separate studies: 1) an analysis of an iso-response contour estimation algorithm used in auditory physiological studies and 2) the dependence of neural synchronization upon frequency. The first study is an in-depth analysis of an automated algorithm used by many auditory physiologists to measure the frequency selectivity of auditory neurons (Kiang, Moxon & Levine, 1970). Specifically, we use a Markov model to specify the behavior of the algorithm. We then compare the properties of the model to the properties exhibited by the automated algorithm with auditory neurons. We present performance bounds as well as suggestions for improving the effectiveness of this algorithm. In the second study we measure neural synchronization decline with increasing frequency and make comparisons to a previously reported study of hair cell receptor potential (Holton & Weiss, 1983a). We show that the process which relates hair cell receptor potential and the neural response is low-pass in nature. We also show that the frequency at which neural synchronization begins to decline in a given fiber does not depend upon the characteristic frequency of the fiber. In addition, we present two new methods to study neural .synchronization. The first method allows the study of synchronized comanponentvariation with frequency separate from the frequency variation of the average firing rate response; we measure synchronization at constant average rate. The second method uses a new synchronization estimation technique which allows the rapid measurement of iso-synchronization (iso-fundamental) contours in much the same way that iso-rate contours are presently measured. THESIS SUPERVISOR: Thomas F. Weiss TTILE: Professor of Electrical Engineering -2- ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS My three year stay at the Eaton-Peabody Laboratory has been a pleasant one. For this I have many people to thank. Ruth Anne Eatock brought a sunny biologists perspective into this engineer's professional life (as of this writing a tadpole sits metamorphosing in a tank on her desk). Denny Freeman, although he may not realize it, was a very important peer for an analytic type like myself. He was one of the few people to whom I could say "Bessel Function" without fear of his fainting. Discussions with Denny about the "wall", "rod" and numerical solutions for problems in fluid dynamics were a lot of fun (I still think you should build a scale model in a viscous fluid, Denny!). John Rosowski was always available to answer the basic questions; John where's the power switch? John, how does the sweep system work? John, do snakes have cars? John, what three rivers flow by Three River Stadium? I will miss John's humor and warmth. Chris Brown provided a much needed wildness outlet in an otherwise staid atmosphere (MAKE 'EM SCREAM! MAKE 'EM SHOUT!) in addition to very good-naturedly introducing me to the guinea pig ear in the middle of his experiments. Ca Williams performed several excellent lizard surgeries for me as well as making sure I did not forget the simpler things like personal hygiene after crawling under a desk in search of an escaped lizard (Chris, do you realize that you have dust all over one side of your big head?). The engineering staff, Ishmael Stefanov-Wagner, Mark Curby and Bob Brown provided continual technical support. It would have been a very hard row to hoe without them. For help with the preparation of this document I must thank my two thesis readers, Larry Frishkopf and Bill Peake. The speed and accuracy with which they reviewed this mathematically dense manuscript on short notice is amazing. They have both greatly improved the quality of this work through their suggestions and coments. M.I.T. Extra thanks, however, is due to Bill Peake who served as a patron saint during my stay at I am greatly indebted to him for his counsel during my early wide-eyed-young-graduate-student years. More than just thanks are due to the two men who had a major influence upon this study. Without Dave Strffens, none of the work on synchronization or the new and improved TCA 3 would exist. Dave wrote all the software (in assembly language!) usually within two days of my initial request and sometimes -3- before I even asked. He was even able to program around one of my hardware blunders. (Rose, the hardware has to dear the ready flag!). Dave was also always available for help on any computer related topic and he always got the message across (even to the unix-uninitiated and the C-unseasoned). I greatly appreciate his help. Tom Weiss, my thesis supervisor, has provided continual guidance and support. When I thought I had all my ducks in a row he would invariably find the stragglers which I had missed. His contribution to this work is immense. Tom has made my professional development enjoyable. Incredibly enough, he managed to do all this without overpowering me. He even managed to avoid laughing outright when I would come to him in the early going of this work with a particularly outlandish idea. He'd simply bite his tongue and let me come to my senses later (Rose, let me get this straight. You want to record from the auditory nerve in the alligator lizard using a multichannel electrode which you will fabricate on a semiconductor wafer? By next week?). Tom, you will be happy to know that although I have resisted your every attempt to transform me into a died-in-the-wool auditory physiologist you have in some sense succeeded. In the noble tradition of Alexander Graham Bell who was initially an audiologist and Bekesy who was initially a telecommunications engineer, I will be working for the telephone company. Very special mention must go to Stephanie and Stephen (Boomer) Rose who made it all worthwhile. Stephanie, a Harvard-trained attorney, never bargained on becoming a hausfrau and having to cook, dean and care for two babies during the last few months of my graduate studies. She has come through it all with very good humor (Chris, it's five in the morning and those dicks and beeps are driving me crazy. Log out or die!). Stephanie and Stephen were a joy to come home to, and on those many late nights at home in front of the computer terminal it was greatly reassuring to know they were there with me. In fact, Stephen, an occasion stayed up to give me moral support. I therefore thought it only fitting that he should write the dosing comments to these acknowledgements. c p.g foc mmbvc,/.k bf p,. off?: polmn l,m&%6 r: b/w=p ;,. ,cd .n, gv cd 9 hb 976mn hg 89, m! ,> I xki z? . Look at all those numbers and symbols! Already a flair for the technical! 4- The Boomer Seal of Approval C-a ~ %I''7At g'LL, . -5- TABLE OF CONTENTS TitlePage. .......................................................... . ..................................................... I Abstract ............................................................................. 2 Acknowledgements ... 3 Seal of Approval. Table of Contents . ..................................................................................................... ............................................ S .............................................................. .................................................................................................... 6 10 List of Figures............................................................................................................ List of Tables ............................................................................................................. CHAPTER 1: ntroduction ............................................................ 14 CHAPTER 2: ISO-RESPONSE CONTOUR ESTIMATION An Analysis of the Tuning Curve Algorithm ............................................................ 16 INTRODUCTION ............................................................ 16 ISO-RESPONSE AND ISO-STIMULUS METHODS IN THE MEASUREMENT OF FREQUENCY RESPONSE CHARACTERISTICS........................................................... 16 FORMALIZATION OF A BASIC IRC ESTIMATION PROCEDURE ........................................ 17 PREVIOUS RIGOROUS STUDIES OF IRC ESTI77MATION ALGORITHMS: to Measurements on Cochlear Neurons ......................................... Inapplicability 18 TCA HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE ...... 19 ... ................................................................. TOOLS USED TO ASSESS TCA PERFORMANCE SIMULI ...................................... ...... ........................................................ ... NEURAL RESPONSE DEFINITIONS ......... ................................................................. ............................................................ TEST CIRCUIT....................................................................................................... ..... 24 .................................25 STEADY STATE BEHAVIOR OF THE MODEL ................................................................ Theoretical Considerations ..................................................... ......... Derivationf SteadyStateEquations .. +6 21 ............................24 ............................................................. MODEL FORMULATION ..................................................... 21 22 A MARKOV MODEL OF TCA BEHAVIOR: Theory and Experiment ..... A DESCRIPTION OF THE TCA ......... 21 ........................... 27 27 ................ 27 TheAverageLevelsE(l) andE( ) ........................................... 30 NumericalDetrminations ofp (l) and p, ( Isuccess)................................................... The TCA Does NOT NecessarilySatisfya Rate Criterion............................................ 33 34 Experiments to Examine TCA behavior in the Steady State ........................................... 36 PredictedValuesof E (I) andE (I,) Using Measurements of g (1). ........................................................... Measurementprocedure .. Measurementson the test ircu ............. ... .. it ........................................ 36 ....... ............36 37 Measurements on cochlear neurons ................................... .................................. Variation of AA with Spontaneous Rate, X0 ............................................................... Prediction of AX vL X0 assuming a homogeneous Poisson spike generation proces . ...................................... 37 39 Measurementson the test crcuit ...................................................................... Measurementson codlear neurons ............. ....... ............... Variationof A(123) with Frequency....................................................................... 40 39 .40 Analytic description . . ................................. 4242 Measurements on codilear neuromns................................................................. 43 Summary of Steady State Properties ........................................................................ 43 TRANSIENT BEHAVIOR OF THE MODEL AND TCA IRC ESTIMATION ERROR ..................... Theoretical Considerations ........................................................... 44 44 Experiments to Examine TCA Transient Behavior ...................................................... 46 Topical Outline.................................................................................................. 46 TCA Errors in Estimating Neural IRCs .............................................................. Qualitative descriptions and simulations.............................47 47 trackingof a frequency-invariantIRC ................................................................ trackingof a linearIRC ................................................................................. trackingof a V-shapedIRC............................................................................. Measurement of TCA errors for cochlear neurons ........... 47 48 49 ......... .....50 50......... effects of initial startinglevel............ ............ ..................................... ... 50 TCA error and IRC slope....................................................................... 51 TCA IRC parameterestimationerror ........................................... 52 TCA Error Caused by Correctionfor the Acoustic System Frequency Characteristic ............ Qualitativedescription........................................................................ Measurementson codlear neurons .................. ................... ................ Miscellaneous Observations.... ........ .............. ........... TCA errors caused by nonmonotonic g(l) ................................................ measurements on a cochlear neuron ......... TCA errors caused by neural adaptation ............................................................... ......................................... measurements on cochlear neurons ..... SUM MARY . ......... ........ INTRODUCTION ........ .... ................................................... -7- 54 54.................................54 55 ............55.......... 55 ..... ........... .................................. 56 ................................................................................................... A CASE HISTORY OF TCA IRC ESTIMATION ERROR .......... 53 .............. 54 .........54 qualitativedescription ................................................................................... qualitativedescription..... 53 .... 53 ......... 57 ............................... ................................. 8 8..... OF RECEPTORPOTENTIALg () ..................................................... RECONSTRUCTION .. EFFECTSUPONREPORTEDRESULTS....... .................................... .............. IMPROVING TCA ACCURACY ........ .... MODIFICATION OF TCA PARAMETERS ..................... ........... 59 60 61 61 ................................................ COMBAT7NG TCA ERRORS CAUSED BY CORRECTION FOR THE ACOUSTIC SYSTEM FREQUENCY CHARACTERISTIC ...... ................................................. ........... 62 SUGGESTION FOR FURTHER STUDY ........................................................................ 64 .......................................................... Figures 65 CHAPTER 3: DEPENDENCE OF NEURAL SYNCHRONIZATION UPON FREQUENCY ...... 129 129 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................. METHODS ... STIMULI. .. ........................................................................................ 131 ... ................................................. 131 ....... . ............................................... 132 132 NEURAL RESPONSE COMPONENTS ......................................................................... Average Rate ..................................................................................................... ................................................................................. Synchronization ............. 132 Measurement of Iso-fundamental Contours .............................................................. RESULTS ............................................................................................................... ISO-FUNDAMENTAL CONTOURS: Dependence Upon Fiber CF ......................................... 134 135 135 SYNCHRONIZATION AT CONSTANT AVERAGE RATE DIFFERENCE: Dependenceupon CF for Low and High-CFFibers.................................................... 136 SYNCHRONIZATION AT CONSTANT AVERAGE RATE DIFFERENCE: ................................................................... Dependenceupon AX for High-CFFibers . DISCUSSION . . 137 .................................................139 NOVEL SYNCHRONIZATION MEASUREMENT SCHEMES ................................................ 139 RELATION OF NEURAL SYNCHRONIZATION TO RECEPTOR ........................................ POTENTIAL A.C. COMPONENT: Free-Standing Region 139 COMPARISON OF SYNCHRONIZATION FOR FREE-STANDING AND TECTORIAL FIBERS ..... 140 COMPARISON TO THE CAT .................... -8- 141................. 143 ................................................................................................................... Figures APPENDIX A: Existence of E(I) ................................................................................. 157 APPENDIX B: Estimates for Average Rate and Synchronization ........................................ 160 POISSONMODEL..... .. ............ .............................................. AVERAGE RATE ESTIMATE ...... ...... SYNCHRONIZATION ESTIMATE .. . . .. .............. 160 .................................................................. 160 ....... ..... .............................................. 161 Estimation of A.................................................................................................161 Estimation of 1/A0 ..................................................... ................................................................................ Synchrony estimate synthesis . 163 164 SYNCHRONIZATION MEASURES FOR IRC ESTIMATION ................................................ 165 APPENDIX C: Tests of Iso-fundamental Contour Estimation Algorithm Accuracy Using Single Cochlear Neurons in the Alligator Lizard ........................................ 167 Figure.................................................................................................................... 168 APPE.NDIX D: Justification of Comparisons between Synchronization Measured with Tone Burst Stimuli and Synchronization Measured with Continuous Tone Stimuli ....... ........................................................................................................... 169 AVERAGE RATE MEASUREMENTS ............................................................................ 169 SYNCHRONIZATION MEASUREMENTS ....................................................................... 170 Figures............................................................................................... REFERENCES.........................................................................................................174 BIOGRAPHY ............ ..................................................................... 177 -9- 171 LIST OF FIGURES 65 .......................................................................................... Figure 2.1: Cascaded system 66 .................................................................................. Figure 2.2: The toneburst stimulus Figure 2.3: Block diagram of test circuit ........................................................................... 67 Figure 2.4: Frequency response characteristic LTI filter in figure 2.3 ....................................... 68 Figure 2.5: Spike generation by threshold crossings of Gaussian noise 69 waveform...................................................................................................... Figure 2.6: PST histograms of toneburst response for test drcuit ............................................ 70 Figure 2.7: Schematic representation of the neural response to a 71 tone-burst..................................................................................................... Figure 2.8: Discrete-time discrete-state Markov models of the tuning curve algorithm at fixed frequency ..................................................................... Figure 2.9: Demonstration that E(l) and E () 72 ... 73 lie near 12,3 ...................................... Figure 2.10: TCA threshold history and g (l) for test circuit and nerve fiber ................................................................................................... 75 Figure 2.11: Off-window rate vs. spontaneous rate ................................. 77 Figure 2.12: Examination of probability/rate shift; g(l) vs. AX at different frequencies ............................................................................... 78 Figure 2.13: Expected value + s.d. of the first success level starting from level k for various values of Lm ..................................................... 82 Figure 2.14: Expected value ± s.d. of the first success level starting from level k using the function g (l) for various values of Lmx, gmin and g . ......................................................................... 85 Figure 2.15: Schematic and simulation of TCA error dependence upon IRC starting initial level for a frequency invariant IRC ................................................. 88 Figure 2.16: Schematic of TCA error dependence upon IRC slope . ..................... 90 ................... 92 Figure 2.17: Simulated average TCA error vs. normalized IRC slope ........... Figure 2.18: Schematic of TCA estimation error for V-shaped IRCs .................................... 96 Figure 2.19: Simulated low-high & high-low TCA estimates of neural IRC . ardchetypical .................................................................................. Figure 2.20: Three TCA IRC estimates for a cochlear never fiber for different initial starting levels .......................................................... -10- .......... 97 98 Figure 2.21: TCA estimate compasm to the TCA3 estimate in alligator lizard nerve fibers ................... ........ ........................ 99 Figure 2.22: Comparison of parameters measured from TCA estimates and TCA 3 esimates for cochlear neurons in the alligator l ard .............................................................................................. 102 Figure 2.23: Schematic of error caused by correction for the acoustic system frequency characteristic ................................................... 107 Figure 2.24: Simulation of artifact caused by correction for the acoustic system frequency characteristic.............................................................. 108 Figure 2.25: Error caused by acoustic system correction upon a TCA-estimated IRC in an alligator lizard cochlear neuron ..................................... 109 Figure 2.26: Schematic of a pathological nonmonotonic g (1)................................................ 110 Figure 2.27: Effects of nonmonotonic g(1) on neural IRC estimation.................................... 111 Figure 2.28: AX vs. level for suprathreshold and subthreshold stimuli ................................................... 112 Figure 2.29: Reconstructed hair-cell receptor potential g() ................................................. 114 Figure 2.30: Simulation of TCA estimation of iso-D.C. hair-cell receptor potential contours ........................................................................... 116 Figure 2.31: Expected value - s.d. of the second success level starting from level k for various values of Lnm ................................................... 117 Figure 2.32: Expected value -+ s.d. of the third success level starting from level k for various values of L. ................................................... 120 Figure 2.33: Expected value ± s.d. of the fifth success level starting from level k for various values of LmR= . 123 ....................................... Figure 2.34: Comparison of TCA and TCA3 performane .................................................. 126 Figure 3.1: The tone burst stimulus................................................... 143 Figure 3.2: Tone burst stimulus divided into M equal time intervals ...................................... 143 Figure 3.3: Instantaneous rate response of a cochlear nerve fiber in the alligator lizard to different frequency tones at 80 dB SPL....................................... 144 Figure 3.4: Iso-rate and Iso-synchrony contours for two low and two high CF fibrs ... .............................................. ............................................ Figure 3.5: Scatter plot of iso- A1 12 contour CF versus iso-rate contour CF for low and high CF units in 12 alligator lizards .................................................. Figure 3.6: Synchronization at constant rate (=20 sp/sec) for -11- 145 147 148 low- and high-CF units .................................................................................. Figure 3.7: Synchroizafion versus frequency for different constant Ax criterion ....................................... ............ .............. Figure 3.8: Comparison, for the free-standing region, of hair cell ........................................ receptor potential and neural instantaneous rate 151 153 Figure 3.9: Comparison of the dependence of neural syncrhonization upon frequency for cochlear nerve fibers in the alligator lzard and the cat. ..................................................... .................................... 156 Figure C.1: Measurement of All at points on an iso-fundamental contour . ..................................................... ...... 168 Figure D.1: Neural instantaneous rate response to a tone-burst ............................................. 171 Figure D.2: Average rate measured over the tone-burst on-window plotted against the average rate measured over the last 80% of the tone-burst on-window...................................................... Figure D.3: Neural rate response averaged over tone-burst on-window plotted against the response averaged over the final 80% of the tone-burst on-window ..................................................................................... -12- ............ 172 173 LIST OF TABLES Table 2.1: Success State History......................................................................................26 Table 2.2: TCA steady state threshold determination (800 trials/point) ..................................... 37 Table 2.3: TCA steady state threshold determination on auditory nerve fibers (14 units in two animals) (150 trials/point) ............................................. 38 Table 2.4: A? for Prob(N T =Tff -No I f >C)=2/3 for 50 ms ........................................................................................... 40 Table 2.5: Average rate difference, AX, vs. off-window rate, offI, measured at stimulus level, 12 3 for the test circuit.......................................................................................40 Table 2.6: 19 average rate differences, AX, vs. off-window rate, koff, measured at stimulus level, 23 for 14 units in two aligator liards ................................................................ -13- 42 CHAPTER 1: Introduction The present form of this thesis is the result of a scientific inquiry whose pursuit required the development of special techniques. Originally, this thesis was to be an experimental study of the neural firing rate response to sound stimuli. Both the average rate and synchronized responses were to be measured and the results were then to be compared with previous work on hair cell receptor potential. For the study of synchronized neural response, a specific experiment was formulated: measurement of neural synchronization as a function of frequency with average firing rate held constant. To perform this experiment it was envisioned that an automated algorithm would be used to hold average firing rate constant as a function of frequency. Measurement of such an "iso-rate contour" in the frequency-level plane using this algorithm would have been followed by measurements of synchronization at points along the iso-rate contour. It became immediately apparent, however, that the algorithm made errors in the estimation of iso-rate contours that rendered it inappropriate for our use. Thus began the analysis of the automated algorithm in order to ascertain how it made errors and how these errors might be corrected. The culmination of this analysis led to the large part of this work in Chapter 2 where we explore iso-response contour estimation. The study of iso-response contour estimation led to the development of a new means to study synchronization: measurement of isosynchronization contours. Use of this measurement technique forms a large part of Chapter 3 wherein we explore neural synchronization. Thus, what started as a strictly physiological thesis rapidly became an amalgam of technical development and physiological inquiry. Since this work addresses both technical issues and physiological issues it has been written in a modular manner so as to keep the two relatively separate. The two chapters of which this work is composed are independent of each other (except for a few tangential references). It was felt that this form would most benefit the reader; to understand the physiology one need not wade through the technical details and conversely, to understand the technical analysis one need not wade through the physiology. We now introduce each chapter in order to acquaint the reader with the salient features of this work. Chapter 2 is an in-depth analysis of an automated algorithm used in auditory physiology to measure the frequency selectivity of auditory neurons (Kiang, Moxon & Levine, 1970). Specifically, we use a Markov model to specify the behavior of the algorithm and compare the properties of the model to the properties -14- exhibited by the automated algorithm with auditory neurons. The type of model used is applicable to many sequential threshold estimation algorithms. We present performance bounds as well as suggestions for improving the effectiveness of this algorithm. In Chapter 3 we study the decline of neural synchronization with increasing frequency. Our results are compared to a previously reported study of hair cell receptor potential (Holton & Weiss, 1983a). We show that the process which relates hair cell receptor potential and the neural response is low-pass in nature. We also show that the frequency at which neural synchronization begins to decline in a given fiber does not depend upon the characteristic frequency of the fiber. In addition, we present two new methods to study neural synchronization. The first method allows the study of synchronized component variation with frequency separate from the frequency variation of the average firing rate response; we measure synchronization at constant average rate. The second method uses a new synchronization estimation technique which allows the rapid measurement of iso-synchronization (iso-fundamental) contours in much the same way that iso-rate contours are presently measured. -15- CHAPTER 2: ISO-RESPONSE CONTOUR ESTIMAilON An Analysis of the Tuning Cmw Algorithm INTRODUCTION The iso-response contour (IRC) measurement method is-biquitous in he fid of auditory physiology. This ubiquity stems from the utility of the IRC measurement method in frequency response estimation for simple systems which model properties of the response of codilear neurons to sound. We illustrate as fol- lows. ISO-RESPONSE AND ISO-STIMULUS METHODS IN THE MEASUREMENT OF FREQUENCY RESPONSE CHARACTERISTICS The frequency response of a linear time invariant (LTI) system (figure Z La.) is commonly obtained by applying a stimulus with constant amplitude, A, and measuring the respon. amplitude, B(o), angle, 4(w), as a function of radian frequency, . The transfer function, and phase (o), which has magnitude H (w) I=B (w)/A , and angle c(w), completely characerizes the relation of the stimulus to the response of an LTI system. Alternatively, H (w) could be measured by detennining the input amplitude A (w) necessary to produce a constant response amplitude B for each w. In this case H(w) =B/A (w). The angle of H(w) would also need to be measured to completely characterize the system. Fmr an LTI system the former iso -stimulus and the latter iso -response methods are equivalent. Now consider a system consisting of the LTI system of fure ity (figure 2.lb). 2.la fdloed Suppose we wish to measure the transfer fmction H(w). measurements of the peak-to-peak amplitude of the response Willnot measue nown function. The alternate iso-response measurem by a memoryless nonlinearith the iso-stimulus method, H(w)1 since F () is an unk- t, measurement cdf the stimulus amplitude A(w) required to keep the peak-to-peak amplitude of the response onstant wil allow H () to be determined to within a multiplicative constant since if the output of F () is constant then the input amplitude of F () is also constant (F () is assumed invertible.) Therefore the iso-response measurementof the cascade system is essentially an iso-response measurement of the LTI system since any miaation in A(gw)will be due to the LTI subsystem alone. Now suppose that F() saturates, i.e-, F(x)=fm, for g>t and F an F () arises naturally if the response has a limited dynamic m. -16- Altwmj =fm-'-,,j for x <a (b >a). Such F is invertible on (a ,b) it is not invertible outside of this range. Thus if the dynamic range of level for B (w) in figure 2.1b is greater than the range (b -a), then the output would be driven to saturation with an iso-stimulus measurement method. Using an iso-response method the value of F may be kept within its invertible range by fixing F =C where f min < C < fma thus avoidingsaturation. This issue of saturation is important in the study of the auditory system since the dynamic ranges of the neural response and of the sound stimulus in the physiological range of the auditory system differ markedly. The neural response saturates over a sound pressure level range of approximately 20 dB whereas the dynamic range of the frequency selective properties (the dynamic range in level of IH(w)1) may be as large as 80 dB in the alligator lizard (unpublished observations). Similar results may be inferred from data in other preparations (Kiang et al, 1965). Therefore, since we may crudely model the neural response to sound as the cascade of an LTI filter and a saturating memoryless nonlinearity, the usefulness of the iso-response method in measuring frequency selectivity of the neural response is readily apparent. As a final note consider the system depicted in figure 2.lc. It has been shown that both G(o) I and IH (X) can be estimated from iso-response measurements (Holton & Weiss 1983a). This particular system is of special interest since the transformation of sound pressure at the tympanic membrane to the receptor potential of hair cells in the alligator lizard can be approximated as such a cascade system. Specifically, middle and inner ear mechanics can be represented by H(w) and receptor potential generation from stereociliary displacement can be approximated by the combination of F(.) and G(w) (Weiss, Peake, & Rosowski (1985)). Since it is likely that a similar system model is applicable to higher vertebrate auditory systems as well, we that the measurement of IRCs is a generally convenient method of assessing the frequency selectivity of auditory response variables. FORMALIZATION OF A BASIC IRC ESTMA 7ON PROCEDURE We have defined an iso-response contour as the set of stimulus points (frequency, intensity) for which some auditory response variable (such as neural average rate) is held constant. To estimate such an IRC, a very simple procedure is used. For the purposes of illustration we will formalize this concept one stimulus variable the role of Let us assign free-variable" (i.e., frequency) and the other the role of "dependent- variable" (i.e., stimulus intensity). The free-variable is held constant and the dependent-variable is adjusted -17- until the desired response level (criterion) is attained. this procedure is then used with a number of different free-variable values to obtain a set of (free-variable, dependent-variable) points for which the response is constant. Although such an estimation method is straightforward and easily implemented, it has a number of practical limitations. Tacit in our formulation were the assumptions that, 1) the value the response variable is accurately and precisely observed and 2) unlimited time is allowed in which to produce an IRC estimate. Both of these assumptions are inappropriate in most practical applications. For example, the response of an auditory nerve fiber is a stochastic process. Thus, the neural response may not be estimated with arbitrary precision in a limited observation interval. In addition, the amount of time for which the neural response may be stably recorded is limited as well. For these reasons we are forced to make a compromise between measurement precision and measurement duration in the estimation of IRC's. PREVIOUS RIGOROUS STUDIES OF IRC ESTIMATION ALGORITHMS: Inapplicability to Measurements on Cochlear Neurons We may readily extend our definition of an iso-response contour to include any set of points (e.g. (time, excitation energy) ) for which a response variable is constant (e.g. luminescence) in order to completely generalize the problem of IRC estimation. We do this with the thought of applying the extensive and rigorous literature which treats IRC estimation to the problem of IRC estimation in cochlear neurons. In each field, however, the formulation of the algorithm used to estimate IRCs is peculiar to the particular application. For example, with PEST (Parameter Estimation by Sequential Testing, Taylor & Creelman, 1967) an algorithm used in psychophysics, the response variable is the probability of the response exceeding a criterion value. PEST measures such iso-probability contours by forming a probability estimate at each value of the dependent-variable. To estimate a probability, a number of observations are necessary at each (freevariable, dependent-variable) point. This necessity greatly increases the amount of time required to make the measurement. In psychophysics, however, the primary obstacle to obtaining an accurate IRC estimate is the stochastic nature of the human observer's response and not a limitation upon the measurement duration. Thus, PEST and similar algorithms for which a great deal of literature exists (Findlay, 1978; Pentland, 1980; Watson et al, 1983) are not readily applicable to the problem of neural IRC estimation. -18- Threshold Hunter (Raymond, 1978; Carley, 1978) is an algorithm used with peripheral nerve fibers to estimate the stimulus shock intensity level (as a function of time) required to hold the neural response constant. Periodic shocks are applied to a peripheral nerve. The shock level is adjusted in fixed size up/down increments depending upon the neural response to the previous shock. The set of (time, shock level) points is the IRC produced by this algorithm. In principle, this algorithm could be applied to measurement of (frequency, stimulus level) IRC's in cochlear neurons by varying frequency as a function of time. Unfortunately, there is a practical drawback to such an application. In the formulation of Threshold Hunter, it is assumed that the difference between the stimulus level required to maintain a constant response (threshold level difference) for sequential points in time is no greater than the stimulus level increment. The threshold level difference between sequential frequency points for auditory neurons is usually much greater than the precision with which we wish to specify the IRC. Thus the constraint that the threshold level difference be less than or equal to the stimulus level increment is violated. Due to the limitations of these previous studies as applied to measurements of codilear neuron IRCs we have chosen to perform a rigorous analysis on a modern version of an algorithm which has been in use for some time in the field of auditory physiology. This is the Tuning Curve Algorithm (TCA). TCA HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE The TCA has evolved from previously used manual methods (Tasaki, 1954). These early methods of iso-response contour measurement used audiovisual tedmiques and required the experimenter to observe the reponse variable magnitude and manually adjust the stimulus level until the desired response magnitude was attained. With cochlear nerve fibers this measuremnt is relatively easy to make with a minimum of equipment required, and, though it requires a subjective judgment, IRCs can be measured reliably in this manner (Kiang et al, 1965). The measurement, however, is time consing; taking ten to twenty minutes to obtain a set of fifteen (stimulus level, frequency) points (Kiang, unpublished observation). In an effort to assess the frequency selectivity of cochlear neurons in a rapid and objective manner, an automated algorithm was developed which yielded IRC's that closely approximated the those obtained with the subjective procedure (Kiang, Moxon & Levine, 1970). This procedure is the TCA. The TCA is objective, reliable and greatly reduces the time required to measure an IRC (approximately sixty points in -19- frequency per minute). Similar automated algorithms have been developed in several laboratories (Klinke & Pause, 1980; Schmiedt, 1982; Allen, 1982). Thus, the TCA and other TCA-like algorithms (Pickles, 1984; Geisler et al, 1985) are coming increasingly into use. However, there has been no comprehensive study of the accuracy of the TCA in measuring iso-response contours. In this chapter we present such an analysis of the TCA. -20- TOOLS USED TO ASSESS TCA PERFORMANCE To test the efficacy of the TCA, we performed computer simulations of a model of the TCA as well as simulations on an electrical circuit which mimicked certain properties of the response of auditory nerve fibers to sound. Tests were also performed on single cochlear nerve fibers in the alligator lizard. See Weiss et al (1976) or Holton (1980) for a description of methods. STIMULI The tone-burst stimulus used in this study is defined as a sinusoid of amplitude, A, and frequency, f, multiplied by a periodic window function, W(t), of period TTB, duty cycle, [5,(1) and unit amplitude as depicted in figure 2.2. W(t) has a trapezoidal shape to reduce spectral contamination of the stimulus so as to allow accurate measurements of frequency selectivity. The time interval spanned by the rising or falling edge of W(t) is called the rise time, stimulus: 1) Frequency f, Tr, of the tone-burst. Five parameters are required to specify a tone-burst 2) Amplitude A, 3) Duty cycle, Parameter values of 5 =0.5, Tra =100 ms and , 4) Period Tm, and 5) Rise/fall time T,. ms were used in this study unless otherwise indicated. T,=2.5 NEURAL RESPONSE DEFINITIONS A neural impulse train will be represented as a regular point process with instantaneous firing rate, X(t) (Johnson, (1974)). Average rate, X, is defined as, l1-t lf(u)dau X= for arbitrary time t and observation interval I (I >0). (2.la) For a tone-burst stimulus, let Xo. be the average rate during the "on window" of the tone-burst and let Xo/! be the average rate during the "off window". We define the average rate difference to be AX = AX, - XAff (1) The nazero roT/f= (1- porticm at W(t) is tamed the "an winmdow"and the )T. -21- (2.1b) . ero prtin the 'f window". T. = T, and TEST CIRCUIT A test circuit was designed to approximate the frequency selective and stochastic properties of neural discharges. The system shown in figure 2.3 is composed of two cascaded blocks. The first block is a linear bandpass filter which has a frequency response grossly similar to that of a single cochlear nerve fiber (Holton & Weiss, 1983a) in the aligator lizard. The bandpass filter frequency characteristic has a maximum near 1000 Hz and decreases for lower and higher frequencies with asymptotic slopes of- 18 dB/octave (figure 2.4). The second block generates an approximately Poisson(2 ) point process of rate Xy (t)] in response to the filter output y(t). The method used to generate a Poisson process with rate modulated by input voltage y(t) is based upon the distribution of threshold crossings of a Gaussian random process as illustrated in figure 2.5 (Rice, 1944; Bendat, 1958; Papoulis, 1965). Each threshold crossing produces a brief voltage pulse (spike). The spike rate, XIc , produced by the threshold crossings for a stationary Gaussian noise process, n (t) with correlation function R, (T) is, (2.2) X(c)= · where c is a constant threshold (Papoulis, 1965)(3 ) . We assume that eqn. 2.2 is approximately correct for c=y(t)-yo where y(t) is a sinusoid of frequency, f, and amplitude a with f much smaller than the bandwidth of the noise process and a much smaller than y0 (both a and y > 0). The Poisson criterion, which dictates that the probability distribution of spikes in non-overlapping intervals of arbitrary duration be independent, is approximately satisfied by setting the correlation time of the noise process (inversely proportional to the bandwidth of the noise process, R() ) to be much smaller than the average inter-zero-crossing interval 1/A. Thus, except for very small time intervals, interspike intervals will be independent. (2) Spowaneos mchlear neuron actvity (3) R(O) and R(O) am moed n be appromated crudelyby a Polson to ext. -22- roms (Fraz, 1976). The average rate of discharge of a cochlear nerve fiber increases with increasing stimulus level. To ensure that the test circuit also exhibits this property, the negative phase of y(t) must produce increases above the quiescent rate (X0 =h (y)) that are larger than the decreases elicited by the positive phase of y(t). This was achieved by setting threshold level to 2 times the RMS level of the noise, yo =2[R, (0)]'. choice of yo we see from the rate function of eqn. 2.2 that I(yo+8)-X(Yo)I < For this Iy 0-8)-X(y)I for posi- tive 8 and this asymmetry of rate difference about Xo increases with increasing B. This assures that as the peak-to-peak magnitude of y (t) increases, the average rate also increases. To simulate the effects of different spontaneous discharge rates upon IRC estimation algorithms we changed the quiescent spike rate of the test circuit. From eqn. 2.2 and figure 2.5 we see that the spontaneous rate, X(yo), may be changed by adjusting the value of yo or R, (O) or by adjusting the cutoff frequency, fc, of the noise shaping filter S(f). ( ) e - y° Since small changes in Yo and R, () produce relatively large changes in , adjustment of Y0 or R(O) bandwidth of the filtered noise signal, (IR' () I fe and R. (0) a ft). was deemed inappropriate. R () We therefore chose to adjust the , which varies linearly with the filter cutoff frequency We used an automatic gain control (AGC) circuit to maintain the RMS level, (R, (O))I, at some preset value, thereby holding e - Y "( ) constant. In this manner the average rate could be repeatably and conveniently adjusted. Shown in figure 2.6 are samples of the test circuit response to simulated tone-bursts. We display an estimate of the instantaneous rate as a function of time, or a post-stimulus time (PST) histogram (Johnson, 1974) in response to a tone-burst stimulus. The average rate of discharge in the on-window increases with increasing stimulus level at each frequency. The spontaneous discharge rate is seen in the off-window. The frequency selective properties of the test circuit are apparent in that a fixed level stimulus elicits diminishing average rate response as frequency is varied from 1 kHz as does the frequency response shown in figure 2.4. Thus, the circuit crudely exhibits some of the frequency selective properties of cochlear neurons. -23- A MARKOV MODEL OF TCA BEHAVIOR: Theory and Experiment It should be noted that although the neural average rate is the response variable in this analysis, these results apply with appropriate modification to any quantifiable response variable. For example, in Chapter 3 we apply these results to the measurement of neural synchronization to a sinusoidal stimulus. A DESCRIPTION OF THE TCA The TCA presents a tone-burst to the ear of the experimental animal at a fixed frequency and level (figure 2.7). The number of neural impulses that occur during the off interval, (Noff) is subtracted from the number of spikes that occur during the on interval (N,). If Ne,-No/>C, This difference is compared to some criterion C. then the stimulus level is lowered by one step (DOWN). If N 0 1-No C, then the stimulus level is raised by two steps (UP). When either of the stimulus level sequences UP-DOWN-DOWN or DOWN-UP-DOWN occurs, a success is declared and the final level (that level attained by the last down step) is defined to be the threshold for that frequency. The frequency is then incremented. The level of the last stimulus presentation at the previous frequency becomes the level of the first presentation at the new frequency and the hunting process begins anew. It is tempting to assume that at frequency f, the TCA finds level P such that AX(P J)=XA X, = CuT.o (T, is the duration of the tone-burst on-window). A(P where ) is the average rate difference between the spike rates in the on and off stimulus windows. It is also tempting to estimate the rate criterion that the algorithm seeks. For example, for a criterion, C =0, the level sought should be that at which O<(N, -Nof/)<1 on the average (berman, 1978). Thus, for a tone-burst stimulus with T,, =50 ms and To = 50 ms, a rate criterion between zero and 20 spikes/sec would be expected. likewise we might expec that for a criterion of C the algorithm seeks a difference N, -No// such that C <Nmo-Nof/<C +1 on the average. These intuitions are, strictly speaking, false. The purpose of this section is to specify a model of the algorithm and to analyze its performance so that the precise behavior of the TCA can be determined. In the following section we will show that the TCA attempts to hold a probability (not an average rate of discharge) constant. Specifically, the 1CA seeks a stimulus level, response exceeds criterion is equal to 2/3. -24- , such that the probability that the MODEL FORMULATION We assume that previous stimulus presentations have no effect upon the response during the present stimulus presentation. More explicitly, for fixed frequency we let the stimulus level at time t (t an integer) be represented by an integer l (t). Hence, Prob( (No-Noff ) > C, 1(t)|I(t-1), (t-2), ... ) = Prob( (No,,-Nff) > C, (t)). (2.3) While this assumption is probably not strictly correct (e.g., Gray, (1966)), it is a reasonable starting point for mathematical analysis. Such an assumption suggests the Markov model represented in figure 2.8a for TCA stimulus level presentations at fixed frequency. g (l) is the probability that (No. -Noff)>C the algorithm increments the level by 2, whereas if (N., -Nff)>C at level . If (N, -Noff )-C the level is decremented by 1. Defining p, (l) as the probability of being in state I at time t we have ptl(1)=g(l+1)p,(l+1) + (1-g(l-2))p,(l-2) A success level (2.4) is generated by either the sequence UP-DOWN-DOWN (UDD) or DOWN-UP-DOWN (DUD) starting from state (4 ) I and ending in state 1. The probability of occurrence of each of these sequences is DUD,= g(l)(1-g(l-1))g(l + ) (2.5a) UDDI = (1-g(l))g (+2)g ( +1) (2.5b) We define DUD and UDD 1 as the probabilities of occurrence of these two sequences from a starting state I. Let us now examine the probability of success in a given state at time t. The history of the process has an effect upon which state transitions result in successes. For example, the sequence DDUDDUD would be grouped as D(DUD)(DUD) where the subsequences in parentheses are successes. The grouping DD(UDD)UD would not occur since the sequence DUD occurred first and after a sucess is declared, the algorithm disregards the prior sequence of state transitions. Thus the algorithm operates on the sequence of state transitions generated by the Markov process in a selective manner and does not identify all subsequences UDD or DUD as successful. With this property in mind we derive an expression for the probability of success in state I at time t, p, (,t). (4) State and level will be used interdmngeably In order to declare a successin state I at time t the process must be in state I at t -3. In addition, the determine which success sequences generate a success in . For transitions which lead to state I at t-3 example, if the process were in state +1 at t -4 and made the D transition to I at t -3, then the success sequence UDD from I at t-3 would generate a success in + 1 at t -1 via a DUD success rather than a sucess in I att via the UDD sequence. Since the success sequences are DUD and UDD, the prefixes (prior sequences) DU and UD cannot occur with DUD otherwise the premature success (DUD)UD or (UDD)UD would result. Similarly, D cannot occur as a prefix for UDD without the same effect. Table 2.1 lists the sequences which will produce a success in l at time t. PREFIX Table 2.1: Success State History START STATE (t6) SUCCESS SEQUENCE DDD DDU DUD UDD 1+3 I DUU UDD or DUD 1-3 UDU UUD UUU UDD none UDD or DUD 1-3 1-3 1-6 success in I UDD or DUD I Therefore, Ps(1,t ) = pt-6( +3)DDD _3DUD,+p,-t_ 1 UDD, 6 (1)DDU +P,-6(1 -3)DUU -_3(UDD, +DUD, )+p,(1 - (2.6a) 3)UDUl _3UDD, +pr-6(1-6)UUU- 6 (UDDi+DUD,)+p,( ,t -3)(UDDi+DUD) Rewriting some of the prefixes in terms of the transition probabilities, g(1), and regrouping we have, p, (l,t ) = p, (1,t -3)(UDD, +DUD,) + p.-6(1 +3 )g (1+3 )g (1+2)g (1+1)DUD, (2.6b) + [p,-,(1-3)g(i-3)+p,-6(l-6)(1-g (1-6))](1-g (1-4))(1-g (l-2))(UDD,+DUD,) + [P -6(1)g(l)+p,-6(1-3)(1-g (-3))]g (1-1)(1-g (1-2))UDD, Using equation (2.4) repeatedly we obtain, -26- (2.6c) p, ( ,t) = p, (1,t - 3)(DUD,+ UDDi)+p - 3(1)(DDI +DUD,) -p, _,( + 1)g (1+ 1)UDD, -P-S(l -I1)[(1-8(l-1))8(l+1)+S(l-1)(1-g(l-2)) -pr-6(l)(1- DUD ())g (1 +2) (1 + 1)DUDI. Thus, given g(1), the probability of meeting criterion at stimulus level 1, and equations 2.4 and 2.6c we have specified the behavior of the tuning curve algorithm if it is allowed to choose stimulus levels and sucess levels (threshold estimates) at a constant stimulus frequency. Equation 2.4 determines the relative frequency of stimulus levels, 1, which are presented by the algorithm and equation 2.6c governs the relative frequency of success levels, ,. Using these equations we will now examine the steady state distributions of stimulus level and success level. Through these distributions we will ascertain what stimulus level the TCA would choose as threshold on the average, were an infinite amount of time allowed for the TCA to choose threshold points. We will show that the average threshold level is near the value of I for which g(l1)=2/3. STEADY STATE BEHAVIOR OF THE MODEL Theoretical Considerations Derivationof Steady State Equations The state occupancy probability as well as the probability of success would become time invariant as t tends toward infinity if the Markov process were composed of a single ergodic drain (Drake, 1967; Papouis, 1965). However, note that if state is occupied at t =0, three steps later at t =3 only states l-3, 1, +3 and 1+6 could be occupied (see figure 2.8a). Every third state belongs to the same subchain if time is measured in three step intervals. Thus the Markov process which determines the behavior of the TCA does not consist 1 of a single ergodic chain and it is not clear that a steady state exists. For example, if po(l)=l then P3-1(l)=Pi-2(1)=O (i =1,2,...) and no steady state aocns. Nonetheless, a steady state does exist for the initial conditions that are likely to be encountered. To ilustrate, we wil identify the set of initial conditiomns for which a steady state exists and show that they are likely to occur. process is derived from the process depicted in figConsider the transition diagram of figure 2.8b. ThIbis ure 2.8a by using only 3-step observation intervals as discussed in the previous paragraph. This nw process -27- is a single ergodic chain and therefore is guaranteed to have a steady state. The original process states, , are spanned by three ( ... l-2, +1,l+4.. such consisting subchains states ( * l-3, ), , +3 H,and ). Wewillca thesesubchainsI, ... -1,1+2,1+5 ), and.. ( the of respectively. A one-step transiton from subchain I will leads to subchain II. ikewise, me-step transitions from subchains I and HI leads to subchains III and I respectively. Consider an initial state occupancy probability for subchain I, l =3i i=..-1,O,1... Po(l) = otherwise (2.7) where a! is the initial state occupancy probability of state I on subchain I at t=O. We also assume that C a' = I so that the probability of being in subchains II or m at t =O is zero. Since a one-step transi- j=-x tion from subchain I leads to subchain fl, at t=1 only subchain II is occupied. ikewise, at t =2 only sub- chain m is occupied. Thus an initial condition on any one subchain will be transformed into a nonzero state occupancy probability distribution on the other subchains. Since each of the subchains is ergodic, a nonzero state occupancy probability at any time t leads to a steady state state occupancy distribution as t-.. There- fore, for the initial conditions proposed on chain I in eqn. 2.7, each of the subchains will achieve a steady state (for time in three step intervals). This may be written as, 1 l=3i, t=lim 3k p,(l) 1, l=3i+1, t=lm 3k+ = f" where pB, 13, and pf (2.8) l =3i+2, t =im 3k+2 are the steady state distributions on chains I, II and III respectively and i is an irnteger. We mw modify the initial condition of eqn. 2.8 to obtain, (ai po(l)= 1=3i pl"al4 Pdam 1=3i+1 l=3i+2 I where P'+ P' + p =1 In this manner we can account for arbitrary initial conditions on the original chain of figure 2.8a. -28- (2.9) Since the difference equation 2.4 is linear, scaling the initial conditions by p scales the steady state solution by p as well. Therefore, I=3i, r=lim 3k 1 =3i +1, t =lim 3k +1 p'pi p p 1=3i +2, t =lim 3k +2 A-- I =3i, t =lim 3k +1 l =3i +1, r =lim 3k +2 p,(l) = (2.10) l =3i +2, t =lim 3k I =3i, t =lim3k +2 &-x l =3i +1, t =lim 3k l =3i +2, t =lim 3k +1 From equation 2.10 we see that if p = = 1/3 we obtain in compact form, 1=3i 3 P()) =3i+l 3 (2.11) l=3i +2 Thus we conclude that if the initial state occupancy probability is equally distributed over the three subchains, then a steady state for equation 2.4 exists. Although the TCA starts from a particular state at t=0 for a given frequency, a priori, there is some uncertainty about the identity of the starting state. This uncertainty will be large enough that we would be unable to say which of any three adjacent states (therefore three different subchains) is a more likely starting state. Therefore, to find the time invariant state occupancy probability p (1), we consider eqn 2.4 with an initial condition which spans each of the three subdchainswith equal probability and thus assure a steady-state state occupancy probability, p () = p (+l)g(l+ 1) + p (- In light of this result, equation 2.6c becomes, -29- 2 )(1-g (- 2 )) (2.12) pI(l ,t ) = p,(l,t -3)(UDD,+DUDi)+ p(l)(UDDI+DUD,) (2.13) -p (I+1) ( +1)UDD, (-1))g ( +1)+g(l-1)(1- (-2)) ]DUDI -p (- 1l)[(10-g -p (I)UDDtDUD Equation 2.13 may be written as a simple difference equation for fixed 1, p,(l ,t) - a(l)p,(lI,t-3) = b (1) (2.14) where, a () = (DUD,+UDD,) and b(l) = p ()(UDD +DUD,)- p(l +1)g(l+ 1)UDD, p(l - 1)[(1-8 ( l -1))g (l + 1)+g (l -1)(1- (1-2)) DUD, -p (l)UDDiDUD, Ignoring imaginary roots, the homogeneous solution to equation 2.14 is, t p,(l,t) = A[a(l)l3 (2.15) Since a (l)< 1, the homogenous solution decays to zero as t approaches infinity, and we are left with the particular solution of, P,( ,) = P (l) = I() (2.16) We now have equations for the steady-state state occupancy probability, p (1), and the steady-state probability of success at a given state, p, (1). Thus, we have shown that steady-state distributions of stimulus level, , and success level, I,, exist. Using these distributions we will provide bounds for the average stimulus level, E (), for arbitrary g (I). The AverageLevels E () and E (,) E(1) is the average stimulus level presented by the TCA. Knowledge of the properties of E(l) should help us determine the expected value of the threshold obtained by the TCA, E (), intimately related (eqn. 2.16). Thus we shall examine the properties of E(I). since p () ad p(l ) are We will show that E(I) is dosely associated with the level 1:23where g (1z,3)=2/3. To assure that E(l) exists,(5 ) g(l) will be restricted (5) The existence of E() is an impoant csideration ince without a unique and finite E(I) no unique threshold detemi- -30- to a certain class. This dass is described in Appendix A. Here we assume that g(1) is in this class. Before deriving the bounds on E (1) we derive two equations which will be of general use. The first of these is obtained by taking the z-transform of the steady state probability equation 2.12. This results in + 2P(z) - z 2 G,(z) P(z) = z-G(z) (2.17) where G,(z) = and Zg(l)p (1), P(z) = i z'p(l) Rearranging terms in equation (2.17), we obtain P( z= 1-) G(z(z) (2.18) (l+) Setting z =1 and noticing that P (1)=1 and Gp (1)=E (g(l)) we find E (g(l)) = 2 (2.19) The second of these useful equations is derived now for convenience. We will make use of it later. By taking the inverse transform of eqn. 2.18 we obtain the difference equation, p (l )8 () = p (l-1)(1-g (1-1)) + p (l-2)(1-g (I-2)) Equation 2.20 may also be obtained by drawing an imaginary vertical line between states (2.20) and !-1 of the Markov chain in figure 2.8a. For the system to be in steady state the probability of a transition to the right must equal the probability of a transition to the left. Equating these probabilities leads directly to equation 2.20. We now determine upper and lower bounds for E (). By differentiating equation 2.18 with respect to z and using the moment generating properties of the z transform (Papoulis, 1965; Drake, 1967) we find that, (2.21a) 3E(g(1)) -2 E () = 1 or equivalently, l (3 ()-2)p(l) Let g() = be a monotone increasing function for which E(I) eists( nation an be made by the algaithm (6) An exac desiption of g(l) may be found in appendix A. -31- (2.21b) 6 ). Consider a shifted version of g(l), g '(l)=g(l-+), p(i), p '(l)=p(l-). 2 where g '(-l)-, 2 g '(+ )> This new function g '(l) gives rise to a shifted version of If p '(I) is substituted into equation (2.21b) we obtain, (2.22) p'(1)(3g '(l)-2)=1 but since (3g'(l)-2) > 0 for 1>0 0 for l<O (3'(l)-2) (2.23) we also have Ilp'(1)(3g'(I)-2) - 1 (2.24) 1=-- Since all the terms in the summation of eqn. 2.24 are positive lp'(l)(3g'()-2) < Ip'(1)(3g'(l)-2) < 1 , I=k k1 (2.25) 1=0 Since p '(I) is a probability function, A-1 0 (2.26) < lp '(l )-<k - 1 1=0 And since g'(1) is monotone, eqn. (2.25) yields lp '(l) 1 <- 3g'(k)-2 I=k (2.27) Combining eqn. 2.26 and eqn. 2.27 we have, E(/') = '() -< p'() k 38 '(k)-2 + k-1 (2.28) Tberefore, 1 (3g'(k)-2) + (2.29) 0 A simil derivation applied to lp E(l) yields "()(3g "()-2) j+1 + 1 - I j< -1 (2.30) To test the bounds we apply them to a 102 point sequence g (I)=0, 0.01, 0.02,... 1.0, 1.0 where the average stimulus level is known (E(l)=66.66) with 14 = 67, _1= 66, j =-5, 56.12 s E(l) - 77.25 -32- k =5 results in (2.31) We now turn to the study of E (,). To compute E (4) we must first obtain p,(l |suacess). This is p,(lIsuccess)= P(() (2.32) Thus E(,) = (2.33) lp,(l Isuccess) I Unfortunately, p,(l) is quite complex (eqn. 2.16), hence it is difficult to derive an analytical expression far E (1,). Therefore, we compute p,(l) and E (l,) numerically. NumericalDeterminationsof p (1) and p (l Isuccess) In this section we compute p (), p,(l), E(l) and E(l,) for several spedfic g(l) functions. With these E(t,) g(l) we illustrate that E(l) l2.,3where l2,3 is the leve such that g(l23) is doser to 2/3 than any other level, I. These results are shown to hold for a larger dass of g(l) than is represented by the specific g (l) functions we have chosen. The technique used to compute p (l) consisted of constaining g (l)=O for I<O and g(l)=1 (L is therefore the number of nonzero terms in the sequence g (l) for which I -L.,) for l -Lm thereby constaining p (l) to be a finite sequence. Since p () was finite and equal to zero for l <0, the difference equation p (,) (eqn. 2.20) was solved numerically by assuming initial conditions of x(-1)=0 and x(0)=1 with the recursive equation, x(l) Ix(l-l)(l-g(1-1))+ x(1-2 )(1-g(t- 2 ))1 (2.34) Equation 2.34 is an algebraic rearrangement of eqn. 2.20 with x substituted for p. The state occupancy probability function, P(l)= L p,(l success) was computed using equations 2.16 and 2.32. Five C x(l) i=0 specific g(l) were used. Of these five, two were linear functions of I (g(l)=I/L,) spacings (/L.) (figures 2.9a&b). with different point For contrast, we chose two other g (1) to be semicircular arcs (figures 2.9c&d); one concave, the other convex. In addition we also chose a discontinuous linma g (1) (figure 2.90). The associated p(l) (I/L.). and p,(l success) are superimposed and plotted as a function of normalized level We see that in every case except figure 2.9e both E() -33- and E(l, ) lie within on step in level of 123 (the l3 point is circled). For figure 2.9e, E(,) lies within one step in level of lz3 while E(I) lies just out- side a level step from 12,3. Thus, for the five different g () considered, the average stimulus presentation level and the average success level are dosely associated with the level la3. Some generalizations may be made on the basis of the numerical results obtained for linear g(l). Notice in figures 2.9a&b that the state occupancy probability, p (1), is concentrated between two values of I. In the case of figure 2.9b, Mp () .95 for .5_g()<.8. Intuitively it would appear that any g(l) which may be approximated by the linear g(l) of figure 2.9b in this interval will have E(l) and E(I,)=lZ. intuition is verified analytically in Appendix A (eqns. A.18 through A.20). briefly. Let p'(l) .5-g(1).82. 'Ihis We present the result here be the steady state state occupancy probability for a sequence g'(l), similar to g(l) for p'(1) and p(l), the distribution associated with g (), will be approximately equal in this range if p (ll- 1)+p (l) and p (12)+p ( 12+ 1) are much smaller than one. As a case in point, notice the similarity of the probability distributions of figures 2.9b and 2.9c. This similarity results from the fact that the average slope of the circular g(l) for .5-g() .8 at .023 per level-step (fig. 2.9c) is approximately equal to the slope of the linear contour at .02 per level-step (fig. 2.9b). In summary we see that for the g(l) we have examined, the average stimulus level E(l) and the average success level (threshold estimate), E(1,) is approximately 123 where g (1z3)=2/3. We have also seen that the approximate congruence of the steady-state stimulus level distribution, p (1), for simila g (l) in the interval (0.5, 0.82) is a very useful property. Using linear g(l) of various slopes (various L.) for which p(l) outside of the prescribed interval is negligible, we may approximate p (l) produced by any other sequence, g'(1), as long as the linear g () and g'() are similar in the interval (0.5, 0.82). The TCA Does NOT Necessarily Satisfy a Rate Criterion The Markov process model of the TCA depends only upon g(l), the probability of being above criterion. 'IThus,p () and p,(l) which characterize the steady state behavior of the TCA, depend explicitly on the fumction g () and not Ak(/). Furthermore, in the absece of a known empirical relation between AX(l) and g () or equivalently knowledge of the underlying spike generation process, g (1) provides no information about A(l). We illustrate as follows. -34- Let A = N, - N . We define a probability function, q (A ll), whose value is the probability of obtaining a spike difference A at stimulus level 1, The probability of being above criterion, g (l), is then g(l) = Prob(A>C)= E q(All) A=C+1 (2.35) The average rate difference AX(l) is then, IAX()= E(AII) -= Aq(AI) (2.36) A=-z where I is the duration of the tone-burst on and off windows (T, =Toff =I). We will now show that given I and a value of g ()= a ( < a < 1) that we may construct q(A i ) so as to leave AX(I) unconstrained. For g (I)= a we define the probability functions, h+(A) = ( ) h_(A) = (ac A> C (2.37a) (2.37b) hf(A) and h_(A) are arbitrary within their prescribed domains. Thus Eh+(A) and Eh_(A) may take on any value within these domains. -=<E, C+1 ()= E+() 1 = Aq(11l) Al A C+1 C (2.38a) q(A I) < (2.38b) Using equations 2.36 and 2.38a&b we obtain, Y Aq(A1l) = (-a)E_(A) From equation 2.39 and equations 2.38 we see that E_(A) -x < A(l) < (2.39) + aEh,(A) = IAX(l) and E+(A) may be chosen so that for 0 < a < 1. Thus, fixing g(l)=cx does not necessarily limit AX(l) to any particular range of values. In conclusion, we see that AA(l) is not necessarily related to g(l). Although an empirical relation between these two variables may exist, if the relation is unknown, then without spefication.of AX(l) cannot be inferred from g (). q (Al ), In short, it should simply be noted that the TCA average stimulus level and average success level are not determined by an average rate difference but by the function g (). Any relation between AX and g is due to the nature of the spike generation process and not the properties of the -35- TCA. Experiments to Examine TCA behavior in the Steady State In this section we will examine properties TCA steady state behavior implied by the Markov model. For each property considered we provide a qualitative description and the results of experiments on the test circuit and cohlear neurons which are designed to test the qualitative description. PredictedValuesof E (1) and E (I,) UsingMeasurementsof g (1) Measurement procedure If the Markov model accurately predicts the behavior of the TCA then g(l) can be used to predict the mean and standard deviation (s.d.) of 1,1, in the steady state. Thus, the experiment devised to test the Markov model of TCA behavior consisted of measuring the function g (1) as well as the sample mean, ,, and s.d., ,l, at fixed frequency for both the test circuit and codlear nerve fibers in the alligator lizard. Specifi- cally, using tone-burst stimuli with 50 ms on and off windows and a criteria of C =0, the tuning curve algo(7 ) The rithm was allowed to determine 80 successive threshold levels while the frequency was held constant. sample mean, l,l, and variance, Fal were computed from the last 50 trials. This procedure was used to reduce the effects of startup transients on the calculated average threshold level. g(l) was then estimated for levels above and below I, by determining the relative frequency of No, -Nof >0 for many tone-burst presentations at each level.(8 ) A line of slope a (linear regression) was fit to the measured g () and from this slope the linear approximation to g(l), g, (l), was obtained. The mean and s.d. of l,l were then predicted using g,,(l) dB level steps). Finally, the sample mean, l,,, and sample s.d. E(l, llgu,), and predicted s.d., E(a,llg). (using 1/4 ,l were compared to the predicted mean, Typical sets of measurements are presented for the test circuit (7) Frequencies of 2KHz and 1KHz were used for the test writ and nerve fibers revely. The level step ize was 14 da the tandhrd deviatim ot the measurement is r=((1-g())g(Q.Y¥) (8) Under the aumpfion o independet tri, We computed the san where R is the number of stimuluspresentations. We assume the maximum value of acr=%(IK). dard error to be .017 for the tee crcuit (800 trials) and .04 for the nerve fibers (150 trials). -36- (figure 2.10a) and an alligator lizard cochlear neuron (figure 2.10b). Measurements on the test circuit For the test circuit, the slope of g (I) is changed by varying the spontaneous rate X0 . Average threshold determinations were done at two spontaneous rates, Xo= 9 spikes/second and 46 spikes/second to compare results obtained with different g(l). E (, The difference between E(lQJ1glm) and 1,1 in dB is Als, aj1 and gli,) are measured in dB. a, the slope of gjl, (), is in units of 1/dB. Table 2.2: TCA steady state threshold determination (800 trials/point). _o spikessec 9 46 In both cases E(lllgj) Als dB 0.0 0.0 1 is identical to 1,I and a 1/dB .07 .05 _ sI dB .95 1.22 E (,l 1gli,) dB 1.08 1.28 sll is comparable to E(ao, lg,,). Thus, it appears that the Markov model of the TCA predicts the steady state behavior of the TCA for the test circuit. Measurements on cochlear neurons Nineteen threshold determinations were made on fourteen units in two alligator lizards. As with the test circuit, the results are tabulated (table 2.3). -37- Table 2.3: TCA steady state threshold level determination on auditory nerve fibers (14 units in two animals) (150 trials/point). The standard error for rate measurements is .5 spikes/second unless otherwise noted. For five units g (I) was measured twice frcxn a single determination of threshold. The second measurement of g(l) is denoted by a " in the ,J1 column. L_ . a At E(arIlg1 ) i/dB .08 .07 .06 .09 .09 .09 .12 .09 .05 dB .78 .82 1.38 1.47 .9 .68 dB 1.02 1.08 1.17 .95 .95 .95 .83 .95 1.28 spikes/sec 4.1 1.8 2.0 5.6 11.7 8.5 9.7 5.8 10.0 dB .21 .38 -.05 -.48 .629 -.48 .03 -.37 .33 8.8 .166 .1 7.3 7.6 9.6 .24 .07 .44 .07 .1 .06 15 +-1 -.03 .1 1.6 6.6 4.5 4.4 .38 .33 .08 -.58 .07 .08 .08 .04 1.03 1.32 .97 1.08 1.02 1.08 1.44 .82 1.08 .90 1.8 .38 .07 .09 +-.34 .08 +.02 In table 2.3 we see that in only two cases does E(l,IIg,,) ured variance, l (correlation 1.02 .86 1.09 .9 1.17 .90 mean +s.d. The predicted variance, E (r .83 1.66 1.04 +±.30 1.04 ±.14 lie more than 2 steps in level (.50 dB) from 1,. lgj, ), however, on a fiber by fiber basis, does not compare well to the measoefficient = .22 ; correlation slope = .11). We feel this is due to the fact that the measurement of g () is less precise for the nerve fibers (150 trials) than for the test circuit (800 trials) thereby causing a larger variation in the predicted slope, a. For all except one of the five units in which g () was measured twice we see slope estimation differences of at least .03/dB. As can be seen by comparing the values of E(a,l Igtic) predicted from the two slopes, the predicted variance is sensitive to changes in the slope of g,,(l). Nonetheless, the sample variance values, cl, are comparable to the predicted variance for the range of slopes measured (compare the mean variances in table 2.3). Thus, it appears that for auditory nerve fibers in the alligator lizard, the model of the TCA does predict the average sucxess level chosen by the TCA in the steady state and roughly predicts the deviation of the success levels chosen about this aver- -38- age level. Variation of AX with Spontaneous Rate, Xo Prediction of AX vs. Xo assuming a homogeneous Poisson spike generation process It is possible that the average rate difference associated with g =2/3 may depend upon the fiber's spontaneous rate. To explore this possibility we perform the following analysis. Let the previously introduced random variables No,, and Nolf have Poisson distributions with average intensities X,,(l)T,, and XoffToff respectively. T, and Toff, as before, are the observation intervals in which we record No, and Noff respec tively. By definition, p(No, I1)= N, eC Tohatl p(Noff) = (ohow Nof ! O ideal-lf tr(f) We have shown that ideally the TCA chooses level l23 as threshold such that g (12)2/3. g (1) = Prob (No,-Nif = C (2.40a) (2.40b) We form g (l) as, (2.41) >C 11) N, -(C +) p(No. I) N.=C +1 E N P(Nof) =O -~o~t)7t0", ^of..-C+') (.(t)T, )N"" (off Trft ),' N,=C1-INff N. ! Noff =O We determined X,, (l23) by setting g(1)=2/3 and solving eqn. 2.41 numerically with C =0,1 and 2 and with various values of Xolf Tol . We list the results in table 2.4 assuming that, T,. =To, =50ms; a standard value used in measurements on cochlear neurons. The assumption of a Poisson spike distribution predicts that AX at g =2/3 will show substantial increases with increasing Xoff. -39- >C)=2/3 for T, =Tolf =50 ms. Off-window rate is Table 2.4: AX for Prob(N,.-Nf! Xf!. All entries in spikes/second. - XAf AX = 1\°1f 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 C=0 C=1 C=2 22.4 24.0 26.4 28.5 30.5 32.5 34.2 35.7 37.2 38.6 46.1 47.6 49.3 51.2 53.0 54.6' 56.1 57.5 59.0 60.0 68.7 70.2 71.9 73.7 75.1 76.3 77.9 79.2 80.5 81.7 Measurementson the test circuit To investigate whether AX varied with Xof mined by the TCA. Tone-bursts with T,=Tff AX was nmeasured at the average success level, 1,1, deter- =50 ms were used with a criterion of C=O0. AX for three different off-window rates are listed in table 2.5. As can be seen, the average rate difference increases with increasing Xof in a manner similar to that predicted in table 2.4 for C=0. Table 2.5: Average rate difference, AX, vs. off-window rate, ff , measured at stimulus level, 1,1 for the test circuit. T,. =T,l =50 ms and the TCA criterion, C=0. All entries in spikes/second. The standard error of all measurements listed is .5 spike/sec. 9 30 46 22 26 30 Measurements on cochlear neurons To determine whether the spontaneous rate of a fiber has an effect upon the rate, A(12,), associated with threshold (g =2/3), we first determine whether the off-window rate, Aoff depends upon spontaneous rate, Xo. We do this since the TCA does not directly use the spontaneous rate in the estimatiqn of l2 but rather, uses the off-window rate Af I . We have compiled PS histograms of the response to tone-bursts (T, =T.o=50 ms, T,=2.5 ms) from miscellaneous eents. We have included only histograms for which A > 20 spikes/sec in order to assure that the fiber response differed from the spontaneous. The spontaneous rate, o0,for each fiber was also measured. As can be seen in figure 2.11 where we plot off versus the spontaneous rate, there is a strong correlation (corr: .88, slope: .67) between off-window rate Xof and spontaneous rate X0 (43 units in 4 alligator lizards). Thus, in light of this result and the prediction of table 2.4 for Poisson spike distributions we would expect that the average rate difference, A?, corresponding to g =2/3 to increase with increasing spontaneous rate in alligator lizard cochlear neurons. In order to test this hypothesis, AX(l) gator lizards. and Xqf (12,3)were measured for fourteen units ( 9 ) in two alli- he results are listed in table 2.6. dependence upon Xff (l2). Specifically, A(12,) AX(l 2 3) for the nerve fibers does not seem to show a is not correlated with Aolf (correlation: .05, slope: .26). This result is at odds with the prediction of increased Ai with increased Xo and suggests that the assumption of a Poisson spike probability distribution is incorrect However, the assumption of a Poisson distribution does provide a rough estimate of the value of AX(12 3). With a criterion of C =0 and a spontaneous rate range of 0 to 20 spikes/sec we predict using the Poisson spike distribution assumption that AX(1l2) is between 22 and 26 spikes/sec. The average value of AX(1z23)falls roughly in this range (22 +3 spikes/sec). (9) bse are the same mits as In table 23. age values are given (italics) For the five uits t.r whidM~(v,) 41- and ff/(t:) were meaured twice ar. Table 2.6: 19 average rate differences, AX, vs. off-window rate, Aof, measured at stimulus level, 2I for 14 units in two alligator lizads All entries in spikes/second. The standard error is .5 for Xof and 1 for AX(12,3) unless otherwise noted. For units in which duplicate measrements were taken we give the average resoonse values (italics). 4.1 1.8 2.0 5.6 11.7 23 19 24 28 22 9.1 24 5.8 28 9.4 7.5 22 20 12.3 - 1 19 4.1 20.5 4.5 4.4 1.8 18 20 19 Variation of AA(l2) with Frequency Analytic description It is possible that the average rate difference Ah measured at the TCA chosen threshold, 123, varies with stimulus frequency. To see why this is so let us first consider the variation of g(l) with frequency. The simplest possibility is that the shape of g () is independent of frequency, f, except for a tanslation along the l-axis, i.e., g(lf) = 8 (t-z(f)) (2.42) where z (f) defines the shape of the IRC. However, since the TCA holds probability but not rate constant, different frequencies, f, may have l/3 that correspond to different average rates, AX(12/3). If this is the case the TCA would seek a different average rate for each frequency (for convnience 'probability/rate frequency shift"). we will call this a he presence of large probability/rate frequency shifts would imply that the TCA iso-probability contour estimate is be different from the iso-average rate contour which it was originally thought to estimate (Liberman, 1978; Holton, 1980; Holton & Weiss, 1983a). Thus, any previous conclusions which explitly depended upon the average rate difference being constant along a TCA-estimated -42- IRC may be in error. Measurements on cochlear neurons To investigate whether large probability/rate frequency shifts exist in the alligator lizard, g(l) and AA(I) were measured for three different sets of 1; each set in the vidnity of threshold for frequencies one octave below CF, at CF and one octave above CF. If Ati(l3) is a strong function of frequency, then the value of AX(l) in the neighborhood of g (1)=2/3 should differ appreciably for different frequencies. In figure 2.12a we plot for one representative unit, g(Q) vs. A(l) to generate the curves g(AX). As can be seen the curves (A)) overlap over a large range of g. Also, it is apparent when all data are superimposed (figure 2.12b, 18 units in four animals) that the rate associated with the lz point is = 20 spikes/sec for many different fibers. In figure 2.12c&d we see that for C =1 and C =2, AX(12) 40 spikes/sec and 60 spikes/sec respectively. These data suggest that at the frequencies studied, the average rate difference associated with the 23 level is not strongly variant with frequency. Furthermore, due to the observed large region of overlap of the g (AT) at various frequencies and criterion levels, and its relative invariance across units and animals, in the alligator lizard there probably exists an empirical relationship between g (1) and A(l). Summary of Steady State Properties We have shown analytically and experimentally that the TCA seeks as threshold the stimulus level 2 where the probability of exceeding criterion is equal to 2/3. In addition we have shown analytically that the TCA uses only the function g(l) and not the average rate difference, AX(I), to determine threshold. We have also shown that this property suggests that the average rate difference empirically associated with g =2/3 may vary with both fiber spontaneous rate and stimulus frequency. We have found, however, that there does appear to be an empirical relationship between g (l) and A(l) which is common to many fibers in many animals. Thus the TCA iso-probability contour estimate is also an iso-rate contour estimate. -43- TRANSIENT BEHAVIOR OF TE MODEL AND TCA IRC ESTMATION ERROR In this section we will show that the TCA produces a biased estimate of the 12,3level and that the magnitude of this bias may be affected by the frequency characteristic of the acoustic system as well as the frequency response of the system under study. Errors in the estimation of Q (the sharpness of tuning), P (minimum sound level required to elicit a response), and CF (frequency at which Pi, exists) will be dis- cussed. Theoretical Considerations We have previously studied the steady state behavior of the Markov model at fixed frequency. During the estimation of an IRC, however, the TCA increments frequency immediately after a success is declared and begins to search for the first success level at the new frequency. In terms of our model, we specify this process as follows. Consider the TCA just after it has declared a success at level k and frequency fo. frequency is incremented to f and the first stimulus level presented at f 1 The is the previous success level (thres- hold estimate), k. The stimulus level is then varied until a success, ,1, is dedared at frequency f 1. This process continues until threshold estimates have been made at each frequency. In choosing the first success at a given frequency, however, the TCA does not allow a steady state to be attained. Thus, E(1, ) will not in general equal 12, and the TCA estimate of threshold will be in error. We now quantify this error for the Markov model of TCA behavior. The probability that 1,1 is the first successful state given the starting state is k may be written as p5 l(lli,k) = where p (,l,k) i (2.43) p,(1,l,k )sj(l 1) is the probability of taking path i from k to ,1 with no intervening successes and the si(l,l) are the allowable success sequence probabilities which terminate path i in a sucess at 1,1.(1 0 ) The expected value and variance of the first success from state k may be computed from p, 1(l,l,k). p,l(l,,k) for a given 8(1) by simulating the Markov chain. We chose gga.,()=/L. We have estimated (1) to be the linear function (see g(l) in figures 2.9a&b). We will later show that this choice af g(l) will provide a lower (10) Recall the premature sumess argument used in fomulatig the state traitin -44- equation for , (eq. 2.6). bound on the expected error made by the TCA. as a normalized measure of the difference between We define the quantity Al=[E(Ilk)-1,3/L. the expected first success level starting from k and the threshold 12,3. Plotted in igures 2.13ab&c is Al versus k/L., 50 and 100, (solid line) where L,,. for L==25, defines the point density of gj.(l). The dotted lines represent + one standard deviation about the mean. If AI were zero then the success level would equal l23 and be independent of the starting state. This occurs only for k = 1X3. Al is nonzero for k different from l2,3 Thus, the algorithm chooses as threshold a level that depends strongly on the starting level. For starting levels below l1a, Al is negative (threshold underestimation) whereas Al is positive (threshold overestimation) for starting levels above 123. As L, increases, AlI versus k/Lx approaches the (dashed) line AI =(k -12)IL= more dosely, i.e., the expected value of the threshold estimate approaches the starting state. This results from the fact that the algorithm is increased, thereby raising the probability of an intervening must traverse more states to reach 12,3as L. success. If we equate the range of k/L, (0,1) to any fixed range in dB sound pressure level then we see that as sound pressure level resolution is increased (by increasing L..), the TCA will produce an increasingly Therefore the TCA estimate of threshold is biased. poor estimate of 13 for starting levels away from 13. This bias depends upon the previous value of threshold, and this bias increases with increasing level resolu- tion. To show that gu (1) provides a lower bound on TCA estimation error, consider the following class of 8(l) fL'.. ga() = g where gm<0.5 and ,.>0.82. (1 1 ) m <L/LL < g /L,.l > 98 (2.44) Sudl g(l) that neither not monotonically approach zero with decreasing I nor monotonically approach one with increasing I are likely to exist in cochlear nerve fibers. The probabilistic nature of spontaneous activity imposes a lower limit on g (l) since even in the absence of aural stimuli (11) 7Tee boxum were determined in the previous ectia to atwe E(,) -45- = J". there is a nonzero probability of exceeding criterion( 2) and for high stimulus levels the fiber response may saturate such that an above-criterion response does not occur with probability one. Intuitively we see that the TCA estimation error associated with gt (l1) provides a lower bound for the TCA estimation error caused by 8,(l) as follows. For l <O, g,,(l)=O cess for stimulus levels 1<0 is zero. Likewise for l -L,, which I >Lu g,i,(l)=l, so that the probability of a false sucthereby making a success at a level for impossible as well. Thus, no matter how distant the starting state, k, is from 123, the thres- hold level, with gu.(l), the TCA estimation error, e, must be such that -l3<e<L,-l for g,r(l), 2 3. Conversely, there is always a nonzero probability of success at all levels. If a starting level k, is chosen distant from 12,l, there may be a sucess declared erroneously well outside the range allowable for gi, (l). Thus the magnitude of the expected error for g, (l ) can only be greater than that produced by gui (). In figures 2.14ab&c we present Al vs. kL,, three values of g and g (quantities as previously defined for figure 2.13) for (see caption for details). For reference, Al vs k/L. included in each figure (dotted line). For all values of L,. and all values of g, g8,h(l) provides a lower bound for the expected error magnitude IAl I. for gi.(l) is also and gm we see that Experiments to Examine TCA Transient Behavior Topical Outline In this section we examine properties of the TCA suggested by the transient behavior of the Markov model. We will cover four topics which conaern TCA IRC estimation error. These are: 71CAErrors in Estimating Neural IRCs TCA Errors Caused by Correctionfor the Acoustic System Frequency Characteristic (12) In tbe abene of aural timuli, the average rates in the -window and df-window c the tmeburst pcse wlMbe idenical. Amning 50 ms on and off windows, a TCA criterion of C =0, and an approdmately Poisson probabiity disibutio of spike mccurnnces (Frem, 1976; Litlefield, 1973), for spcntanemorates cf 10 qpid/asecand 40 spikedwc the robbilities of exceeding criterion, Pob(N.,-Nf >0), we .27 and .4 pectively. This probability approaches 12 as sqontanus Rae -x. Miscellaneous Observations TCA Errors Caused by Non-monotonicg (l) TCA Errors Caused by Neural Adaptive Effects In addressing these issues we will use Monte Carlo simulations of the Markov model. In most cases the linear, probability of exceeding criterion function, 8u, (1) will be used to estimate the g (1) of cochlear neurons. This function supplies a lower bound for the average TCA error. TCA Errors in Estimating Neural IRCs Qualitative descriptions and simulations tracking of a frequency-invariant IRC As an introduction to TCA estimation error we consider the TCA as it estimates threshold for a very simple IRC; one that is invariant with frequency. We introduce the constraint, however, that the first level presented by the TCA is substantially different than threshold. In Part I we showed that the expected value of the TCA threshold estimate is not necessarily the threshold level, l. Instead, if the starting level, k, is different from 123, then the expected value ot the threshold estimate will lie between k and 12,3. In figure 2.15a we show schematically that starting at a level (X) above threshold (solid line) the TCA estimates threshold (0) and then uses this threshold estimate as the starting level for the next frequency. Since the initial starting level is above 12,3, however, the expected TCA threshold estimate will also be above 123. Thus several frequency steps will be required for the TCA threshold estimate to converge to l.2/3 This property is illustrated for an initial starting level below iz/3 in figure 2.15b. Notice that the estimation error magnitude will be larger in this ase since, as shown theoretically, for starting levels lower than threshold the expected value of the threshold estimate is larger than for comparable starting levels above threshold. Since the IRC depicted does not vary with frequency, we see that the TCA has produced an artifactual component in its estimation of the IRC. Simulated TCA estimates in which the initial level was above and below threshold (dotted line) are shown in figure 2.15c, thus validating our qualitative argument. The TCA will produce an artifactual cornm- -47- ponent in its IRC estimate if an initial starting level is chosen much different than threshold. This com- ponent is larger for initial starting levels below threshold than for initial starting levels above threshold. We extend this result in the next two sections and show how it will cause predictable errors in the estimation of V-shaped neural IRCs. trackingof a linearIRC Iso-response contours of cochlear neurons are roughly V-shaped. They may be crudely approximately by two line segments in the frequency-level plane: a low-frequency segment where threshold decreases with increasing frequency and a high-frequency segment where threshold increases with increasing frequency. To assess the accuracy of the TCA in measuring such contours we shall first consider the performance of the TCA in tracking a linear contour which is an approximation to one segment of a neural IRC. figure 2.16a indicates schematically the types of errors expected of the TCA given the transient response properties of the Markov model. Consider first a segment with negative slope (figure 2.16a). Suppose that the first threshold point chosen is exactly l2/3. Then the starting level at the next (higher) frequency will be above threshold. Thus, the threshold estimate at this frequency will be above threshold as discussed in the previous section. As is depicted, we would expect this process to continue and the average threshold levels chosen by the TCA to be above the IRC. We would expect this pattern to be reversed were the IRC of positive slope as depicted in figure 2.16b. On the average, threshold levels would be chosen by the TCA which were below threshold. In addition, the expected error would be larger as indicated (also as discussed in the previous section). In order to systematically study the effect of IRC slope upon TCA estimation error, we studied the behavior of the Markov model with a Monte Carlo simulation (hereinafter called simulation). IRCs ,z(f), of slope a were obtained from the equation z(f) = alogldJ (2.45a) where the set fi is composed of consecutive frequencies that are fixed fractions of a decade apart (i.e., 1/40 decade). Thus a determines the difference in level between adjacent threshold points. The function g ( Jf) was of the form g (l ,f )=g (I -z(f )) with -48- 1max +L7m 2a 2 Lm a g(l)= 1 3 ma l2-L ,, 0 (2.45b) otherwise This particular form of g(l) was chosen so that threshold at any given frequency corresponded 12/3=z(f) (threshold is 12/3and g (0)=2/3). For a given set of (Lm x, a), using the TCA simulation twenty five estimates were made of an IRC of slope a. The first point on the IRC was chosen as the TCA starting point. This set of measurements is described graphically for a =5 and Lm=100 in figure 2.16c (only four estimates are shown for clarity). To quantify the error made by the TCA, for each a an average IRC estimate was computed. The average difference between this estimate and the known IRC divided by Lmax was defined as the average error. In figure 2.17 we plot average error ±s.d. as a function of normalized slope for L,=25, (1 3 ) 50, 75 and 100. Immediately apparent is the fact that error is greater for positive slopes than for negative slopes and that this asymmetry increases with increasing L (increased level resolution). In addition we see that for positive slopes the error grows more quickly with slope magnitude than fr negative slopes. Thus, for greater IRC slope magnitudes we would expect larger differences in estimation error between TCA estimates using positive and negative frequency increnents. tracking of a V-shaped IRC To illustrate the types of errors which may our in the TCA-estimation of neural IRCs we construct an IRC grossly similar to a neural IRC by joining an IRC of negative slope to one of positive slope as depicted in figure 2.18a. For such a V-shaped threshold vs. frequency contour we would expect the TCA when starting at low frequency (low-high) to underestimate the true threshold on the high frequency side and overestimate an the low frequency side in a manner consistent with the previous discussion of TCA error vs. IRC slope. If the algorithm were to start at high frequency (high-low) on this same contour, we would expect this (13) The last 80% of the estimate was used to avoid the transient effects asociated with stating the TCA at a level much different than 2l as discussed in the previous ection. -49- error pattern to be reversed. The general shape (high and low frequency slopes) of the true threshold contour is preserved in the estimate although it is slightly distorted near the low threshold tip. Such distortion may lead to a systematic error in estimating the characteristic frequency and Pro, the sound level at the characteristic frequency. The overestimation of Pa, may lead to an underestimation of QOs(14) which we will call tip- blunting". This effect may be intensified for IRCs resembling high-sensitivity neural IRCs where the Q near CF is high (Holton & Weiss, 1983b). Such an IRC is approximated in figure 2.18b. We see a substantial decrease in Q near CF in the qualitatively predicted TCA IRC estimate. We used a neural IRC archetype with a Q1 /s near CF of 2.5 and high and low frequency slopes of 80 dBldecade and -40 dB/decade respectively (Holton & Weiss, 1983b). For the simulation we used standard level and frequency steps of 1/4 dB and 1/40 decade respectively. Using the average value of g (l) slope, a =.08/dB (table 2.3), L,=50. figure 2.19 shows a low-high, and high-low average IRC estimate produced by the tuning aurve algorithm on this archetypical IRC. As predicted the general shape of the IRC is preserved in the TCA estimate (the high and low frequency slopes of the IRC and the average estimate are similar), and when starting at high frequences the TCA will overestimate on the high frequency side of the V-shaped contour, underestimate on the low frequency side, and underestimate CF (1/40 decade). This error pattern is reversed when the TCA starts at low frequencies. In addition, Prm/ is overestimated ( an appreciable decrease in Qioa for both estimates ( 5dB) and 30% from 2.5 to 1.8 in the high-low estimate and 1.7 in the low-high estimate). Measurement of TCA errors for cochlear neurons ffects of initialstartinglevel Starting at high frequency and decrementing to low frequency, we estimated the IRC of a cochlear nerve fiber using a set of initial starting levels which spanned a 40dB SPL range. The results of .these meas- (14) Q.,, is defined as the ratio of the chraeistic frequency to the x dB bandwidth, where the x dB bandwidth as the difference in fquency between the fist poins to either ide of CF which are x dB above Pi,. -50- defined urements are shown in figure 2.20. The high frequency portion of the IRC estimate shows a component which strongly depends upon starting level in the manner qualitatively predicted and shown in simulation. If we assume that the IRC started at 70 dB SPL is most nearly correc then we see transient overestimation for the IRC estimate started at 90 dB SPL and transient underestimation for the IRC estimate started at 50 dB SPL. In addition the underestimation transient is larger than the overestimation transient. Regardless of the accuracy of the IRC estimate which was started at 70 dB SPL, however, we see that the TCA will produce IRC estimates with grossly different characteristics if the initial starting level is either much different than threshold. TCA error and IRC slope For each fiber, an estimate of the IRC was measured using the TCA with positive frequency increments (i.e., from low frequency to high frequency, low-high). For comparison, we used a more accurate IRC estimation algorithm CCA3 )( 1 5 ) to estimate the neural IRC as well. Another TCA IRC estimate was taken with negative frequency increments (high-low). The results of these measurements are displayed in figures 2.21a&b where we show the low-high and high-low TCA IRC estimates compared to the same TCA 3 estimate. Also shown is the difference between the TCA 3 estimate and the TCA estimates. We see the predicted error pattern of overestimation to one side of CF and underestimation to the other side. Results from 9 units with different CF are compared in figure 2.21c. Hre we plot the difference between the TCA 3 and TCA estimates on a frequency scale normalized to the TCA 3 CF. We see average overestimation errors of 3.5 dB and 2.3 dB on the low and high frequency slopes respectively. We see average underestimation errors of -4.5 dB and -9.6 dB on the low and high-frequency slopes respectively. To determine whether the Markov model roughly predicts the magnitude of these errors we recall that the average sope of g(l) was determined to be .08/dB (from table 2.3). We assume that this result holds for the units used here. Using 1/4 dB level increments as was done here this correspds igure 2.17b where L,=50 to L. =50. Looking to we see that using the linear approximationto g(l), gA(l), the maimum TCA (rther (15) The more aiute estimate used the .ame TCA algorithm except that 3 ucces each frequency. The tird success was declared threshold. We dtherere cell tis algarithm the in TCA ACCURACY IMPROVEMENT the CA 3 is a muc more anrate IRC etiatorn. -_51- dun me) we required at CA 3 . As we denribe later overestimation error is 2.5 dB (1/4 dB level increments). The maximum underestimation error is -4.0 dB. Thus, except for the underestimation error on the high frequency portion of the IRCs the Markov model predicts the approximate magnitude of the expected TCA estimation error. However, it should be recalled that the linear approximation to g (), g, (l), only provides a lower bound on the TCA average error. Thus, we find that the experimental results are consistent with the Markov model predictions of TCA behavior. TCA IRC parameterestimationerror The TCA and TCA 3 were used to obtain IRC estimates in the same unit. Parameters derived from these two estimates are compared in the scatter plots of figure 2.22a-e (28 units in 5 animals). figures 2.22a&b show that the low and high frequency slope estimates are, on the average, similar for TCA 3 and the TCA (Low-frequency: corr=0.76, slope=1.06 .17; High-frequency: corr=0.69, slope=.97 .19). This corroborates the result of the previous section where we saw that the TCA estimation error was constant at high and low frequencies (see figure 2.21c). Overestimation of P, the fact that in only six of 28 cases does the value of Pr is seen in figure 2.22c as evidenced by taken from the TCA 3 estimate exceed that taken from the TCA estimate. The average Pm, overestimation is 2.8 ± 3.5 dB. figure 2.22d shows the TCA estimate of CF plotted as a function of the TCA 3 CF estimate. The low-high (ircle) estimate slightly overestimates CF whereas the high-low (triangle) underestimates CF. Far the most part the difference is on the order of 10%. The Qiod derived from the TCA estimate is plotted against the Q10dB of the TCA 3 estimate in figure 2.22e. The TCA estimate consistently has a Qloaw smaller than that of the TCA 3 estimate. Quantitatively, the average Q0lo 1.79±.4. of the TCA estimates is 1.19±.35 whereas that of the TCA 3 estimates is This is difference of approximately 30%. Thus the TCA performs as predicted for alligator lizard cochlear neurons. High and low frequency slopes are estimated correctly on the average whereas Pi is overestimated, CF is slightly over or underes- timated depending upon whether the algorithm uses positive or negative frequency increments respectively, and Qe0, is underestimated. -52- TCA Error Caused by Correction for the Acoustic System Frequency Characteristic Qualitative description The spectral properties of the acoustic system may cause artifactual components to be generated in TCA IRC estimate. Consider the cascade of the acoustic system with iso-response contour X(f), re 2.23a, and the ear with iso-response contour, H(f). obtain H(f) as shown in fig- Assume that X (f) is known but H(f) we would measure the response of the cascade logR (f)=logH(f is not. To )+logX (f) and then subtract logX(f ). For log/F(f) a line of constant slope (figure 2.23b), the overall transfer function and the expected value of the tuning curve would be as shown in figure 2.23c. However, if we subtract the known iso-response contour of logX (f) from this estimate we obtain the contour of figure 2.23d as our estimate of the threshold contour of logH(f ). This estimate is different from the real threshold contour of logH (f) which should be a line of constant slope. The acoustic system, X (f) has caused an artifact in the measurement of H (f ). We demonstrate this property of the TCA in a straightforward manner using a Monte Carlo simulation of the Markov model with L,=50 (corresponding to the average slope of g ()= .08/dB for cochlear neurons and level increments of 1/4 dB). Using the acoustic system and ear iso-response contours of figure 2.24a&b respectively, we show the cascade characteristic (logX(f)+log/f(f)), the average IRC cascade estimate (ow-high) and the average corrected estimate in figure 2.24c&d (25 trials). The corrected estimate obviously differs from the linear contour of figure 2.24b thus showing that the simulated acoustic system may induce a prominent artifact in the TCA iso-response contour estimate. Measurements on cochlear neurons The TCA 3 was used to estimate the IRC for a cochlear nerve fiber Ihis IRC and the iso-response contour of the acoustic system are shown in figure 2.25. Upon examining the TCA 3 estimate of the neural IRC we see decrease in sensitivity of the fiber with increasing frequency in the region between 4 kHz and 6 kHz. This is similar to the iso-response contour used in the simulation (figure 2.24b). The acoustic system isoresponse contour in this frequency range is also similar to the acoustic system iso-response contour of figure 2.24a (between 2.5 and 4 kHz) in that it shows, in sequence, a decrease and increase that are comparable in -53- magnitude to the slope of the neural IRC. Such a combination of slopes caused error in the simulated estimate of figure 2.24d. We see a similar type of error in the TCA IRC estimate of a cochlear neuron in figure 2.25. This prominent error is marked with an arrow where we see underestimation of the IRC by approximately 15 dB. This error corresponds to the error seen in the simulation above 3 kHz (figure 2.24d). Miscellaneous Observations TCA errors caused by nonmonotonic g (l) qualitativedescription If g (I) is not a monotonically increasing function of , then the TCA may not find a unique criterion level. To illustrate, consider the g (l) shown in figure 2.26. If the TA chooses a starting level greater than 1., it is unlikely that criterion will be met. Therefore, the level will be raised toward lm. At this level it is even less likely that criterion will be met so the level wil be raised once more. It can be seen that the TCA in this regime will fruitlessly seek the desired criterion level of 12,3 by incrementing until the maximum level is reachded. measurementson a cochlcarneuron Evidence of nonmonotonic g (l) was found in one unit.( 16 ) The comparison of two IRC estimates (one incremented from low to high frequency (low-high) and the other from high to low (high-low)), revealed gross differences on the high frequency portions (figure 2.27a). The curves were measured using the TCA 3 . The large descrepancy in levels chosen by the algorithm in the two cases at high frequency prompted the measurement of g ()(17) at a point in this frequency range (figure 2.27b). g(l), after reaching a maximum of .8 at 73 dB SPL began to decrease. This form of g(l) can account for the deviation in the high-low and low-high estimates at high frequency as follows. The first level used by the algorithm will be the threshold chosen for the previous fre(16) It should be noted that although this pbenomena was found in only one unit, no ehaustive search for uch unlts was instituted. Thus the behavior of this unit may not be unique mung cochlear nerve fibers in the alligatr lizard (17) See Prected Valuesof E() andE(I,) UsingMmaemem of g(l). -54- quency. On the high frequency side of the contour since we increment from low to high frequency, this first level will be substantially below the true threshold since the fiber sensitivity decreases with increasing fre quency. Thus the algorithm will be operating on the lower level portion of g (1) and will likely choose a level close to the lower 12,3. The reverse is true when decrementing from high to low frequency. At high frequency the algorithm uses the high level portion of g (1) because even were the threshold chosen accurately at one frequency point the starting level at the next frequency point would be substantially above threshold since the sensitivity of the fiber increases with decreasing frequency on this portion of the IRC. Thus the level likely to be chosen in closer to ,,.(18) When the algorithm reaches the frequency region where g (1) is again monotonic (below 450 Hz in this case), there may be a 60 dB level difference to traverse between l, and threshold. We have previously shown that if the starting level used by the TCA is above threshold then the expected threshold estimate will lie between the starting level and threshold. This explains the continuous rather than abrupt transition between the high and low frequency portions of the high-low estimate. Thus, g(l) functions which are nonmonotonic can induce large errors in the IRC estimate produced by the TCA. It should be noted, however, that most sequential estimation algorithms which depend upon iteratively comparing a response measurement to a criterion level are subject to similar problems. Therefore, the possibility of a nonmonotonic 8 (1) should be considered before application of such an estimation procedure to the measurement of iso-response contours. TCA errors caused by neural adaptation qualitative description Tacit in the use of a Markov model for TCA behavior is the assumption of a stimulus-historyindependent neural response. It was assumed that a given tone burst had no effect upon the response to subsequent tone bursts. Here we present some evidence that at higher response criterion levels this assumption may be invalid and that the effects of neural adaptation may cause smal but noticeable TCA threshold (18) A maximum sound level of 90 dB SPL is imposed by the algithm -55- to avid damaging the ear. measurement error. As seen in figures 2.21a&b the CA produces a distinctive error pattern when applied to a V-shaped IRC: overestimation of threshold on one side and underestimation on the other. Consider the side on which the TCA overestimates threshold. The levels presented to the ear are substantially above threshold and it is possible that the nerve fiber shows adaptation with repeated suprathreshold tone burst presentations. Thus, a still higher level would be required to elicit the criterion response than if the fiber were unadapted. On the side where the TCA underestimates threshold, since the levels presented are substantially below threshold, we presume that our original assumption of stimulus-history-independence is valid. Thus, the TCA may elevate threshold by overstimulation. measurements on cochlear neurons To test the hypothesis that TCA overstimulation may cause a shift in the sensitivity of a neuron near the TCA-chosen threshold, we investigated the effects of suprathreshold and subthreshold stimulation on threshold in cochlear nerve fibers. We used the TCA to estimate a neural IRC. We then chose a frequency where the TCA was likely to have overestimated threshold (in this case on the high frequency IRC slope). Starting at a level below the TCA-chosen threshold, tone bursts were presented consecutively with 2 or 3 dB level increments. When threshold was reached the stimulus sequence was repeated until ten seconds of measurement time was spent at each level. Likewise, starting above threshold, tone bursts were presented consecutively with 2 or 3 dB level decrements until a point 2 or 3dB below threshold was attained. In both cases the average rate in the on and off windows was obtained at each level. Shown in figure 2.28a are plots of rate difference A vs. level for subthreshold (ex) and suprathreshold (triangle) stimuli in two units. Threshold was determined using the TCA with 50ms on/off tone bursts and a criterion of 0. There is virtually no difference between the two level curves for each unit. For other units the same experiment was performed with one modification; the measurement was made 10 dB above the TCA-chosen threshold. Shown in figure 2.28b are sub-"threshold" and supra-'threshold" rate vs. level curves for two units. In both cases the two rate vs. level curves are shifted relative each other by a noticeable amount. The cases shown here showed the largest shifts (~ 6 dB). For 19 units the average shift was 2.5 dB +1.5. This result suggests that as aiterion level is raised, the TCA may make errors in -56- threshold determination by artificially raising threshold through overstimulation. This effect does not appear large in the alligator lizard, however. SUMMARY (1) In the steady state the TCA estimates the level, N, and Nff such that g(12) = Prob(N,,(l)-Nof (l)>C). are the number of spikes recorded during the on and off windows of the tone burst stimulus and C is a constant. In the alligator lizard there is a relationship between the probability of exceeding criterion, g(1) and the average rate difference AX(l). Using tone burst with 50 ms on and off windows with criteria of C = 0, 1 and 2 we have found that A(Iz ) is approximately 20 spikes/sec, 40 spikes/sec and 60 spikeslsec respectively. In general, however, since the TCA holds probability constant, the rate at the level, 12,, is not necessarily as simply related in other preparations. Nonetheless, if similar spike generation processes are assumed for different preparations, then the TCA indirectly estimates an iso-rate contour by estimating a uniquely related iso-probability contour. (2) The transient response of the TCA causes the inaccurate estimation of an iso-probability contour. The magnitude of the inamcuracy depends upon the function g(l), the IRC being measured, and the frequency characteristic of the acoustic system which delivers sound to the ear. The primary source of these errors is due to the TCA choosing the first success level, 1,1, at each frequency thereby not allowing a steady state to be attained. Thus, the threshold estimate, 1,I at any given frequency depends upon the level of the first stimulus presentation at that frequency. underestimation of Q ( The inaccuracy of the TCA causes 309%),the tuning sharpness and overestimation of P,. (= 3 dB). Depend- ing upon whether positive or negative frequency increments are used, the TCA will either overestimate or underestimate CF respectively ( 10%). TICA IRC estimation error, however, does not (on the average) seem to affect the accurate estimation of low and high frequency IRC slopes. -S7- A CASE HISTORY OF TCA IRC ESTIMATION ERROR INTRODUCTION The physiological precursor of the neural action potential is the hair cell receptor potential. Thus, a comparison of the tuning properties of the receptor potential to those of the neural response could shed light on the trarnsduction process which converts receptor potential waveforms into action potential trains. To this end, Holton and Weiss (1983b) compared iso-response contours of the D.C. component of the hair-cell receptor potential with those of the average spike rate (1 9 ) of cochlear nerve discharges. Both were measured with TCA. The hair cell and neuron IRCs were similar in many respects. The similarities induded comparable low/high frequency slopes and comparable sensitivities. The principal difference cited was that the hair cell IRCs were less sharply tuned than the neural IRCs. Several possible sources of this difference were proposed by Holton & Weiss. We will demonstrate an additional factor that is likely to have contributed to the difference in sharpness. This factor is a result of the IRC parameter estimation error discussed in the section entitled 7CA Errors in Estimating Neural IRCs where we showed that the TCA underestimates Q, the quality of tuning. We will show that TCA error alone could account for the reported difference. In order to compare the two sets of measurmets, however, we will first examine the conditions under which the algorithm was used in each case. The neural iso-AX response contours presented by Holton & Weiss (1983b) were measured using a frequency point density of 100 points/decade and a level resolution of 1/3 dB SPL whereas the iso-D.C. con- tours were measured with a density of 10 points/decade and a level resolution of 1/4 dB SPL The level resolution difference is insignificant. The frequency density difference, however, is ten-fold. We therefore aspect that the accuracy of the 'ICA-estimated iso-D.C. contours is not as high as that of the iso-AX contours since high density in frequency results in greater accuracy. In order to quantitatively assess the performance of the TCA for both the receptor potential and the nerve discharge data we will need to determine (19) Akhougb thee cmtoum were measured with the TCA and ae thefore, was shown previousy that hee omtours are iso-ate ontours as well. -58 tricdty qealrlng, io.proluility emoun, it g (l), the probability of exceeding criterion at level I for both measurements. RECONSTRUCTION OF RECEPTORPOTENTIALg (l) We made direct measurements of g'(l) for 15 cochlear neurons in the alligator lizard. The dope of g(l) was 0.08/dB SPL +-.02 in three alligator lizards. The g(l) for the receptor potential D.C. component if we assume that the source of uncertainty has not been measured directly. However, we can estimate () in the measurement is a zero mean additive white Gaussian noise process, w(t), E (w (t )w (T))=Wuo(t -T)). of intensity W (i.e., Let us define the average value of the signal plus noise in the on-response win- dow as s,,, and sof as the response in the off-window. Letting Vo(l) be the receptor potential in response to a tone burst stimulus at level I we have, , - (vo( )+W ())d . (2.46a) A1+A2 .. sff ~ f f w(t)dt = 2 (2.46b) A where (0,Al) and (Al,l 1 +A 2) are the on-response and off-response measurement intervals respectively. and sof are derived from non-overlapping intervals and the process w(t) is white and Gaussian, Since s s, and So Gaussian random variable of variances W/A 1 and W/A 2 respectively. are statisticaly indepent Specifically, p (so~, 1 ) = 1rW · 2WAI (2.47a) P(sf)e= 6w 2WAI 2 (2.47b) Letting s, -ssff =x we may obtain g(1) as, 8g() = Prob(x>V) = ( W( L +) f, 2 Wo(1 A )) d 2 = (2ir)f -_ ,2-dr, a where = V -V(l) and V is the criterion votage used by the TCA. W( 1 +-) Al A2 -59- (2.48) The calculation of g(l) requires the specification of W, the noise intensity in the receptor potential . P cc. recording system bandwidth, Vo(l), the time constants A1, A2, and criterion voltage V¢. Holton and Weiss measured the dependence of the D.C. component on sound pressure upon level. Using results given in table 2 of Holton & Weiss (1983a) we represent Vo(l) for low level stimuli as( 2 0) Vlo." necarCF1 lo10v 0 ' PICm Vo(l)= where I is the sound pressure level in dB (=0 j (2.49) is defifiMas the sound level necessary to produce the cri- terion response voltage. W is obtained from the broadbantRMS noise level, VRJS, and the doublesided fre- -T r( quency bandwidth bw using the relation, W = V Mw (2.50) we may calculate W where VjRS is the brdadband, RMS&dise level and bw is the doublesided bandwidth of the receptor potential recording system. VS is .im..1~ be the "ad hoc" noise floor spedfied by Holton & Weiss (figure 9 (p221), 1983a) of .3 my and the bandddth, the measurement system used in the hair-cell a Weiss (1983a)). Using these patarmters and V, = (.1il, bw, 8KHz. 1=47 ms and A2=2 ms as with Methods:Iso-voltage contors (p210) Holton & .3 my, 1 my), g(l) at CF and away from CF for i in dB are shown in figure 2.29. In all cases the gQ>yirresponding to V, =.1 my shows a much more gra- dual dependence upon stimulus level the for other criterion voltages. EFFECTS UPON REPORTED RESULS Faor V, = .1 mv the slopes of the D.C. receptor potential g(l) are .07/dB SMP away, from CF and .04/dB SPL near CF. The off-CF g(l) slope is similar to the average neural g(1) slope and the lope of g (l) near CF is much shallower. Thus, were RP and neural IRCs to be estimated by the TCA with identical frequency point spacing and level increment size we would expect similar performance away from CF and much poorer performance near CF since shallow g(l) implies larger L and therefore greater eror magitude (see figures 2.13 and 2.17). As seen previously, error is substantial for the ICA estimate of neural IRCs at 40 points/decade frequency spacing and 1/4dB level increments and this error causes undescmation (20) The measurements presented in Holton & Weiss (1983a) do not extend to values of VO in the neighlbofamd of .mv due to meann m i aonideratins. Nonetblem we extrapola these resluts into s vola rane fr analyticsmpicity. of Q. The density of the D.C. RP contour measurement was 10 points/decade thereby increasing the difference in threshold level between each point. As shown previously, increasing the slope of the IRC under measure increases error (figure 2.17). We therefore expect poor fidelity of the TCA iso-D.C. contour estimate to the IRC and we would expect the poorest fidelity in the neighborhood of CF. The implication is that a sharply tuned D.C. IRC would be poorly estimated by the TCA algorithm at a V¢ =.lmv criterion level and the estimate would show decreased tuning sharpness, Q. A simulation of the receptor potential IRC measurement made by Holton & Weiss (1983a) was done on the archetypical neural IRC defined in TCA Errors in Estimating Neural RCs to quantitatively test whether the sparse frequency point spacing used could account for the difference in Q between RP and neural IRCs. We used the g(l) obtained in figure 2.29a&b with 10 point/decade frequency increments and 1/4 dB level increments. The result is shown in figure 2.30. An obvious decrease in Q is apparent. average Qm of the TCA IRC estimate( 1 ) is 1.52 ± .18 versus Q Quantitatively, the =2.5 for the archetypical IRC. The decrease in QOdB seen between neural iso-Ax contours and receptor potential iso-D.C. contours is from 2.34 to 1.2. Thus we have shown that it is possible for TCA estimation error to account for most of the Qa10, difference seen between neural IRCs and receptor potential D.C. IRCs in the alligator lizard. IMPROVING TCA ACCURACY MODIFICAI7ONOF TA PARAMEERS In order to minimize inaccuracy in the TCA iso-probability contour estimate we must consider the source of the inaccuracy. We have seen that as the number of level steps to reach threshold from a given level increased, the expected error also increased. Conversely, taking larger steps in level, though it decreases inacraacy, obviously will decrease level resolution and thereby decrease precision. There are at least two ways to minimize inaccuracy in the TCA iso-probability contour estimate without sacrifiDig precision. The first consists of a modification of a parameter in the TCA If we assume that any . TAh iso-probability contour is continuous in frequency, then by choosing frequency increments snal enough we tlur it (21) The Q was mesured for twenty TCA muated estimtes. rbt Evmge Q was the mean t4AU- faE -61- te meaemme. are assured that successive threshold points will lie dose to one another in level. Therefore, once the algorithm finds a good threshold estimate at one frequency the expected values of the subsequent estimates are assured to be dose to the 123 point. Choosing smaller frequency increments diminishes the difference in level between adjacent frequency points. In terms of the model we have proposed for the ICA, this is equivalent to decreasing the apparent slope of the IRC being measured. We have previously shown that TCA inaccuracy decreases with decreasing IRC slope (figure 2.17). Another approach would be to modify the TCA so that instead of choosing the first success as threshold, the mA success is chosen. In examining figure 2.13 we suspect that IE((._l)1I8)-23JI > IE( I)-l2,31 (2.51) or in words, the expected value of each successive threshold estimate at a given frequency is closer to the true threshold of 12,3. m could be chosen to achieve any desired accwracy. This suspicion is borne out in simulation. Shown in figures 2.31 and 2.32 are E (, lo) for m =2,3. As in figure 2.13 the standard deviation about each point is shown. As m is increased further, (figure 2.33, m =5) E (, lo) approaches l, much more closely. This property was exploited in the development of the TCA 3 IRC estimator used previously in this chapter (m =3). We have verified the superior performance expected of the TCA 3 for ten cochlear neurons in the alligator lizard by measuring g and AX at several points on the TCA IRC estimate and on the TCA 3 IRC estimate. The results are illustrated in figure 2.34 (see caption for details). The TCA 3 is more effective than the TCA at holding g and AA constant as a function of frequency. This increase in accuracy, however, is achieved at the expense of an increase in the required measurement time. Using the test circuit, we found that the ICA estimates sixty points of an IRC in an average of approximately 60 seconds. The TCA 3 requires approximately 150 seconds to estimate sixty points along the same contour. COMBA7TING TCA ERRORS CAUSED BY CORRECTION FOR THE ACOUSTIC SYTEM FREQUENCY CHARACTERISTIC We have seen that the tuning curve algorithm is sensitive to the characteristic of the amustic system which delivers sound to the ear. Either of the aforementioned techniques used to minimim TCA error would effect a reduction of the IRC estimation error caused by the acoustic system. A more efficient means -62- to combat this effect, however, would be to nullify the effects of the acoustic system characteristic, H(w) by preceeding it with a filter whose transfer function is H-l(w)=1/H(o) thereby making the cascade indepen- dent of frequency. This is easily accomplished under computer control if the acoustic system characteristic, H(w), is known. Assume that the TCA normally controls Vi,, the magnitude of the sinusoidal input voltage to H(t). If at angular frequency w, the TCA premultipliesV, by I/IH(w) to produce Vj=Vj,/nH(o), then for fixed V, the output magnitude of the acoustic system depends only upon the input, Vi,, and not upon frequency. Thus, the premultiplication of VL, by 1/]H(w)J effectively removes the acoustic system from the cascade. Although this technique has yet to be implemented and tested in this laboratory, it is felt to be the most efficient and effective means to eliminate acoustic system induced artifact in the TCA IRC estimate and it is certainly simpler than physically constructing an acoustic system which has a frequency invariant characteristic. -63- SUGGESTION FOR FURTHER STUDY In the case of both the improvements, to the TCA mentioned in P7CA Error Minimization", accuracy is increased at the expense of increased measurement duration. In the case of increased frequency resolution, more points must be taken to cover the same frequency range. For m >1, we must wait for m successes to occur. Thus the question arises; how can we achieve the greatest amount of accuracy in the least amount of time? One method is to implement an algorithm which uses all available "information" to estimate the set of points (I J) which describe an iso-response contour. Information" in this sense includes all a priori knowledge of neural response properties such as the slope of g (l) and the general shape of neural IRCs as well as the additional information supplied by the response to a probe toneburst. Synthesis of such an optimal IRC estimation algorithm, even if the implementation were not technically feasible would provide a bound on how well any algorithm could estimate an IRC with given accuracy. -64- v C) 0- a , a m 2w aD t . -S I '9 la8 I: -65- . m - R§ a' T-- - ~~~1. -i I \ _~~~~V I Ii - lFz' c c _L c'--'--- I - -66- a M c 4) $XkO L" CO CZ U) 3 I (1) 0 .a Ad f '6 o~~~~~~c g b L r C X 9*8C t if t 'L LX eh 8 fq 0t~-C 'tx -4-, -4-, u) -67- 1 1 .4 w m ci w 0 0 100 10000 1000 FREQUENCY CHZ) Figure 2.4: Frequency response caracteistic -68 of LTI filter in figure 2.3. 0 Co L. g ok \ em~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ N %i II~~~~ I.- .5~~ .~~~~~~~~~~t t= Q II2Zr E 3 _ H CA e L" o '~. I oC ~ ~'c v -69- IC a I * * N I ii a . a: n o(IS za: z a: NPI V) w :3 U1 W Lli Ij w LL 1 z 0-4 R i j: I I 1, I:~~~~~~~~~~~I-L 1 A I "i C33S/S3MIdS) > 31UU m -J w -70- -J = ICA A STI ---RESPONSE _· I ·I T 1 I I NOFF = 2 NON=S Figure 2.7: Schematic representation of the neural response to a tone-burst (see text). -71- , '- * - - (-) -3) (.+) a WA) Ok tat ) I. I. . . a() = DD i b() = uo O + 0U D + 0ou oc~)- V CuO + U OUj puuA dOC)= UUUl b Figure 2.8: Discrete-time discrete-state Markov models of the tuning curve algorithm at fixed frequency, a) Markov chain modeling TCA behavior at fixed frequency. g(l) is the probability of exceeding criterion a stimulus level I; b) Ergodic Markov subchain derived from the Markov of chain of part a) (see text). -72- + p(l) g(I) ° P (ll success) 2.I .I . a .S * I *I e I ___ a * a I-1 I. .S - I · 2.3 - .3 I I eI b . 9*^ . ** a 8 , '~- 09 ~ .e. -.~ S ,I~,, I w . . . U 1.I . . w w - . '. . . :. .S · .I ].W .1 * 0.e-.. * P .5 C . . * .9* e a -- A .5 . . I . e I .e ! I I S I .$ I I .9 I/L max Figure 2.9b&c: Demonstation that E(I) and E(I,) lie near 12. Numerical dterminations of p(I) and p,( Isuccess) for five model g(l) plotted as a funtion of /Lm,. p(l) - (plus), p,(l) - (diamond). Along the abscissae we have put diamonds and plus signs at the points E(l)/L,n and E(, )/L respectively. These quantities were derived fromnthe probability distibutions shown. With the exception of part e) the points are sufficiently dose so as to be indistinguishable. Perpendicular lines are drawn from the points E(l )/La, and E ()IL,.m to (). 12/3is cired. (L,=20); b) g(l) = g(!)/L.m (L=50); c) g() = [/Lm -73- - (/Ln) a) (l) = /L 2 ]~ (L=50); + p ) SS) l I succe o p .! .t I g(l) _ 4 _ · .5S d 1.0 .2A ]e3' F e 0 4 .! i . I L . -- - - - , , I I.B W -- I/L max Figure 2.9d&e: d) g(l) = -1-[1-(/Lx) (Ln=20). 2 (Lax=50); -74- e) g(l) = I/L,m, l-14, 0 othrwise 0 m 0 l- (n 4 I- a) II 4 0 LJ I 00 L_ -PM 00 - Ar N 0 II .- 0 () 0 (1 0 -e I 0 (0 CD 00 0 0 0 0 I II I I I I I I I I I I I I I 0 00 .g . tE UD 0) 0.0 II 4N L (b I I 0Nl- I I I I I I I I I 0 I iL CD (ID (EP) 19AtI I -75 -4--i I I I 0 00 .iC 114 o(013) m 'l] I.b_ SNwe lb _Ztf; I a I I I I I I p- 0 m ad U) I- a) L. I o L..+ I i I I (t0q 00 0 lb ! t E.5 0 0 0 - I I I I I a, rii jj d .F oo c Ib I I I I I I I I I I J -0 L n -0D E m (0 (D -0j 00 1I <b , - LO 1 I , . 0 ILO (EP) I I . I 't I I LO I@A@I -76- I - I i 0 1tj O @& (4 ;1 L i J , J ~ i i i t J J _ . c 00 0O ( D _ U) 'Ii g.~S 1 11 - CO Q U) aD 01) 0- Oz q4 U) :3 1! O O 0) c 4C _I 0 co I / 1I 0 O · 1 0.,d- · I · I C \_ t-%- · 0 O N (3s/ds) G4Ddl MOpU!M--;;O -77- **I CL U gm ill *g I ' ' - _ O _ It xX - O D I I I I I I I I I (o<(i)yv)qod -78- ' ' ' C, !I u _n U) i4- -/ 'b <~ § .o~~~~~~~. 't a m I II ) i C~~~j xac 1. _ C) 1 p3E I ,, i,) · XCho i * I I I I I I I I I I O on -- , O dr A ri xo t C-) 0(0 (n Do 0 d ^ U) a3) l ^o f vl 4 -o0- .4 0 00 C d- C\ (o<(I) vv)qoJd -7?- - 0 04| oo *g5 P r8 S. C I I I i I I I I I I 'ill .IO -0 00 0 a a) 0 o 0 4 (D \ 0 i a) D -o0 v/) I I I I C D oO - 00 d . I I 0 I I I I q- 0 Co C * * * ( L<0)yV)qo d -80- I 0 o 0 N d ,S II II i I I I I I I I I I I is 0 B 11 0 [] a) 0 1 Ix o U) CU a) riS °tq d- 4 o0 X9 I O · TI ·I 00co Io I I i I I (D d dC (z<(I) vv)qo-d B ja_~ 0 I 0C; W. 3 v -81- :t g. 11 4t, u) 0 0 0 8- I 1 II 0 I lS d ,_ x 0 CD E i .|{ :8 Q 1 II +1 0 0 u) i vi u3 0 L~~~~~~~~ 0 It Lfl~~~~~~~~~~~~~ iC- -82- I I, - 0 LO 0 O) Ui) i.k - 1 0 0 .4 11 A CD CD cr x E J I IIi II a 0 .i O L O)0 O L tO 1 U i e. EDB . . -83- 8 AD LO 0 0 0 0 In 1 *1S 0Co II j [ ,3 q-k (0 (0 d x E I fiii " Ij1IIi 1 WI II = 0 0 U 0 , IC) 0 IC) .. I I t -84- E N L) " 7A oC) 0O Cdc tO L) 00 0 oo oo to x E +cl o b I (0 Lu) 11 II x BEa E -J 06; 0 AS 1-1 I I o C I -85- %- l +I b e .n aD LO 0 \ kI LO 0 Co ii I 0 .sI b 0 (D " H x a E 0 0 ad 3 Ec .BE O ^l M 6 C; a dd~~; Od00 C\. ll 4 §0 00 gB -86- a P N Ur O LO oo od L* 0 IC vI 6 vd 0 0 acg to x i bi E I (,g S" o t h 0 0 . - 0 ,..l 0 0 c 1 ;o B Jo u 0 I .NIT -87- start q) ii__ _ · ___ _ i __ (a) Q) frequency · (b) - ) start start - - rg _ frequency Figure 2.15a&b: Schematic and simulation of TCA error dependence upon IRC initial starting level for a frequency invariant IRC. Circles- points chosen by the algorithm as threshold, X- starting level at a given frequency. a) Initial starting level above threshold; b) Initial starting level below thresabold. -88- I II I I II II II I I II 11 1 0 d ;MIto 1E o At C) U) Q) -- 0 E 2. -C c . IB cr I -i 1 I I I I I I i i 0 I I 0 I I I I I I I1 1 0 0©- I 8: .4i I (BP) leAlI (4 t B rt 10 m .4 stort a) (a) Q) I frequency .4 (b) Q) Q) tart frequency Figure 2.16a&b: Schematic and simulation of TCA error dependence upon IRC slope. Cirdes- points chosen by the algorithm as threshold, X- starting level at a given frequency. a) negative slope; b) positive slope. _-n- c0 r& rn Go 11 _ I I I I I I I I I I I 0O: :3 LjI I 0 ;3 'a - G) cn __ C L- 0, 0 t 110° 0 II C)~~C q at E O C Q) II IJo I 0 0 I CI 01 I I I I 0 LO) I (8P) ()z I I I 0O _- "S oe ,e 7e ; V] CLa L Lj 8 W I u . .,,,,. x E -p- - Lr) IC) 0 0 0 a6DJ@AV I ~~~~~~~~~r 0 I JoJJ ram ee "' J: t,,,q -92- 8-8 VS LUD D -o la o 0*1 x o 0 w %-. i I 0 Lf 0 0 JOij@ a 6DJ@AV4 Ln _ I X 0 D H9 P. .q 'R :! 0 .9 U) U x o E o0 i f LO . r", m I L") 0 0 0 "' I 0 I JO.Ja 96DJ@AV -94- 0 i.u ; . ._ #~~~. -.la 8(I1 L - U _0 E i a n Ea m '.-I LO 0 a4DJ G5DJOAV w Ln It I -95- I I Srt 75 (a) a) J frequency !. stort ) (b) q) ) frequency Figure 2.18: Schematic of TCA estimation error for V-shaped IRCs. Solid line: IRC. Dashed line: TCA estimate. Xs: starting level at indicated equency; 0: TCA threshold estimate at indicated frequency a) moderately sharp frequency selectivity. b) sharp frequency selctivity. -96- A 0 0 0 b0 J. ' II 1 a I I - - mX ~ - ~ 1 * o X bo o) I I w -0 N o >a t a D 0: Za) :v ) (L) L f~~~.X I 0C I 0O0 (IdS I I I 0CDO 0 0 r-. LO I 0 SP) I@GA@1 -98- Q vZ 100 -J ~~ ~ ~ ~ I _ __ , I __ .Ir ___ g-__ 80- 0) 0 60- (9 40- c) 20 _ __ w| _ 100 __ vs w_ w w 1000 10000 frequency (Hz) I 20- . . . . . . fI fI I ..... 0 C 0 L_ Co -v -20- . .. 100 . .. . 1000 ... 10000 frequency (Hz) Figure 2.21a: TCA estimate comparison to the TCA estimate in alligator lizard nerve fibers; Upper panel: TCA (dotted) TCA 3 (solid line), lower panel: difference between TCA and TCA3 estimate; Positive frequency incremets. -99- I 100 -J 80 03 () 12, 60- . . . . . I · · I ·· 40£n 100 - -'7-1000-' 100 1000 10000 frequency (Hz) I 20 . . . . . . .... I . m a-) C 0- L_ 4- V1 -20 100 1000 10000 frequency (Hz) Figure 2.21b: TCA estimate comparison to the TC estimate in alligator lizard nerve fibers; Upper panel: TCA (dotted) TCA 3 (solid line), lower pane: difference between TCA and TCA estimate; 3 Negative frequency increments. -100- I 4 I II I J I ' 1 I I Il I - 0- 20 CD 3 O -101- 1 9_ -20- -30 -A /lr meon error 1.0 frequency (re TCA3 CF) I I I 10.0 I 7 - co 0 L-20 I - 20 3 0aD - 10 I 0 I -20 - 0 i mean error -,n -/./V j -O I -10- OL - I I I 0.1 IU -10 I i I 4 n , -Io I V l_ I I I I I _ --if% - -I 10.0 0.1 1.0 frequency (re TCA3 CF) Figure 2.21c: Difference between TCA and TCA 3 for nine fibers and average diffcrenc as a funcion of frequency referenced to CF of TCA 3 IRC estimate (see text for explanation); Upper panel: positive frequency inrements; Lower panel: negative frequency inacreents. -101- Q 16 I I Im I I I I I I 0 I I - _ -o _0 4 o n 4 4 ; l o 4 I 0 "%.an rr3 : 0 0 I 0 I 0 00 d0 (a Np _ I 0 O0 I I 0'4- 0CO 0C) 114- Ii0.Gd '1- a< -I c. 1 -82 1 (apooap/sp) adols -102- Ad VOd 41) 8t '"I I t r I I~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~. i iU 0 0'e '0 -Cq) o ala II0_ He ' I 0 I, I 0 0 0 p/8p) @dols -103- , 0b I I I I (@pD , 41VOI . *if (X e D L I I II I I I I I I I I I I I I __ IL I II o 4 o~ 4a 4 l 4 4 4 III I O(ds - -........................... -- I II I I I I I I' I I 1 -- I I I I GP) " d VOl -104- -l 0 N or, M I 8 Cini; m i Io- S C 1i i I 0 0 0 (ZH>) 0 JO 0 0 I . . VO1 P -105- I g - I [ Is I. a~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ 0408 la 4 m - 4 r) o) a I ;p ii e oz I" c1S I * 0 0 ,s 4?=- Ipo vol -106- I 0 0 0. F- 8*I c~ .EJ 9 il .5 ' (v) levas level (h 1 I tq 0 ! (c) (dG) Ic6le 4 Figure 2.23: Schematic of error caused by correction for the acoustic system frequency characteristic. a) Acoustic system iso-response contour. b) IRC to be measured c) Iso-response contour of IRC and acoustic system in cascade (solid line). TCA cascade IRC estimate (dashed line). d) TCA cascade IRC estimate corrected for acoustic system frequency characteristic (dashed line in c) minus contour in a)). The arrow points to the most prominent error caused by the acoustic oorrection. -107- o r I 1 r | I 0o 0o N r- r 0 :3 U a, a,- D , N 0 0 oLu) 0 N4 I I I L. #4- I #- 0 oIN (eP) (BP) I9AI t 0 Ci I 0 In I IJAJ ol I 0 .8 II o _- o N N 2: cC0* c x Cr a, U) L. 44 0 L. 0 -1 ; o ( 8 P) I I I In 0o (8p) (BP) I^Al -108- IAI to I I uUeI I I I I I I I I I I I I C t) ,(\0 E a) 0 CT In V~~~~ -4a~~ 0 < 0o 00 I I (D Or Q Q) w 6C-) I y 0 ir t-I *:IiS< N 4-. I .i O Q) L- 4- iii 0 I 0t I I 0cNl I I o I I 0 1X! I C\1 (dS 8P) -109- 19t@1l fZ ao X scI) l Ow4 0 I Is to Lm~& Figure 2.26: Schematic of a pathological nonmonotonic g (I) (see text). -110- 80 v) 60 _V 40 20 _ 1 · ____ 1000 100 frequency (Hz) Figure 2.27a: Effects of nonmonotonic g(l) on neural IRC estimation. High-low and low-high TCA2 IRC estimates as shown for a ochlear nerve fiber in the alligator lizard. Frequency inacrement: 1/40 decade, level incrcnant: 1/4 dB. 1.0 0.8 X X x 0.6 X g(1) X x 0.4 x x X X X 0.2 x 0.0 50 60 70 80 90 100 level (dB SPL) Figure 2.27b: Effects of nonmnotonic g(l) on neural IRC estimation; Noamonotonic g() f =530Hz for unit whose IRC( are shown in part a) of this figure. -111- at .4 dL ^^ IUU I I I - l 0 80- N a) 60 C, 40e< 20old - 80 70 90 100 110 level (dB SPL) 120: 0 100- O 80 - Q. <n 60- us A suprathreshold x subthreshold 4020- n. v TCA threshold - III 60 I I I I I - I I I I 70 I I I I I I I I 80 lIl - Il 90 level (dB SPL) Figre 228a A vs. level for suprathreshold and subthreshdd stimuli (see text for description). Threshold (AX(l23) 20 piessc) is marked by an arrow. -112- 1111111111111111111111111111111 100 - A x 0 80 - C, 0~ C, e< 6040- u, 20- 0- threshold ...i !,I. i. . . . 80 70 . . .. ii.... +10OdB ...........................I .. .. .I I 90 100 level (dB SPL) ! ,.A n~ I = ! I I OU 0 'I) 0) (n 0, 1< 6040 a suprathreshold x subthreshold I i 20 80r 80 I90 100 90 100 )dB 110 110 I 120 level (dB SPL) Figure 2.28b: AX vs. level for prae hold ad subthiresholdstimuli (see text for description). Measurament of figure 2.28a repeated at = 10dB SPL above the AX(12) = 20 spikes/sec theshold level. -113- A 1 I I I m I I ~~ ~ ~ . ~ ~ ~ I~ ~I I I I I LO ci I I -0 m L8 I ci ) I I I 00o I 66 I I I - co -114- I oN 0 I I 0 0 a> I.X. Ia I.l I I I I I I I I I I A 9 J-o I ci) I Is 13C >P E -0O a) E LL 0 I- 4- -0 0l I 0 to I 1 I c 00 I I I D d I I I Cl 0 C 0 666660 -115- I I~j I I - I - I I I I - I I I g. - o 'i~ji1 I . .v- 0 N TF _' O , 0 O 0 I O · 4 a: L- O 4- r . I o C I 0 - I I I I 0C I 0 (: (sP) I@A@ -116- I I 0 Ns 0 0 0 r I a u ",42li ,g0Z 1, II r 0 L0 6 LO I 0 'I1 r-J CD x 0 E II I * v LO II +1 w; 11 .4 .4 Y x E d d> 0 LO 0 0 0 Ln 0 i@ CZ -A = 3 -W i Lr 6 0 d~~*1 ) o I LO . 1 a3-UDe I o. o (0 CD oI E o , :X X <S C) 0 d 0dO 0O e jw, ,86~~~~Ij I 1 0 .E A~~& O 0 LO LO C) 6 I +l. II T- t adu .I TA (D D 6 I x E I rr ho A 8 . h1 aNIVI. Ng) 4 0 6 C:) . ^ 0 I) LO 0 0 10 0 I 'ii g . A -11 Q- -k ll II O UD 0 6 O~ ii I L I 0 (D (D 0 x E I Ii II1 '9qI II - LO II x0 11 E I 0 6 t) Ln C0 o IC I -1 9n- C) lai 00 1 1 . E00 o o I Ii CD o E c) r0 C) 0 0 0 l 11 t t LO) 0 <io~~~~~~i -z II 0 06o 0 0 LO II 6 Bo. To q co x E a~I . rcM s8i o I 11 n (o j. 0 0 0i Lf) 0 0 0 LO 0 I -122- 3i. ei +@11 X M - II , 0 LO 6 6 tO c1 C0 ft .I 0 CD (D x II o E I eg . a II~i Lnl C\J i5 x 11 E . 0c~ 06 To 1.0 0 0 0 LO 0 I s"; @ .0.=3 AD <1 1 %1 [i 6 6Lr)~~~~~ o o I. I - ~ (D - E ;-.11 a a x I 0 ¢b) LO 0 0 0 t) 0 r4 O I]<ji . 4 , a<'" Irk .8 -124- 6.1n0 I (0 I- I 0 o -i 11 C'b LO Ln (~~~~~1 LO . ti i aZ$ -12.5- 100 80O I 1n m I 60 40 Q 20. 1000 10000 frequency (Hz) 100 1.0- ------- 0.5 0.69 0.4 0.2 nn V. 0.5 1.0 2.0 frequency re CF . . . . . . . . . ·. . I. 60 0 0 a] I 20- _ 05 0.5 n. . L . ...... 1.0 2.0 frequency re CF Figure 2.34.: Depiction of measuremnts of g and Ai at points on an iso-responsecontour estimate. The frequencies used were .5CF, .707CF, 1.OC, 1.41CF, 2.OCF where CF is the characteristic frequency of the fiber obtained from the appropriate IRC estimate. Tone burst stimuli with To. = Tol = 50 as, Tr = 2.5 ms were used. In all cases both the TCA and the 'ICA 3 used a criterion of C=0, 1/4 dB level increments, -1/40 decade frequency increments and the starting frequency was 6310 Hz. Tbe initial starting level was 90 dB SPL; Upper panel: TCA IRC estimate (solid line). The triangles are 3 the points at which g and AX were measured. middle panel: g measured at points depicted in upper pane; Lower panel: AX asured at points depicted in pper panel -126- I . 1.U i. .. .. .. .. .. ..· . I I I I I I I I . · . .· 0.8 0.6 g 0.4 0.2 n n 0.5 I.U I I I a I a I I a I I I I I I 2.0 1.0 frequency 4 I re CF I I ,I I I I I I 0.8 0.6 9 0.4 0.2 nn %v.u 0.5 I I 1.0 frequency I I- 2.0 re CF Figure 2.34b: Comparison of TCA and TCA perfom ane. Meauts of g at points iso3 response ontour estimates. The frequencies used were always .5CF, .707CF, 1.0CF, 1.41CF, 2.0CF where CF is the charaderistic frequency of the fiber obtained from the appropriate IRC estimate. Tone burst stimuli with T = Tf = 50 ms, Tr = 2.5 ms were used. In a cases both the TCA and the TCA used a criterion of C =0, 1/4 dB level increments, -1/40 decade frequency increments and the starting fiequency was 6310 Hz. The initial starting level was 90 dB SPL; Upper panel: Measurements of g at points on TCA estimated iso-respose contours for 10 units. The dashed line is the threshold, 8 =2/3. Lower panel: same as upper panel only TCA3 was used. -127- I · ,., bU O0 C) (D I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 40 _a 0L 20 ,< <l 0 i I I I I I 0.5 I 2.0 1.0 frequency I 60 I t I I i I re CF I i I I I \Q) 40 n0 "/ 20 I TCA 3 0 I 0.5 I I I I I I I I I I I 2.0 1.0 frequency I re CF Figure 2.34c: Comparison of TCA and TCA 3 performance. Measurements of AX at points on isoresponse contour estimates. The frequencies sed were always .5CF, .707CF, 1.CF, 1.41CF, 2.OCF where CF is the characteristic frequency of the fiber obtained from the ppropriate IRC estimate. Tone burst stimuli with To= T = 50 50 ms, Tr= 2.5 ms were used. In all cases both the TCA and the TCA 3 used a criterion of C =0, 1/4 dB level increments, -1/40 decade frequency increments and the starting f'equency was 6310 Hz. The initial starting level was 90 dB SPL; Upper panel: measurments of AX at points on TCA estimated iso-response contours for 10 units. Dashed lines denote the expected range of threshold rate values. A range rather than a specific value is supplied since the TCA and TCA 3 are iso-probability contonurestimators and a unique value of threshold for rate may not be specified. We approximate the range of average rate difference threshold by examining the spread of average rate difference about g =2/3 in figure 2.12b and find that 18 spikes/sec < AX < 30 spikes/sec. Lower panel: same as upper panel only TCA3 was used. CHAPTER 3: THE DEPENDENCE OF NEURAL SYNCHRONIZATION UPON FREQUENCY INTRODUCTION Cochlear nerve fibers respond to acoustic stimuli by a change in their instantaneous firing rate. When this rate has a waveform whose structure is similar to that of the acoustic stimulus, the response is said to be synchronized to the stimulus. Synchronization is a basic property of the firing patterns of cochlear neurons in response to sound. (Kiang et al, 1965; Rose et al, 1967; Johnson, 1974; Littlefield, 1973; Kim & Molnar, 1979; Anderson, 1973). Synchronization, however, is limited. If the rate of variation of the stimulus with time is large, then the neural response will not contain the fine structure of the stimulus waveform. Specifically, in mammals, the synchronized response is limited to stimulus frequencies below a few kilohertz (Gray, 1966; Rose et al, 1967; Anderson, 1973; Littlefield, 1973; Johnson, 1980). We therefore ask: What coichlear mechanisms limit neural synchronization? Studies of hair cell receptor potential, a physiological precursor to the nerve fiber response, have shown the magnitude of the AC. component of receptor potential decreases with increasing frequency (Holton & Weiss, 1983a; Russell & Sellick, 1978). his attenuation of the A.C. componenthas been attributed to the low-pass filtering properties of the hair cell membrane (Weiss, Mulroy & Altmann, 1974; Russell & Sellick, 1978). However, it is not known whether the decrease of synchronization with frequency in the nerve can be fully accounted for by attenuation of the A.C. receptor potential. In order to address this issue, we have studied neural synchronization as a function of frequency for single cochlear nerve fibers in the alligator lizard using techniques sinilar to those used to measure the hair cell receptor potential A.C. component in this animal (Holton & Weiss, 1983a). 7he most systematic and quantitative previous study of synchronization was performed by Johnson (1980). Johnson measured the maximum level of synchronization as a function of frequency in codilear nerve fibers of the cat. This measurement technique is limited in that: 1) synchronization may not be studied as a function of stimulus level, 2) synchronization may not be measured at low levels and 3) the measurement is time consuming since first synchronization must be measured as a function of stimulus intensity in order to determine the saturated (and therefore maximum) synchronization level. In this study we have developed -129- two new methods of studying neural synchronization analogous to the methods used by Holton & Weiss (1983a) for hair cell studies. The first method allows the study of synchronization variation with frequency with out the complicating effect of average rate variation with frequency; we measure synchronization at constant average rate. This technique also allows the measurement of synchronization at low levels. The second method allows the measurement of contours in the (frequency, stimulus level) plane along which synchronization is constant (Littlefield, 1973). These contours are measured rapidly using a new synchronization estimation technique and an automated measurement algorithm. We have found that synchronized responses of low characteristic frequency (low-C) (1 ) fibers (CF < 900 Hz) and high-CF fibers (CF > 1 kHz) differ markedly; synchronization decreases with frequency much more rapidly for high-CF fibers than for low-CF fibers. In addition, we have found that synchronization in high-CF nerve fibers decreases more rapidly with frequency than does the AC. component of receptor potential (Holton & Weiss, 1983a) between receptor potential and the neural response for high-CF fibers. Thus, it appears that the receptor-potential/nervefiber-discharge transduction process behaves as a low pass filter. (1) The dcaratestic fequency (Co) is deined as the frequncy at which a minimmw stimulus level, Pm wi produce a threshold response. This definitim applies to any responsevariable. By high-CF and low-CF fibers, howeva, we specficlly mean fibers whoseaverage rate respcmseis most sensitiveat high and low frequendes,respectively. -130- METHODS In this section we describe the stimulus paradigms and methods used to measure the average rate and synchronization of cochlear nerve fibers. Detailed descriptions of the dosed acoustic system used to deliver sound to the ar of the animal, surgical preparation and neural recording in the anesthetized alligator lizard are described elsewhere (Weiss et al, 1976; Holton, 1980). Unlike Weiss et al (1976) and Holton (1980), however, the anesthetic used was de-ionized H 2 0 and Urethane (4:1 by weight, respectively). The dosage was .01cc per gram body weight. Immediately after surgery and periodically throughout an expiment, the health of the preparation was assessed by measuring the electrical response (at the round window) to acoustic clicks. The sound level necessary to produce a just discernable response is called the Visual Detection Level. The details of this preparation assessment procedure are described elsewhere (Holton & Weiss, 1983a). Animals with an initial VDL of greater than -55 dB were not used in this study and if the VDL during an experiment rose above -55 dB, the experiment was terminated. We maintained the body temperature of the animal between 200 and 21°C. S7IMULI The tone-burst stimulus is a sinusoid of amplitude, A, and frequency, f, multiplied by a periodic window function, W(t), of period T, duty cycle, 3, and unity amplitude as depicted in figure 3.1. The nonzero portion of W(r) is termed the on-window" and the zero portion the off-window". T, = Tolf = (1-P)Te. T, and The trapezoidal shape of W(t) helps minimize spectral contamination of the stimulus and thereby allows accurate measurements of frequency selectivity. he time interval spanned by the rising or fal- ling edge of W(t) is called the rise time, ,, of the tone-burst. Five parameters are required to specify a tone-burst stimulus: 1) Frequency f, ,. 2) Amplitude A, 3) Duty cycle, , 4) Period T,, and 5) Rise/fall time Parameter values of T, = 2.5 ms(2 ) J3= .5 and To = 100 ms are used in this study unless otherwise noted. (2) A rise time of 2.5 ms produces little pel 100 Hz in this study. contamination eragy above 100 Hz. We wi cly use fequeies -131- above NEURAL RESPONSE COMPONENTS A spike train will be represented as a regular point process with the instantaneous firing rate function, A(t). We will be concerned with two properties derived from this rate function; average rate and synironization. We now provide definitions of these measures. Derivations of estimates for these quantities are presented in Appendix B. Average Rate (3 ) In response to a tone-burst input we define average rate difference as the difference between the rates in the on and the off windows. Specifically, let A0 be the average rate during the on-window and let Xf be the average rate during the off window. Then we define the average rate difference to be Ak = A 0- Xo (3.1) . An estimate of average rate, i, is I, I ._ (3.2) where N is the number of events recorded in an observation interval, 1. Synchronization Synchronization is defined in the folowing manner. We assume the instantaneous rate, (t), in response to a tone is periodic with period T equal to the tone period. ThIus X(t) can be expanded in a Fourier series (en, 1970, Liu & Liu, 1975), A(t) = 2 Ae . (3.3) where k is an integer. We define synchronization, S, as the ratio of the complex amplitude, AI( 4 ) of the fundamental component of x(t) in the tone-burst ant-window to the average on-window rate, A, (3) Avee rate = f(t)dar for arblitrry t md obeatin interval,. (4) In this dvelpnat we treat the esimation of the compex quantity A&. This is impler dan e arg (Ai) a has been done previously (Johnm, 1974; Littlefield, 1973; Kim & Moiar, 1979). -132- _sitig A I ad S = A (3.4) The magnitude of this measure is identical to that used by Johnson (1974). It should be noted, however, that we estimate synchronization during the tone-burst on-window rather than for a continuous tone as has been done in previous studies (Kiang et al, 1965; Rose et al, 1967; Anderson, 1973; Littlefield, 1973; Johnson, 1974; Kim & Molnar, 1979). We discuss this difference in Appendix D. Briefly, we do not feel this difference should pose a major obstade in the comparison of our results with previous work. If we assume a bandlimited rate function, X(t), as described by h, X(,) where K = (3.5) .IT is the highest frequency component of X(t), then we may estimate Al by quantizing the tone- burst on-window into M consecutive time intervals of duration ?.( 5 ) For each of Q tone-bursts presented the number of neural spikes which occur in time interval (bin) m, n,, occuring in bin m over all tone-burst presentations, i.e., , = q1 is recorded. N. is the sum of spikes (n,,)q. Thus, the set N. comprises a PST histogram (Johnson, 1974) of spike occurrences within the tone-burst on-window. Letting MQ T=I, the total observation interval, we obtain M-1 A 1 -iIf -j2wm N. e e (3.6) 0 Isinc- T A measure of synchronization as defined in equation 3.3 is, s =A/(N + 1) (3.7) where N is the total number of spikes occurring in the on-window observation interval (N = C N,). (5) In this study was set to T/10. We assume that the mte _fulin. o(), harmonic(.=5). -133- ontins no harmonics ighMr thn the 5th Measurement of Iso-fundamental Contours To measure iso-fundamental contours, an automated algorithm similar to the tuning urve algorithm (see chapter 2) is used. P= - IA 12, the power in the fundamental component of the rate function, is used as the response estimate since it is unbiased for small observation intervals (please see Appendix B). If we that assume X(t)has no frequency components above the second harmonic (i.e., (t) well described by eqn. 3.5 with K.2), then the estimate of P is = ( [(No-Ne + (N-N 3)- N] (3.8) The N are obtained by subdividing the tone-burst on-window into intervals of length T/4 as indicated in fig ure 3.2. Thus, each tone cycle is divided into four equal intervals which we number 0, 1, 2, and 3. We record the number of nerve impulses which occur in each interval over the tone-burst and the sum of the spikes from intervals numbered i is Ni. N =No+N,+N2+N3 (6) Often methan one N is the total number of spikes which occur in the on-window, (6 ) nburst is usd in older to obtain a more acrte estimate of . in mch a ae the mpones to the Q tone-bursts,(N,),, are added togetherto fom Ni', and the obevation interval,T,, becomesQ,,. -134- RESULTS The response of a fiber to a low and high frequency tone burst is shown in figure 3.3. Both responses have similar average rates (AA=94 spikes/sec at 500 Hz, AX=88 spikes/sec at 1000 Hz). Howeve, the response to the low frequency tone-burst shows a large component synchronized to the individual cycles of the tone, whereas no such synchronized component is apparent in the response to the higher frequency toneburst. To examine this synchronization decrease in cochlear nerve fibers, two types of measurements were taken: measurement of iso-fundamental contours and measurement of synchronization at constant average rate difference. ISO-FUNDAMENTAL CONTOURS: Dependence Upon Fiber CF Iso-funmdamentalcontours for a given fiber were produced by estimating the power in the fundamental component of the firing rate, A, 12, during one or two tone-bursts and using an iso-response contour (IRC) estimation algorithm to determine the sound pressure level which produced a criterion response. Thus these contours are iso-A 1 12contours.7) Iso-fundamental (iso- A2) and iso-rate (isoAX) contours of representative low and high-CF units are shown in figure 3.4. For the low-CF units (fig3.4b), the shapes of the isofundamental and iso-rate contours differ only slightly whereas there are differences between the iso-rate and iso-fundamental contours for the high-CF units. For high-CF units (fig 3.4a), the synchronized component falls off rapidly at a frequency much lower than the iso-rate CF. Thus, the CF det ined from the iso- fundamental contour is much lower than that determined from the iso-rate contour. For low-CF units the iso-rate and iso-fundamental contour CFs are similar. These comparisons are smmarized in figures 3.5a&b where the iso-fundamental CF is plotted against the iso-rate CF for both low and high-CF units, respectively. As can be seen, the iso-fundamental CF for high-CF units is different from the iso-rate CF. In addition, the value of the iso-fundamental CF does not seem to depend upon the iso-rate CF (corr: 0.07, slope: 0.02) whereas the iso-rate and iso-fundamental CFs differ only slightly for the low frequency units (corr: 0.93; (7) A tuig crve algorithm(deumbed in cpter 2 as TCA3 ) was ed to estime io. lA I rtorin. Tirefore, to ioIA 12 contour i actually an iotpmrobaility ,i, ur with the probbiity of IA,12C equal to 2/3 where C the atedon. We ame that the probability 2/3 level arsp to a uique value d IA, 12 o al values of (see appedix B for autha detail). -135- slope: 0.9). The average CF of high-CF iso-fundamental contours was 346 ±90Hz (27 units). Thus, for high-CF units, the synchronized response sensitivity begins to decrease in the vicinity of 350 Hz and this frequency is independent of the fiber iso-rate CF. As a final note, for high -CF units the average low and high frequency slopes( 8) were -33.6 -10.8 dB SPIJdecade (min: 49.2, max: -18.6) and 74.0 23.2 dB SPUdecade (min: 40.0, max: 114.0) respectively. This result will be used later when we consider previous measurements of receptor potential iso-A.C. component contours. SYNCHRONZATlON AT CONSTANT AVERAGE RATE DIFFERENCE: DependenceUpon CFfor Low and High-CFFibers The fundamental component of the instantaneous rate of firing, A1, was measured at constant average rate, (9 ) AK, for low and high-CF units. The synchronized response, nitude taken. The resulting quantity, , (eqn.3.5) was estimated and its mag- A1 /A0, is the synchronization index. In figure 3.6a we plot syn- chronization index at constant rate versus frequency for representative low and high-CF units. Immediately apparent is the rapid decrease of synchronization for the high-CF unit at frequencies above 250 Hz vs. the more gradual decrease of synchronization with frequency of the low-CF unit. Since these data were taken in the same animal, this difference is not attributable to response variation between preparations. To determine whether the decrease of synchronization is dependent on frequency selectivity, we measured the frequency at which the synchronized response is 3 dB below the average maximum synchrony, . 3 ,(10) and compared f.3 to the iso-rate CF. Figure 3.6b shows f 3 vsus the iso-rate CF for 21 (8) To estimate the high. and low-frequency slopes, a linear regression was performed on 10 points above and below CF respectively. Ts mll number of points was used far the following reaon Iso-fundamental contours wre measured at high timulus levels (average Pin=73.7 6.8 dB SPL). To avoid damage to the ear, a maximum stimulus level of 90 dB SPL was impued. Only 15 of the 27 contours from which CF could be estimated bad the requisite number of points below 90 dB SPL to either side of (F. The large scatter observed in these meurm ts may have been prtially due to the man number of points used to estimate the slopes. (9) ao-X contomurwere measured using the aurate CA3 algorithm presented in Part111 of chapter 2. dhw that this algorithm does indeed hold A cunstant for cear nerve fiben in the lligator lizard. in appadix B we (10) Thef ,a point ws estimated by dwing straight l segments through the data (as in figure 3.6e) and noting the equency t hich the synchronized respome was 3 dB below the average low frequency maximum. The average low frequeney maximum was computed by aveingg the value of the sadunirtixim index for dt below 300 Hz. Thus, f wa aily estimated for hig;C units for which at least two synchronization data points below 300 Hz existed. This ad oe method for determining a low frequency reference syncronization level was based on the qualitative observtion that the ydmraimtion index approaches a low frequency plateau below 300 Hz (see figs 3.6d&e). -136- high-CF fibers. The mean f is 369 + 37 as compared to the mean iso-rate CF of 1674 ± 268. A slight negative correlation is seen between f3, slope is only -.1 indicating that f3, f3s and the iso-rate CF (correlation= -0.18), however, the regression and the iso-rate CF are approximately independent. Figure 3.6c shows versus the iso-rate CF for 16 low-CF units. The mean f 3a is 498 + 38 Hz as compared to the mean iso-rate CF of 285 - 108 Hz. The correlation between the iso-rate CF and f3ad is -0.32 but, as for the high-CF units, the regression slope is small (-0.08). These results indicate that for high and low-CF units the decrease of synchronization with frequency is not dependent upon the fiber CF. The pooled data from 39 high-CF units (7 animals) and 19 low-CF units (4 animals) are shown in figure 3.6d&e Synchronization decreases steeply above 350 Hz for high-CF units whereas it decreases much less steeply for low-CF units. A regression line, computed from the synchronization vs. frequency data above 350 Hz for high-CF units, had a slope of -41.8 ± 2.1 dB/decade. A similar line for the low-CF data (f >300 Hz) had a slope of -13.0 - 1.2 dB/decade. SYNCHRONIZATIONAT CONSTANT AVERAGE RATE DIFFERENCE: Dependence Upon AX for High-CF Fibers The measurements of synchronization at constant rate presented thus far were all made with AX 20 spikes/sec. For two units we were able to measure synchronization at constant rate for AAX 20 and 40 spikes/sec. Figure 3.7a shows synchronization as a function of frequency for these two units at Ai 20 spikes/sec and at at AK= 40 sp/sec. The general dependence of synchronization upon frequency is siilar for these units. Figure 3.7b shows pooled data from 9 high-CF units at the AX= 40 spikes/sec. The regression line fit to these data points above 350 Hz had a slope of -60.4 +4.4 dBdecade which is steeper than the slope of 41.8 dBldecade obtained for A=20 depended upon the choice of frequency, f, spikes/sec. To determine whether this apparent difference above which the regression line was computed, we varied f between 350 Hz (the qualitative "knee" for both sets of data in figures 3.6b and 3.7b) and 500 Hz (the frequency above which data for AX=40 spikestsec are scarce; <20 points) and found that the regression slope of the data in figure 3.6b did not vary by more than 3 dl/decade and the regression slope of the data in figure 3.7b decreased monotonically as a function of frequency to -81.2 8.8 dBldecade at 500 Hz. Thus it appears that, on the average, the slope of synchronization versus frequency at constant average rate for AX=40 spikey/sec is significantly (at least 14 dB/decade) steeper than the average slope of the lower criterion -137- measurement at AXh 20 spikes/sec. This result, however, differs from the finding that in single units (figure 3.7a) the synchronization vs. frequency slope does not vary greatly between the two average-rate criterion levels. To resolve this issue, further experiments must be performed in which syndlronization variation with frequency is measured as a function of criterion level in single units. -138- DISCUSSION NOVEL SYNCHRONIZATION MEASUREMENT SCHEMES We have developed two new methods of studying synchronization in single auditory nerve fibers: the measurement of synchrony at constant rate and the rapid measurement of iso-synchronization contours. Using these methods we have uncovered a phenomenon which appears independent of the average rate CF: a rapid decrease in the synchronized component above 350Hz. Both methods can be used to study the dependence of synchronization upon response level by adjusting the rate criterion for synchrony at constant rate, and by adjusting the criterion used by the iso-fundamental algorithm. Previous methods (Anderson, 1973; Johnson, 1980) did not permit such a study. Furthermore, the rapidity with which these measurements can be made permits the detailed study of synchronization in individual neurons. Previous methods due to their time-consumin nature (Johnson, 1980) did not permit as detailed study of individual units. RELATION OF NEURAL SYNCHRONIZATION TO RECEPTOR POTENTIAL A.C. COMPONENT: Free-Standing Region According to previous studies (Weiss et al, 1976) high and low-CF fibers project to two morphologically distinct types of hair cell. Low-CF fibers project to "tectorial" hair cells (in a region of the papilla covered by the tectorial membrane) and high-CF fibers project to "free-standing" hair cells (in a region without a tectorial membrane) Thus we can compare the synchrony of high-CF fibers with the A.C. receptor potential of free-standing hair cells studied by Holton & Weiss (1983a). Figure 3.8a shows hair cell and neural responses to different frequency tone-bursts for a hair cell and nerve fiber of similar CF (2.0 kHz and 1.6 kHz respectively). The A.C. component of the receptor potential declines gradually with frequency. At 1 kHz a robust A.C. response is seen. At 2 kHz the magnitude of the A.C. component has declined by a factor of two. The decline in the magnitude of the synchronized component for the neural response is much more precipitous. At 500 Hz a robust neural synchronized reponse component is present, but it becomes undetectable at 1 kHz and above. In figure 3.8b iso-response contours of tectorial hair cell and high-CF cochlear neuroms are compared. Comparisons between iso-D.C. and iso-average-rate-difference have been described previously (Holton & Weiss, 1983b) and indicate that these contours are similar. The iso-A.C. and iso-fundamental contours of figure 3.8b, however, are markedly different. The neural synchronized response declines rapidly the range of -139- frequencies (350 Hz <f < 1kHz) for which A.C. receptor potential becomes more sensitive. For 15 highCF units the average iso-fundamental high frequency slope was 74 - 23 dB/decade whereas for 28 freestanding hair cells the average iso-A.C. contour high frequency slope was -33.6 + 2.1 dBldecade. This indi- cates a large reduction in synchronized component between receptor potential and neural impulse train. Measurements of AC. component of receptor potential at constant D.C. receptor potential for hair cells in the free-standing region revealed that the A.C. component decreases with frequency at a rate of approximately 20 dBldecade (Holton & Weiss, 1983a). We compare these data with our measurements of syruonization at constant rate in figure 3.8c. Consistent with examination of tone burst responses and isofundamental contours, the neural response declines much more rapidly with frequency than the receptor potential A.C. component. To make a more quantitative comparison we again note that free-standing hair cell iso-D.C. contours and high-CF neural iso-rate contours are very similar (Holton & Weiss, 1983b). Let us assume that these iso-rate and iso-D.C. contours are identical. Specifical let us assume that receptorpotential D.C. is constant along a neural iso-rate contour. Thus during measremnt of syn- chronization at constant rate, the D.C. receptor potential is also constant as well. Hence, we can compare the measurements of A.C. at constant D.C. and synchronization at constant rate directly. Figure 3.8c indicates that neural synchronization declines at a rate of 42 dR/decade above 350 Hz as compared to the dedine in A.C. receptor potential of 20 dB/decade (Holton & Weiss, 1983a). Under our assumption that neural average rate difference and receptor potential are equivalent for free-standing hair cells, these results suggest the existence of a low-pass process relating the receptor potential to the neural response which causes further synchronized response attenuation at a rate of about 20 dB/decade. COMPARISON OF SYNCHRONIZATION FOR FREE-STANDING AND TECTORIAL FIBERS We have shown that the decrease of synchronization with frequency (at constant rate) is much steeper for free-standing fibers than for tectorial fibers. Since no receptor potential data exist for tectorial hair cells we propose two hypothetical explanations. First let us we assume that the transduction of receptor potential into neural impulses is similar for the two types of hair cells. In that case a possible explanation for this difference in synchronized response is a difference in receptor potential behavior in the free-standing and tectorial hair cells. Receptor potential may increase with increasing frequency in tectorial hair cells thereby -140- countering the low-pass process relating receptor potential to the neural response. Alternatively, the receptor potential to neural transduction process may be much different in free-standing and tecorial hair cells. Previous work has shown that there are many differences in structure between these two dasses of hair cell (Mulroy, 1974) and recently, morphological differences between the synapses of tectorial and free-standing hair cells have been discovered. Specifically, tectorial hair-cells are more richly endowed with afferent synaptic bodies than free-standing hair-cells (Mulroy, (personal communication)). This morphological evidence suggests the possibility that synaptic transmission at the receptor cell/neuron junction differ for these two populations. COMPARISON TO THE CAT As with any comparison, differences in preparations and recording techniques must be explicitly stated. These are: (1) The temperature of the alligator lizard was kept between 200 and 210 C. The body temperature of the cat is between 370 and 380 C. Such a difference in temperature, even were the sound transduction mechanisms identical in both animals, would probably cause differences in the response to sound of the two systems; synaptic transduction consists of a series of electrochemical events (Aidley, 1978) and most chemical reaction rates depend upon temperature. (2) In previous studies (Rose, 1967; Littlefield, 1973; Anderson, 1973; Johnson, 1974; Kim & Molnar, 1979) a continuous tone stimulus used. In this study we have used the tone-burst. The effects of this difference are considered minimal, however (please see appendix D). (3) We have measured the frequency dependence of synchronization at much lower levels than either Johnson (1980) or Anderson (1973) and the synchronized response at high and low levels may differ due to neural adaptation or fatigue. Johnson (1980) reported a decrease in the maximum synchronization index for frequencies above 1kHz in the cat. As in our study of synchronization in the alligator lizard, this phenomenon appeared independent of the fiber's iso-rate CF. In figure 3.9 we show synchronization data for free-standing and tedorial nerve fibers in the alligator lizard along with Johnson's data (1980) for nerve fibers in the cat replotted on a log-log -141- scale. The magnitude and frequency dependence for the tectorial fiber synchronization and the cat cochlear neurons do not differ appreciably for frequencies below 300 Hz but the tectorial fiber synchronization begins to decrease above 350 Hz. he magnitude of the free-standing fiber synroniztd response is at all times less than that of the cat fibers and also begins to decrease near 350 Hz. These two set of data, however, bear a stiking resemblance; both exhibit a rapid decline of synchronized component. The slopes of these declines are virtually identical: -41.8 2.1 dBldecade in the alligator lizard (above 350 Hz) and 43.6 2.8 dBdecade in the cat (above 2.5 kHz). To attempt more than a simple phenomenological comparison we would need to know the behavior of receptor potential in the cat. Unfortunately, receptor potential data do not exist for this animal. However, there are some hair cell data pertinent to synchronization loss in the guinea pig. Russell and Sellick (1978) found that A.C. component of receptor potential in guinea pig inner hair cells declines relative the D.C. component at a rate of approximately 20 d(/decade. This is similar to the findings for receptor potential in the lizard. Based on this similarity, let us then assume that A.C. receptor potential behavior in mammals is similar to A.C. receptor behavior in the alligator lizard. This would imply the maximum A.C. receptor potential decreases at a rate of 20 dB/decade in mammnal as well. Let us now assume that in Johnson's measurements of maximum synchronization index as a function of frequency that the A.C. receptor potential was saturated as well. In conjunction with Johnson's data which indicate that neural synchronization decreases at approximately 40 dBrdecade, our assumptions imply that the receptor potential/neural transduction process in the cat produces an additional 20 dB/decade attenuation of synchronized component. This is the same result as obtained for alligator lizard free-standing nerve fibers. Thus the primary difference between the synchronized response of alligator lizard free-standing neurons and cochlear nerve fibers in the cat may be the frequency at which synchronization begins to decline. It will be of interest to study the dependence of synchronization attenuation in the alligator lizard as a function of tamatn ture. Specifically, the investigation of whether syn Monization decrease with increasing temperature would be the next logical course of study. -142- begins at higher frequencies AI I T I r rl I m- (1-P8)TTB PTTB Figure 3.1: The tone burst stimulus (see text for details). stimulus 02 intervals spikes 1 llllllllllllll11111 1111Iif]II ll 13 I I 20 I I 3 12 I IL Figure 3.2: Tone hburststimulus divided into M equal time intervals. We divide the toneburst onwindow into M time intervals of duration T/4 where T is the period of the tone. Spikes occuring during time interval i are snmed to obtain N i . In this schematic, N 0 =, N 1=], N 2=2, and N 3 =1. -143- U) E a, E -. _ o 0 o 0 o 0 00 .i1i N I / I O o j 8D (oas/sa>!ds) aGDJ .9i in 4 . iiu E N U.e E 0 I e5 B °~ S o0 (o o0 o 0 0 B jq~iI X e1 0 C (oas/sa8!ds) ii%-I 91DJ -144- I v1 '4 irt I I I -J 0Ln cn -I 80 - I I r I ' II I 0 a: 40 - w a w :D 20 ISO-FUNDRMENTAL a: ISO-RATE LI 344.341 1 0 _ I I I I TI I I I I I I 10000 FREQUENCY - IIJ M 1 1000 100 CHZ] I ! Cf I I \I, m 0 60 Li a 40 - - - - - ISO-FUNDAMENTAL (n w a 20 U) a: ISO-RATE - 0~ 4n. 4m 0 !- 100 _-I I I .I I) I I FREQUENCY I I i l li 10000 1000 CHZI Figure 3.4a: Iso-rate and Iso-synchrony contours for two high-CF fibers. The response criterion was approximately AI1 = 30 spikes/sec. -145- 100 II -J 0cn 0m :I II 60 - 40 w a: Ln RL 20 w -__ O. I SO-RRTE ) 0 I ! ___ I I' II Ij '~~4 lr2tlf I I vilaK 1000 100 (HZ) FREQUENCY 100 80 - 0 m ' I- a: 40 ar LU IENTRL 20 a: 0n J3 ,. 0II 14M2 ... 100 I T 1 I I 10000 1000 FREQUENCY 1 (HZ) Figure 3.4b: Iso-rate and Iso-synchrony contours for two low-CF fibers. The response criterion was approximately A1i= 30 spikes/sec. -146- N I 0.6 0.5 X x X XX XX xx 0.4 LL I x X X 0.3 C) X x X (a) x 0.2 I 0 *-_ 0.1 - I 1.0 w w r I 2.0 3.0 iso-rate wl - 4.0 5.0 6.0 CF (KHz) I 0.6 - I N x 0.5 x 0.4 X 0.3 XX C) x (b) XX 0.2 x ni - Ir 0.1 0.2 iso-rate 0.3 0.4 0.50.6 CF (KHz) Figure 3.5: Scatter pd of iso-fundamental contur CF versus iso-rate cmntour CF for low and highCF units in 12 alligatr lizrds; a) 27 high-CF units; b) 19 low-CF units. -147- I I 100 .!. i 80O c, '0 60), 40 20 _ ________ 100 _ _ _ _ 1000 I 10000 Frequency (Hz) x 4 ^ . ! . . . ..... I I I.U 4- 0.1 N C- A L high CF unit - X - >n cn v.v 1nn I I 100 low CF unit . . .. 1000 10000 frequency (Hz) Figure 3.6.: Synchronization at comtant rate (A =20 sp/sec) for low- and high-CF units; Upper paneld: bw- and high-CF iso-rate cmontours for two fibers in a single alligator lizard. Synchronization was measured at marked (frequency,level) points along each contour, Lower panel: Syndronization masuremnts at constant rate for the two fibers (Xs, low-CF; triangles, hiSh-Q); -148- I I- r^, - . 500 la A ,A A 400 A f3dB a aA'k Aa A 300 fln _ _ A - 1 000 iso-rate Cnn (b) 41~ A I 3000 2000 CF (Hz) . . I x x xx xX , X x x 400 f3dB (C) Ann I,U 400 200 iso-rate 600 CF (Hz) Figure 3.6b&c: Synironization at constant rate (AX=20 sp/sec) for low- and high-C units; b) scatter plot of f 3 (see text) versus iso-rate CF for high-CF units (21 units in 6 alligator lizards). c) scatter plot of f 3 (see text) versus iso-rate CF for low-CF units (16 units in 4 alligator lizard). -149- v n I.U IL -'1 Q) I [ .... -n I I C: C 00 N (d) 0.1 A c- o' O A tiLA A cn A la 0.01 A A 100 W w I w 1000 10000 frequency (Hz) x () 1 I.U n . ' * Z C: | * * - . x x 0 0N * (e) 0.1 LO c) 0.01 1 100 · ·- 1-·-- _ 1000 10000 frequency (Hz) Figure 3.6d&e: Synchronization at constant rate (AX 20 sp/sec) for low- and high-CF units; d) Pooled data for synchronization versus frequency at constant rate for high-CF units. The straight line is a linear regression for points above 350 Hz; slope= -41.8 -2.1 dB/decade, (39 units in 7 alligator lizards); e) Pooled data for synchronization versus frequency at constant rate in low-CF units. The straight line is a linear regression for points above 250 Hz; slope= -13.0 + 1.2 dB/decade (19 units in 4 alligator lizards); -150- x 1.0 - 0o 0.5 l I I I · · I '1 · Q) o 0 N 0 A- c-' >,, X-A=20 0.1 spikes/sec _ _ __ _~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ 1000 200 frequency (Hz) x -o n 4 I.U I . I 0.5 0 C N *0 Co l nu ri I I 200 1000 frequency (Hz) Flgure 3.7.: Synchronization versus freqeincy for different constant AA criterion: high-F units; Comparison of synchronization at constant AX for two units. AX=20 spikes/sec (Xs), AX.40 spikes/sec (trianIs). II-/ ~ '~ I.U l ~ f"Nk . . . ^ I I ^ . I I () F U N C c cc- nf w ,( 0. x r), I U. I I 100 . .. 1000 I 3000 frequency (Hz) Figure 3.7b: Synchronization versus frequency for different constant AX criterion: high-CF units; Syndchronization at AX=40 spikes/sec linear regression line for data above 350 Hz; slope= -60.4 + 4.4 dB/decade, (9 units in 4 alligator lizards). 1 re jfL CU o CJ c'J I -O ;u 0 9c I dL LLi .L In 0_ I I It. i o 1 X1X'E q 8i -; CJ In Oc I 0 0 I1 rI 0 0 0to LJ > O >rJI 0a~) 0 co a~" a I I II I I t 11111 I 11111t 100- w 00 S 80- 0 0 S \\ 0% 60cr n U3 U) . 0 - - T 40- w 0W 20- 0.1 mV *,VO= _ I rI I VI =0.1 mV --- 22-5-1.03 0- _ _ ______ _I__ 10.0 1.0 0.1 I I 11 I I I 11111 I (kHz) FREQUENCY 100 - 0 0 80 o 0 00 0 a 60 - Io a 0 %0 .- 0o tc rr U) 40- 0 soee 031 Lo 0 a I SO-RRTE 0 0 o o I SO-FUNDRMENTRL j 4 1-23 * 100 I .. .. . . I,,. I I I 0I 0 0 1000 I II . i .i . I 101300 FREQUENCY (HZ] Figure 3.8b: Comparison, for the free-standing region, of hair cell receptor potential and neural instantaneous rate. Upper panel: iso-D.C. (filled cirdes) and iso-A.C. (solid line) contours for hair cell receptor potential. Lower panel: iso-rate (open cirdes) and iso-fundamental (solid line) contours for single cocdlear neuron. -154- I . p I a I, fih I I ,,, CU w m 1 0- F C 0 a P U C S S 0 V3w 0-1- E 0 z 0.01- L 01 I I·1111 i[Iw I I p l[ ,I UTTTUTT 1 . ,,,1 1.0 100 Frequency (kHz) x 1.n 'I.- L ..... - * E E . _., e ! J Q) _C N 0.1 A 0 A L_ I O a >, o3 A 0.01 a - 100 . . 1000 10000 frequency (Hz) Figure 3.8c: Comparison, for the free-standing region, of hair cell receptor potential and neural instantaneous rate. Upper panel: Frequency dependence of A.C. receptor potential at cnstant D.C. (iso-Vo frequency response) compared to frequency dependence of synchronization at constant rate (lower panel). Neural data: 39 units in 7 alligator lizards, Receptor potential data reproduced from Hdton & Weiss (1983a): five hair cells. -155- X -1 r % I.U -- 1. A , , I 1I I 11 1 CAT -_0 / C o 0 N o C 0 U 0.1 o LIZARD high-cf ,') *C 0.05 . A d w~~~ 100 - w ww a o a .. .. 1000 10000 frequency (Hz) Figure 3.9: Comparison of the dependence of neural synaonizati upon frequency for cochlar nerve fibers in the alligator lizard and the cat. Triangles: high-CF units in the alligator lizard; Xs: lowCF units in the alligator lizard; boxes: cochlear neurns in tlhe cat. Tbe cat data are taken from Johnson (1980) and replotted on a log-log scale. -156- APPENDIX A: Existence of E (1) Equation 2.20 facilitates the existence proof for E (I). Suppose g (l) is of the following dass. g () (A.1) g ( -1) forall I there exists L 1 , L 2 s.t. g (L2-2) >2 g(L 1 ) -<, Since g (l) is an increasing function we may rewrite 2.20 as (1-g ) ) ,(l) _(p(l-1)+p(l-2)) For l cL 1 (A.2) where g (L 1)-<2, P(1)>-p(-1) + p(l-2). (A.3) p() (A4) Since p (l)>O for all l, p( -1) I sL Therefore from equation A. 3, p(l) > 2p(-2) which implies using the geometric series a a with a =112, -a = I sLI p(l-2i) p(l) (A.5) l<L1 i=l (A.6) We now determine a lower bound for E (). E(l) L' I L1 I lp(l)_ I=-x lp(l) (A.7) for LI L' and 1 (A.8) L'-c0 The right side of equation A. 7 must be less than zero since all the p (l) are positive and all I are negative. - Using eqn. A. 5 we have t 'I L lp(l)- (2)p(L =O2 2(L' 1-2)p(L' )(L1-22)+ 1) 2 + 2(L' 1 -3)p(L'1 p(L'-1)(L'-2 - 1) (A.9) 1) Therefore, by eqns. A. 7 and A. 8, LI Y lp(l) - 2(L '1-2)p(L '1) + 2(L'1-3)p(L '1-1 ) l.-S -157- (A.10) Thus E(I) has a lower bound. To find an upper bound for E() we use equation 2.20 and the mono- tone property of g () again to obtain p(l) 1 (l-2) (-1)+p(I- 2 )) (A.11) K O (A.12) First consider the sequence x(I+2) = K(x ( +l)+x ()) If we let x(l)=a and x(1 +1)=b, then the ratio (x(l +2)+x ( +3))/(x(l)+x(l (x(1+2)+x(1+3))_ =K 2+K 2 (x(l)+x(l+l)) = Since K=(1-g(1-2))/g(l-2) a+bj +1)) is (A.13) <K2+2K is a monotonically decreasing function of , so is K2 + 2K. Therefore, if K =(l-g (L2-2))/g (L2 -2) then for l -L 2 , p (+2) + p(l+3)) (A.14) Lp(I)+ (+l)p(l+1) < (I+)(p(l) + p(1+l)) (A.15) p(l) +p(l+l) > 2 And since, i I=L2 If we consain K 2 +2K<l tp(l) (L2+21+l)(p(L2)+p(L 2+1))(K2+2K) t (A.16) I=0 then g (L2 -2)> 2 . Using the geometric series once more we obtain, i Lp() <-(L2+1)(P(L 2K) 2)+P(L 2+1)) _(K2+ (A.1 +(p(L2)+P(L2+1)) ((1K2_2K9 Since both tails of the summation for E (l) converge, the summaion E (1) must itself converge. Therefore, for g (l) as described by eqns A. 1(1), E (1) exists and is finite. We now derive a result which will be important for the comparison of different g(1). Equation A.6 implies that, Lt-2 p(L 1 -1) + p(L1) >p(t) (1) In addition, E(I) must mst for ay finite sequence gt(1), but the proof -158- 1 g(L) s amitted for this ninftreing (A.18) case. Thus, the total probability in the lower tail of the distribution, p (1), for arbitrary g(l) may be no greater than the sum p(L 1) + p(L 1-1). If p(L 1 ) + p(L 1 -1) is small then the probability associated with this lower tail is negligible. Similarly, if we let we see that fo g(l) - .8165 we have K 2 + 2K (A.19) 1-9g(f) 8(l) K < 1/2. Therefore, using equation A.14 and the geometric series (see derivation of eqn. A.6) we obtain p(L 2) + p(L 2 +1) > x p(l) /=L 2-2 g(L 2) > .8165 (A.20) for arbitrary 8 (l). Thus if p (L2) + p (L 2 +1) is small, the probability in the upper tail of p (1) is negligible. -159- APPENDIX B: Estimates for Average Rate and Synchronization POISSON MODEL We assume that the spike sequence can be represented as a Poisson process with rate function, X(t). The probability that N spikes occur in an interval I is, p(N I) = ( J)N/ (B.la) N! where t+l =1 /IA(f)da (B.lb) AVERAGE RATE ESTIMATE We obtain the maximum likelihood estimate, X,,, by determining the maximum of p (n IA) in eqn. B.la with respect to X. For convenience we maximize the natural logarithm of p (n I). n(p (N Ix)) = Nln(KI) - - In(N!) (B.2) Differentiating eqn B.2 with respect to X and setting the result equal to zero yields, fAi (B.3) = N The expected value and variance of this estimate are, E(.,,,~) : =A (B.4a) and, var(k.{)= IA (B.4b) respectively. The Cramer-Rao lower bound is (Van Trees, 1968), var () > - [E- np (N X) (B.5) Using eqn. B. 2 to evaluate this expression yields, vw(a) A (B.6) Thus the estimate of eqn. B. 3 is efficient (has the minimum variance of any unbiased estimator). An unbiased estimate of var (i,,,), which we use as an approximate error bound for i,, is -160- a.2 = N2 (B.7) SYNCHRONIZATION ESTIMATE To form the synchronization estimate of eqn. 3.4 we estimate A1 and l/oA and form the product, S = ia (B.8) where & is an estimate of 1/Ao. ( 1 ) We now derive expressions for A1 and &. In addition, a simple method used to estimate IA1 12is presented for use in the measurement of iso- A1 12contours. Estimation of A 1 Let X(t) be well described by eqn 3.5. We observe the average rate in the m6' time interval, X(m) where (m + 1) X(m) = 1f We wish to estimate X(t)dt m=0,1,...,M-1 (MT>>T) (B.9) from X(m). This may be done as follows. Combining eqns. 3.5 and B. 9 Ak yields, (m +1) ( g 2 m)= 1 T mt Ake dt = Rm .2__ Ake T e kj Tsinc r kT (B.10) T k =-K.. k=-KIow We then find the Fourier coefficients of X(m), _ Al = 1-1 M A _jlmt X(m)e -Kmax < (B.1) -< Kmax Substituting eqn. B. 10 into eqn. B. 11 yields, K . Al,: (1) Later we will show tat dent. ht I A= -K. Ak 'T e . -Kin,smc 'rr = T0 1M-I j 2 -Ia (k -)m]' a when the number ci observaticns is lage then the quantities il and -161- (B. 12) " Ao0 amemtistilly indepen. We notice that for Km=T/T< 2) and k l, the term in parentheses approaches zero for large M and is identically zero for M T an integer multiple of T. For k = l, this term is unity. Therefore, kr Ak AT sincerT k c K,,, and since sinc7r - O0 for -Km T r< 2Km (B. 13) e we have using eqn. B. 11, (m)e e Ak = (B.14) rT T We now form the estimate Ak by replacing X(m) with the efficient estimate, est((m)) = (B.15) T where n, is the number of spikes which occur in the interval (mT, (m +1)T). Letting MT=To, the duration of the tone burst on-window we form the efficient( 3 ) estimate, AAi= c M1 :I, T 03.16a) J =O T,.sincrr E(Ak)=Ak since E(nm)=h(m)T. nBe For a Poisson process, since the nm are obtained from nonover- lapping intervals they are independent. The variance of Ak, E[(Ak-Ak)(Ak-Ak)'] where * denotes complex conjugation is, 1 -1l_ (m -0 var (Akk) = )0 (B.16b) = C2 T T, silnt T sinc2ukT We now modify equations B.16a&b for the case where Ajt is estimated from the response to Q tone bursts. A simple average of the At obtained for each of the Q tone bursts and letting (2) Th is exactlythe Nyqui citerion for sampled pnals at ample rate 1/r and maximum frequencyKU./T. (3) Since the estimate of eqn. B. 15 is efficient, thi estimate is fficient as well due to the fact that the tranmatica K(m) to A is linear (Van rees, 19fi8). -162- from Q Nm q-1 ( ) (B.17a) and (B.17b) I = QTf produces as the estimate of At for Q tone bursts, Ak = N · Isinc7 (B. 18a) T T Jm= and M-L (=18b) var(.k) = Ismnc2W Isinc 2 ,7r T - T Estimation of Ao Let a 1 The distribution of N, the number of spikes occuring in the observation inter- Ao val, I is N! The maximum likelihood estimate of a, found by maximizing eqn. B.19 with respect to a is, (B.20) 1 =v N The expected value of this estimate does not exist due to the singularity at N = 0. Hence, we use the asymptotically unbiased estimate of, (B.21) &L = N+ N+1 whose expected value is, I E(-) N+1 )= (-e--) -163- d21 I AO (22a) . The variance of this estimate is, For large values of I, A0 > >1 thus E () (A0 ?A 1 )2(e-A12 2=E(a2)_( (.22b) (AOl)" 12e-A0 ( (A N2O(N+1)!(N+1) = (A /) )eAo k=1 kk! ( 1 (1-e-d A = (-)e -A[Ei (AO)-n (ol Ao )2(l-eA,)2 )2 )]-( )2(1-e-') 2 Ao where y= .5772156... (Euler's constant) and Ei (x) is the exponential integral function( 4 ) The consistency of this estimate is shown as follows. For large x, Ei(x)=eX/x (Abramowitz & Stegun, table 5.2 (1972)). Thus for large values of Aod substitution of the limiting value ofxD Ei(AOd) into eqn B.22.b yields, 2 ( ed(_l)-(A )2 0 (B.23) Synchrony Estimate Synthesis Before combining the estimates A1 and & as in eqn. B.8 we must first show they are independent. From eqn. B.17a and B.18a we see that the Ak, are the weighted sums of the independent random variables n,. As MQ tends toward infinity the Ak become Gaussian random variables according to the Central Limit Theorem (Davenport & Root (1958), Drake (1967), Papoulis (1965)). Thus the A,kare marginally Gaussian for large MQ. To show they are jointly Gaussian we note that if z =ax +by is Gaussian for all nonzero a and b then x and y are jointly Gaussian (Papoulis pp. 224 (1965)). This is true for any two Ak, since they are themselves weighted sums of independent random variables. This makes z a weighted sum of independent random variable as well and therefore as the number of terms in the sum gets large z will also be Gaussian. Thus the Ak are jointly Gaussian. Fminally,to prove (4) See Abramowitz and Stegun (1972) for identities (eqns. 5.1.2 and 5.1.10) and tabulatiom. Also West (1984) and Gradshteyn & Ryzhik (1980). -164- independence, we show that the Ak, are uncorrelated. a vector of independent random variables, on the set of orthogonal vectors [No,N 1 · ·- ,N(m-1)], [1,e -J2lrj- ,e are the projections of The A -J2i-r(M - 1) ] and are therefore uncorrelated by definition (Van Trees (1968)). Since the Ak are jointly Gaussian and uncorrelated they are also independent (Davenport & Root (1958), Papoulis T[Ao]=&=l/(o+ (1965)). l/I)=I/(N+l) Thus any transformation T[-] of Ao (i.e., is independent of A1 (Papouis (1965)). We may now form the synchrony estimate of eqn. B.8 using eqns. B.18a & B.21, | -J2=- ||lM-1 eicT 1 Ne (B.2a) T rith variance, cr = var(A 1 ) (B.24b) ia SYNCHRONIZATION MEASURES FOR IRC ESTIMATION An iso-synchronization contour can be defined as the set of (tone frequency, intensity) points for which IS I is constant. We may define an iso- IA1 contour similarly. To measure such an isosynchronization contour a suitable estimate of synchronization is needed. The estimates of eqns. B. 18a & B. 24, however, are not immediately useful since they are estimates of a complex quantity rather than its magnitude. Furthermore, the derivation of unbiased estimates of IA1 I or IS I for small observation intervals I is difficult. 11 2, a We now show that if instead of choosing A 1 1 as the response variable we choose 1A simple unbiased (though var(x) = E(xx')-E(x)E(x) inefficient) ( 5 ) and that E(,) IAk12 estimate is obtained. Recall that = Ak. If we letx=A* we obtain, = E(IAk 12 ) - (B.25) ar(Ak) Using eqn. B. 18b we see that, exist using N, (a definedinn eqution B. 15) as the basic efficient stastic since I A I and (5) No efiient eimat (Van Trees, 1968). cmmute over nlinear tran(cmatis nlinear functicns of ?)(m) and efiiency does mnot -165- ISwe E (B.26) = ,ar(,(k) Therefore, EI&k12 = IAkl2 N Ix (B.27) Thus, letting Pk = Ak 12and substituting the expression for IAk I from eqn B.18a we can form the estimate I Nco 2 N.cos Isinc T 2Mrm 2 T+ N-1sin21rmT Nsin 2'm + =0 =0 - - N, (B.28) m 0 The form of this estimate for k=1 is simple when X(t) is well described by eqn 3.5 with Kmax 5 2. Setting T=-, b 4 eqn. B.28 becomes, + (N-N P 1 = 2I((2)2[(No-N2)2 21 M/4 whereNi = N, +iandN = i=0 3 3) 2) - N] (B.29) M-1 Ni = Y N,. i=O m =O Due to its computational simplicity, eqn. B.29 is useful in the rapid calculation of a A112 estimate immediately after the presentation of a single toneburst. This estimate can then be used with an iterative iso-response algorithm in much the same way as an average rate estimate is used (see tuning curve algorithm description; Chapter 2). -166- APPENDIX C: Tests of Iso-fundamental Contour Estimation Algorithm Accuracy Using Single Cochlear Neurons in the Alligator Lizard Here we show the accuracy of the new iso-fundamental contour estimation algorithm we have developed. In six cochlear nerve fibers (in the alligator lizard) we measured iso-fundamental contours. We used the TCA 3 algorithm (CHAYER estimate 1 2: IMPROVING TCA ACCURACY) with the synchronized response described in Chapter 3 (eqn. 3.8) and Appendix B (eqn. B.29). Thus, similar to the TCA when used with the average rate response, P 1 was compared to the criterion, C. The tone burst stimulus had T = 100 ms, 01= 0.5 and ,= 2.5 ms. The TCA3 was used with a criterion of C =0, -1/40 decade fre- quency increments and level increments of 1/4 dB. In order to obtain a better estimate of P 1, two tonebursts were used (see Chapter 3: METHODS). At several (frequency, level) points along each iso-fundamental contour we measured, IA1 , the magni- tude of the fundamental component of the neural firing rate function. These measurements are shown in figure C.1. Solid lines connect the measurements of All along each contour. These data illustrate that the iso-fundamental algorithm does indeed hold IA1 approximately constant For the set of measurements showing the most variation with frequency (as marked by the arrow) in figure C.1 the deviation from the mean value of IA!1 26 spikes/sec is within + 5 spikes/sec. -167- ii - I I I I 0O I 1 I-% . N I s 8 '- a) O8 at /I/ I D C I I I i 0 0o I I 0 0 (3@s/s>!ds) -168- ILVi v 'X ala . I APPENDIX D: Justification of Comparisons between Synchronization Measured with Tone Burst Stimuli and Synchronization Measured with Continuous Tone Stimuli We have consistently used a single stimulus paradigm throughout this study. Thus we can directly comrnpare measurements of iso-fundamental contours and synchronization at constant average rate difference. The choice of the tone burst as the stimulus was primarily motivated by its utility in the rapid estimation of isofundamental contours. However, in previous studies of neural synchronization (Rose, 1967; Littlefield, 1973; Anderson, 1973; Johnson, 1974; Kim & Molnar, 1979) and hair cell receptor potential A.C. component (Russell & Sellick, 1978), continuous tone stimuli were used. Thus, in order to justify a comparison of our results with these previous studies we need to determine whether tone-burst stimuli produce roughly the same response as do continuous tone stimuli. Let us first examine the neural tone burst response (figure 3.3). Both histograms of spike activity exhibit two common features: a rapid transient response and a steady state response. At stimulus onset, the neural response shows a large transient which decays to a steady state within 10 ms of stimulus onset. The tone bursts of figure 3.3 have on-windows 12.5 ms in duration. Our standard stimulus has an on-window duration of 50 ms. The response to such a stimulus is shown in figure D.1. The transient portion of the tone burst response will form only 20% of the total on-window response to a 50 ms tone burst. In addition, we notice that the detailed shape of the transient response is such that the average rate over the first 10 ms of the response does not seem (qualitatively) to be too different from the steady state average rate. We would therefore expect that the transient response will not cause the value of response averaged over the full duration of the tone burst to differ substantially from the tone burst steady state response. To test this notion, we measured response variables Ao , IA11 and IS I over both the steady state portion of the tone burst response (last 80%) and over the full tone burst response for our standard tone burst with a 50 ms on-window. Variables measured for the steady state portion of the response will be superscripted with ss. AVERAGE RATE MEASUREMENTS In figure D.2 we plot A0 vs. A. The data span a tone burst average rate difference response range of 0 spikes/sec to 136 spikes/sec and come from fibers with a range in CF of 170 Hz to 3980 Hz (111 units in 9 animals). It can be readily seen that the steady state and averaged tone burst responses are very similar -169- (correlation: .99, regression slope: .85 -. 008). SYNCHRONIZATION MEASUREMENTS In figures D.3ab&c we plot Ao versus A', IA11 versus 1 1 1" and IS I versus IS S, respectively (10 units in one alligator lizard). The average rate difference, AX, for all these measurements was between 20 and 30 spikes/sec. The relations between the steady state responses and average responses are (correlation: .96, regression slope: .80 .03) for A and AS, (correlation: .99, regression slope: .88 IA1 155,and (correlation: .99, regression slope: .98 +.008) for IS I and .02) for IA1 I and Is! . In light of these results which relate the average rate and synchronized components in the steady state portion of the toneburst response to the average tone burst response we conclude that measurement of the average tone burst response is comparable to measurement of the tone-burst steady state response. In the case of synchronizaton these two response measures are virtually identical. If we assume that the steady state response to a 50 ms tone-burst is proportional to the steady state response produced by a long tone-burst (Westerman, 1985) then it is likely that measuing synchronization over a tone-burst is comparable to measuring steady state synchronization in response to a continuous tone. -170- C) Co w 0 (I) 100 0 TIME Figure D.: CMS) Neural instantanous rate respome to a tme-burst (T,=Tff = 50 ms). Rate in 10 ms af tone-burst spikes/scc. Notice that the transient portion of the response has disappeard by Znpoe. toneabumt the of 80% mprises tbIrcore cuset. The Beady state portion of the response -171- I 11111,,,,,,,,,,, liii, lilt l IX -N I C I r I r - Lr) I U)o E-0 (1 Ii~ - t 0 rr r -t II < iv4i -1 00 N I II 111111 II I I I II I 5 0iL t 00 ° -172- 0L 0 "] Ei L mA= I L I 1 · Du. N=70 4* 40. a Ao 20 JP6P 0 0 40 20 40 60 N=70 30 AI, b 20 10 1, AAll 0 1.0 N=70 0.8 0.6 ISi 0.4 0 C 0.2 0.0 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 ISI Figure D.3: Neural rate response averaged over tone-burst on-window pltted against the response averaged over the final 80% of the tone-burst on-window (see text for details). a) Average rate, A0 (spikcessec); b) Fundamental rate aomponent, a, (spikes/sec); c) Syndronization index, IS I (nmit- ks). -173- REFERENCES 1) Abramowitz, M & Stegun, I.A. National Bureau of tandards, Handbook of Mathematical Functions with Formulas, Graphs, and Mathematical Tables. 10 edition (1972) John Wiley & Sons. 2) Aidley, D.J. The Physiology of Excitable Cels. 2n d edition (1978) Cambridge University Press. 3) Allen, J.B. Magnitude and Phase-Frequency Response to Single Tones in the Auditory Nerve. Acoust. Soc. Am 73 (6), (1983), 2071-2092. 4) Anderson, D.J. Quantitative Model for the Effects of Stimulus Frequency upon Synchronization of Auditory Nerve Discharges. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 54 (2), (1973) 361-364. 5) Chlen, C.T.Introduction to Linear System Theory Holt Rinehart and Winston (1970) 6) Davenport, W.B.,Jr. & Root, W. Random Signals And Noise. McGraw-Hill (1958). 7) Drake, A.W. Fundamentals of Applied Probability Theory. McGraw-Hill (1967). 8) Frezza, W.A. Spontaneous Activity in the Auditory Nerves of the Alligator Thesis, Dept. Elec. Eng. & Comp Sci. (1976) Cambridge, Mass. 8) Geisler, C.D. et al Thresholds for Primary Auditory Fibers Using Statistically Defined Criteria. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 77 (3), (1985) 1102-1109. 9) Gradshteyn, I.S. & Rhyzhik, I.M. Table of Integrals, Series, and Products. Academic Press (1980). 10) Gray, P.R. A Statistical Analysis of Electrophysiological Data from Auditory Nerve Fibers in Cat. M.I.T. R.L.E. Technical Report #451, (1966). 11) Holton, T. Relation between Frequency Selectivity and Two-Tone Rate Suppression in Lizard Cochlear-Nerve Fiber. Hearing Res. 2:21-38 (1980). 12) Holton, T. & Hudspeth, AJ. A Micromedanical Contribution to Cochlear Tuning and Tonotopic Organization. Science (1983) 222:508,510. 13) Holton, T. & Weiss, T.F. Receptor potentials of Lizard Cochlea Hair Cells with Free Standing Stereodciliain Response to Tones. J. Physiol. (1983a) 345:205-240. 14) Holton, T. & Weiss, T.F. Frequency Selectivity of Hair Cell and Nerve Fibers in the Alligator Lizard Cochlea. J. Phys (1983b) 345:241-260. 15) Johnson, D.H. The Response of Single Auditory-Nerve Fibers in the Cat to Single Tones: Synchrony and Average Discharge Rate. M.I.T. PhD. thesis Dept. Elec. Eng. & Comp. S (1974) Camb., Ma. 16) Johnson, D.H. The Relationship of Post-Stimulus Tune and Interval Histograms to the Timing Characteristics of Spike Trains. Biophys. J. 22, (1978) 413-430. 17) Johnson, D.H. The Relationship between Spike Rate and Synchrony in Responses of Auditory-Nerve Fibers to Single Tones. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 68, 1115-1112 (1980). -174- J. izard M.I.T. S.B. 18) Kiang, N.Y.S. Discharge Patherns of Single Fibers in the Cat's Auditory Nerve. Monograph #35, M.I.T. Press (1965). 19) Kiang, N.Y.S., Moxon, E.C. & Levine, R.A. Auditory Nerve Activity in Cats with Normal and Abnormal Cochleas. In Ciba Foundation Symposium on Sensorineural Hearing Loss, ed. Wolstenholme, G.E.W. & Knight, J., pp.241-273. London: Churchill. (1970) 20) Kim, D.O. & Molnar, C.E. A Population Study of Cochlear Nerve Fibers: Comparison of Spatial Distributions of Average-Rate and Phase-Locking Measures of Responses to Single Tones. J. Neurophysiol. M.I.T. Research 42 (1), (1979). 21) Klinke, R. & Pause, M. Discharge Properties of Primary Auditory Fibres in Caiman Crocodilus: Comparisons and Contrasts to the Mammalian Auditory Nerve. Exp. Brain Res. 38,137-150 (1980). 22) Leong, R. & Weiss, T.F. A Model for Signal Transmission in an Ear Having Hair-cells with FreeStanding Stereocilia: V. Hair-cell electric stage. (1985) (In preparation). 23) Liberman, M.C. Auditory-Nerve Response from Cats Raised in a Low-Noise Chamber. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 63,442-455 (1978). 24) Littlefield, W.O. Investigation of the Linear Range of the Peripheral Auditory System. Wash. U.Sever Inst. Tech. ScD. thesis (1973) St. Louis, Mo. 25) Liu, C.L. & Liu, Jane W.S. 26) Mulroy, M.J. Cochlear Anatomy of the Alligator Lizard. Brain Behav. & Evol. 10:69-70 (1974). 27) Papoulis, A. Probability, Random Variables, and Stochastic Processes McGraw-Hill (1965). 28) Peake, W.T. & Ling, A.L. Basilar Membrane Motion in the Alligator Lizard: Its relation to tonotopic organization and frequency selectivity. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. (1980) 67:1736-1745. 29) Pentland, A. Maximum likelihood estimation: The best PEST. Perception & Psychophysics (1980), 28 (47), 377-379. 30) Pickles, J.O. Frequency threshold carve and simultaneous masking functions in single fibres of the guinea pig auditory nerve. Hearing Research, 14 (1984) 245-256. 31) Rose, J.E. et al Phase-locked Response of Low-frequency Tones in Single Auditory Nerve Fibers of the Squirrel Monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 30, (1967) 769-793. 32) Rosowski, J.J. et al A Model for Signal Transmission in an Ear Having Hair-cells with Free-standing Stereocilia: II. Macromechanical stage. (1985) (In preparation). 33) Russell, I.J. & Sellick, P.M. Intracellular Studies of Hair Cells in the Mammalian Cochlea. J. Physiol. 284, (1978) 261-290. 34) Sdhmiedt, R.A. inear Systems Analysis McGraw-Hill (1975) Boundaries of Two-Tone Rate Suppression of Cochlear-Nrve Activity. Hearing Research, 7 (1982), 335-351. 35) Tasaki, I. Nerve Impulses in Individual Auditory Nerve Fibers of Guinea Pig. J. Neurophysiol. 17:97-122 (1954). -175- 36) Taylor, M.M. & Creelman, C.C. PEST: Efficient Estimates on Probability Functions. Aroust. Soc. Ama. 41(4) April, (1967) 782-787. 37) Watson, A.B. QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percep. & Psych (1983), 33(2), 113-120. 38) Weast, RC. 39) Weiss, T.F., Mulroy, M.J. & Altmann, D.W. Intracellular Responses to Acoustic Clicks in the Inner Ear of the Alligator izard. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 55(3) (1974) 606-619. 40) Weiss, T.F. et al Tuning of Single Fiber in the Codilear Nerve of the Alligator Lizard: Relation to Receptor Organ Morphology. Brain Res. 115:71-90 (1976). 41) Weiss, T.F. & Leong, R. A Model for Signal Transmission in an Ear Having Hair-cells with Freestanding Stereocilia: III. Micromechanical stage. (1985a) (In preparation). 42) Weiss, T.F. & Leong, R. A Model for Signal Transmission in an Ear Having Hair-cells with Freestanding Stereocilia: VI. Model properties and comparison with with measurements. (1985b) (In preparation). 43) Weiss, T.F. et al A Model for Signal Transmission in an Ear Having Hair-cells with Free-standing Stereocilia: IV. Mechanoelectric transduction stage. (1985) (In preparation). 44) Westerman, L.A. Adaptation and Recovery of Auditory Nerve Responses. Institute for Sensory Research, Syracuse University, Syracuse, N.Y. January, (1985). 45) Van Trees, H.L CRC Handbook of Chemistry and Physics. 6 5 th edition (1984) CRC Pres. Detection, Estimation and Modulation Theory: Part I Wiley (1967). -176- Journal BIOGRAPHY Cristopher Rose was born in New York City on January 9, 1957. In June of 1979 he received the Bachelor of Science degree from the Massachusetts Institute of Technology in the Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science. In June of 1981 he was awarded the degree of Master of Science in Electrical Engineering and Computer Science also at M.I.T. Dr. Rose's research interests include auditory physiology, chronic and recording/stimulation, prosthetic device control, stochastic processes and telecommunications. Awards 1979- Bell Labs Cooperative Research Fellowip 1979- NSF Fellowship (declined) 1979- GEM Fellowship (dedined) 1977- General Motors Scholarhip (declined) 1975- National Achievement 4-year Corporate Sholarip -177- acute neural