POPULATION RESPONSE OF A DECLINING SONGBIRD TO SILVICULTURE: HOW CERULEAN WARBLER (SETOPHAGA CERULEA) TERRITORY SIZE AND SETTLEMENT PATTERNS FARE IN THE FACE OF FOREST DISTURBANCE A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE MASTER OF SCIENCE BY RYAN H. DIBALA BALL STATE UNIVERSITY ADVISOR: DR. KAMAL ISLAM MUNCIE, INDIANA MAY 2012 ii ABSTRACT DISSERTATION/THESIS/RESEARCH PAPER/CREATIVE PROJECT: Population response of a declining songbird to silviculture: How Cerulean Warbler (Setophaga cerulea) territory size and settlement patterns fare in the face of forest disturbance STUDENT: Ryan H. Dibala DEGREE: Master of Science COLLEGE: Sciences and Humanities DATE: May 2012 PAGES: 110 Over the past 5 decades, populations of the Cerulean Warbler (Setophaga cerulea) have declined precipitously and the response of populations to silviculture has been identified as a high-priority research need. This species was studied in nine forest management units in Southern Indiana following a harvest that took place in 2008. Males were detected, territories were demarcated, and male age-class was determined to identify settlement patterns. Vegetation was measured in all territories and associated random non-use sites. Data analyzed in ArcMap (ArcGIS 10) show that Cerulean Warbler territory size was smallest and density was highest in even-aged units. Territories contained a greater number of small woody species than non-use sites but no vegetative differences existed between male age-classes. Instead, males appeared to select areas by relying on social cues from experienced neighbors. It is possible that “social attraction” management techniques could influence male Cerulean Warbler settlement patterns, providing a valuable tool for the conservation of this species. iii Acknowledgments This research would not have been possible without financial assistance from IDNR/Purdue University, US Fish and Wildlife Service, Amos W. Butler Audubon Society, Indiana Academy of Science, and Ball State University Internal Grants Program (ASPiRE). I am grateful for the continual help and support of Jennifer Wagner while in the field. She is one of the most inquisitive people I know and has helped me redefine myself as a field biologist and free thinker. The amount of data collected and strides we have made in better understanding the Cerulean Warbler would not have been possible without the hard work of David Rupp, Jordan Schindler, Amy Wilson, and Edsel Koscielniak. I am lucky to have had such a hard-working and dedicated group of technicians. We all learned so much from each other throughout our summer together. I would also like to thank Erin Arnold for her invaluable assistance in data entry. I am forever indebted to my advisor and mentor Dr. Kamal Islam who guided me patiently and faithfully every step of the way. He was a joy to be around and always encouraged me to explore new avenues in research and to think outside the box. His excitement and passion for nature has inspired me to continue teaching and conducting research. I’d also like to thank Carol Islam for all her support, help, and kindness throughout my two years at Ball State. I would also like to thank Dr. David LeBlanc for his guidance and help with statistical analyses. Through his teaching, he has given me a solid foundation with which to continue research. Additionally, I thank Dr. Kevin Turcotte for his help with GIS and Drs. Keanre Eouanzoui and Kemuel Badger along with Stephen Jacquemin for their help with the vegetation data analysis. Big ups to my main man David Sturgeon, who has been an amazing source of motivation and positive affirmation. He created a space for me in his house and has left an imprint on my heart. I thank him for his friendship. Last but certainly not least I thank my parents Richard and Karen and my sister Christie. I would not have had this incredible opportunity to explore the natural world and try to make some sense of its iv mystery if it weren’t for my amazing family. They exposed me to inquiry at a young age and have continually supported me in all of my endeavors. I love you and wouldn’t have been able to do this without you! v Table of Contents Abstract………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… ii Acknowledgments……………………………………………………………………………………………………….. iii Table of Contents…………………………………………………………………………………………………………. v Thesis Summary…………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 1 Chapter 1 Forest Management Effects on Cerulean Warbler Territory Size in Southern Indiana………………………………………………………………………………… 7 Chapter 2 Conspecific Social Cues Strongly Influence Cerulean Warbler Male Settlement Patterns in a Managed Forest................................................ 42 Appendix I………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 83 Appendix II…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………. 98 Appendix III………………………………………………………………………………………………………………. 103 1 Thesis Summary The Cerulean Warbler (Setophaga cerulea) is a Nearctic-Neotropical migrant songbird that breeds in mature, contiguous deciduous forest in eastern and central North America and winters on the slopes of the Andes Mountains of northwestern South America (Hamel 2000). Over the past five decades, numbers of Cerulean Warblers are estimated to have dropped approximately 70% and now this species is experiencing the greatest decline of any North American wood warbler (Sauer et al. 2008). The Cerulean Warbler has quickly become the poster child for many migratory birds that have been affected by the loss of contiguous forest cover in both their breeding and wintering grounds, and various conservation and habitat management plans have been erected in hopes of restoring populations throughout its range. Cerulean Warbler response to land management activities such as silviculture has been identified as a high-priority research need (Hamel 2000, Hamel et al. 2004). Some authors have suggested that Cerulean Warblers may be attracted to canopy gaps or openings and these gaps may be an important aspect of nest-site selection (Bent 1953, Oliarnyk 1996, Oliarnyk and Robertson 1996). Conversely, there has been concern that too many openings within a forest’s interior could maximize edge-effects and internally fragment what once was continuous forest (Annand and Thompson 1997). Thus, it is critical to evaluate Cerulean Warbler population response to various levels of forest disturbance. 2 During the summers of 2010 and 2011, I studied a population of Cerulean Warblers at Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana to assess the effects of even-aged and uneven-aged silviculture on male territory size. Additionally, in 2011, I investigated age-specific male settlement patterns to examine behavioral and social cues that influence territory establishment in a managed forest. Ultimately, understanding an animal’s behavior may be just as important for conservation as understanding its environment (Clemmons and Buchholz 1997). In order to make informed management decisions that ultimately ensure Cerulean Warbler conservation, I analyzed both vegetative components and social mechanisms that may be important to this species. Chapter 1 presents results of male territory sizes over five years in nine management units at Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. Uneven and even-aged harvests were implemented in the fall of 2008 across six of nine managements units; three control sites received no treatment. Cerulean Warbler territory size was not significantly different between pre- and post-treatment years. However, there were a greater number of territories that were significantly smaller in even-aged units than they were in control units. It is difficult to determine the long-term effects of even-aged silviculture because songbirds often exhibit a lag response to disturbance (Brooks et al. 1999), but Cerulean Warblers seem to be attracted to even-aged units in these forests. 3 Chapter 2 focuses on research conducted in 2011 on age-specific male settlement patterns. I recorded male age-class along with territory size and found that yearling male territories were significantly larger than mature adult male territories. Although no statistical difference was found in clustering between age-classes, smaller territory sizes may mean that mature adult males often settle in higher-density areas than yearlings. In fact, a comparison of average nearest neighboring territories showed that mature adults had significantly closer neighbors than did yearlings. Both ageclasses settled significantly closer to other mature adult males, indicating that this species shows a clear preference for settling closer to experienced adults. There was no difference in vegetative structure and distance to important habitat features between the age-classes, providing evidence that male Cerulean Warbler settlement patterns are strongly influenced by conspecific social cues. Many species of birds have been shown to be attracted to areas where they experience poor survival or reproductive success, especially in human-altered landscapes (Gates and Gysel 1978, Wilcove 1985). In the Appalachian Mountains, Boves (2011) found that although Cerulean Warbler territory density and body condition was highest after intermediate and heavy forest disturbance, reproductive success was significantly lower. Cerulean Warblers in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests had lower reproductive success in both even and uneven-aged areas than control areas (Jennifer Wagner unpublished data). Even-aged areas could be serving as ecological 4 traps that could ultimately have a negative impact on the population (Pärt et al. 2007, Boves 2011). For species that are highly sensitive to conspecific attraction, conservationists could use broadcast vocalizations known as playbacks to attract birds to settle in known high-quality habitats (Mills et al. 2006, Alatalo et al. 1982, Muller et al. 1997). It is possible that vulnerable songbirds like the Cerulean Warbler could be coaxed into settling on protected lands where limiting factors can be controlled and breeding success may be higher. This may prove to be an important management tool to prevent further decline of this denizen of the deciduous forest. 5 Literature Cited Alatalo, R. V., A. Lundberg, and M. Bjorklund. 1982. Can the song of male birds attract other males? An experiment with the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca. Bird Behaviour 4:42-45. Annand, E. M. and F. R. Thompson III. 1997. Forest bird response to regeneration practices in central hardwood forests. Journal of Wildlife Management 61:159 171. Bent, A. C. 1953. Life Histories of North American Wood Warblers, parts I and II. Dover Publications, New York. Boves, T. J. 2011. Multiple responses by Cerulean Warblers to experimental forest disturbance in the Appalachian Mountains. PhD dissertation, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN. Brooks, T. M., S. L. Pimm, and J. O. Oyugi. 1999. Time lags between deforestation and bird extinction in tropical forest fragments. Conservation Biology 13:1140-1150. Clemmons, J. R., and R. Buchholz, editors. 1997. Behavioral approaches to conservation in the wild. Cambridge University Press, New York. Gates, J. E., and L. W. Gysel. 1978. Avian nest dispersion and fledging success in field forest ecotones. Ecology 59:871–883. Hamel, P. B. 2000. Cerulean Warbler (Dendroica cerulea). The Birds of North America, No. 511 (A. Poole and F. Gill, eds.). The Birds of North America, Inc., Philadelphia, PA. Hamel, P. B., D. K. Dawson, and P. D. Keyser. 2004. How we can learn more about the Cerulean Warbler (Dendroica cerulea). Auk 121:7-14. Mills, A. M., J. D. Rising and D. A. Jackson. 2006. Conspecific attraction during establishment of Least Flycatcher clusters. Journal of Field Ornithology 77:34-38. Muller, K. L., J. A. Stamps, V. V. Krishnan, and N. H. Willits. 1997. The effects of conspecific attraction and habitat quality on habitat selection in territorial birds (Troglodytes aedon). American Naturalist 150:650-661. Oliarnyk, C. J. 1996. Habitat selection and reproductive success in a population of Cerulean Warblers in southeastern Ontario. M.S. Thesis, Queen’s University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada. 6 Oliarnyk, C. J. and R. J. Robertson. 1996. Breeding behavior and reproductive success of Cerulean Warblers in Southeastern Ontario. Wilson Bulletin 108:673-684. Pärt, T., Arlt, D., and Villard, M-.A. 2007. Empirical evidence for ecological traps: a twostep model focusing on individual decisions. Journal of Ornithology 148:S327 S332. Sauer, J. R., J. E. Hines and J. Fallon. [online]. 2008. The North American Breeding Bird Survey,Results and Analysis 1966-2007 version 5.15,2008. USGS Patuxent Wildlife Research Center, Laurel, Maryland, USA. Wilcove, D. S. 1985. Nest predation in forest tracts and the decline of migratory songbirds. Ecology 66:1211–1214. -Chapter 1Forest Management Effects on Cerulean Warbler Territory Size in Southern Indiana Ryan H. Dibala 8 Abstract I studied the effects of silviculture treatments on territory sizes of the Indiana state-endangered Cerulean Warbler (Setophaga cerulea) in Morgan-Monroe and Yellowwood state forests in southern Indiana. Uneven and even-aged harvests were implemented in the fall of 2008 across six of nine managements units; three control sites received no treatment. I hypothesized that silviculture treatments would result in an increase in territory size to account for loss of forest cover. Cerulean Warblers were detected and territory sizes were recorded during the summers of 2010 and 2011. A general linear model showed that there were no differences in mean territory size between pre- and post-harvest years. When analyzed at the treatment level over all five years, a one-way ANOVA showed that even-aged areas had territory mean sizes that were significantly smaller than territories in control areas (F2,341 = 3.39, p = 0.035) but territory means in uneven-aged areas were not significantly different from mean territory sizes in either control or even-aged areas. Additionally, mean number of territories in even-aged areas was significantly greater than in control areas (F2, 41 = 5.82, p = 0.006). Territory density was highest in even-aged units, suggesting that Cerulean Warblers are attracted to these areas. Keywords: Cerulean Warbler, Setophaga cerulea, even-aged silviculture, territory size. 9 Introduction In the wake of human population growth and overuse of natural resources, conservationists have devoted exceedingly greater research efforts to forest conservation and management at local and global levels. Timber harvesting, in particular, has had noticeable effects on forest community assemblages and alternative silviculture practices have been shown to both benefit and detract from the needs of forest songbirds (Thompson III et al. 1993). One area-sensitive and forest-dependent songbird, the Cerulean Warbler (Setophaga cerulea), has received recent attention because it is especially sensitive to ongoing forest fragmentation and isolation (Robbins et al. 1992). The Cerulean Warbler is a Nearctic-Neotropical migrant songbird that breeds in mature deciduous forests of eastern and central North America and winters on the Andean slopes of northwestern South America (Hamel 2000). Over the past five decades, numbers of Cerulean Warblers have declined precipitously. According to the North American Breeding Bird Survey, there has been an annual population decline of 4.1% between 1966 and 2007 (Sauer et al. 2008). Populations are estimated to have dropped approximately 70% since 1966 and now are experiencing the greatest decline of any North American wood warbler (Sauer et al. 2008). This decline is so alarming that the species has been listed as a species of conservation concern by the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service (U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service 2008) and has been declared as “vulnerable”on the International Union for the Conservation of Nature’s (IUCN) red list 10 (Birdlife International 2004). The reason for this population decline has been attributed to loss of contiguous forest cover and habitat fragmentation throughout its range (Hamel 2000). These alterations often play host to more insidious threats such as increased brood parasitism and exposure to predators on breeding grounds (Bakermans and Rodewald 2009). Also, survival and return rates from their wintering grounds could be a limiting factor, but little has been published about Cerulean Warbler non-breeding ecology (Robbins et al. 1992, Jones et al. 2000). Cerulean Warblers are an area-sensitive species that depend on large, contiguous blocks of mature deciduous forest and tall trees for successful breeding (Hamel 2000). Several studies have compared vegetative components within territories and non-use sites to identify habitat characteristics that are attractive to Cerulean Warblers. It appears that a heterogeneous, three-dimensional structure of the canopy within the forest is an important landscape feature for this species (Lynch 1981, Hamel 2000, Jones et al. 2001). In Southern Indiana, Cerulean Warbler territories were associated with a tall canopy, high percentage of canopy cover, steep slope, and trees with a DBH ≥38 cm. Additionally, nest sites and territories were associated with Virginia creeper (Parthenocissus quinquefolia) and grape vine (Vitis sp.), respectively (Roth and Islam 2008). Kaminski (2010) found that Cerulean Warbler territories were approximately 153.3 m closer to small conifer patches than non-use sites. Some authors have suggested that Cerulean Warblers may be attracted to canopy gaps or openings and these gaps may be an important aspect of nest-site selection (Bent 1953, Oliarnyk 11 1996, Oliarnyk and Robertson 1996). Conversely, there has been concern that too many openings within a forest’s interior could maximize edge-effects and internally fragment what once was continuous forest (Annand and Thompson 1997). Hamel (2000) identified the response of populations to land management activities as a high-priority research need for Cerulean Warblers. Since then, studies have focused on the negative effects of abrupt habitat change associated with mountaintop removal mining in West Virginia (Wood et al. 2005a), large-scale landscape disturbance events (Jones et al. 2001), and the effects of silviculture treatments (Register and Islam 2008, Wood et al. 2005b, Bakermans and Rodewald 2009). Hamel et al. (2004) further identified studying the effects of silviculture treatments on the occurrence of this species as a pressing research need. Silviculture practices commonly used in North America can be grouped into one of two classifications: even-aged and uneven-aged management (Thompson III et al. 1993). Even-aged stands are characterized by trees that are the same age class throughout and are maintained by techniques such as clear-cutting and shelterwood. Clear-cutting involves removal of the entire stand (<1 ha to >100 ha) in one cutting, whereas shelterwood requires the gradual removal of the entire stand in a series of partial cuttings (Thompson III et al. 1993). Uneven-aged stands are single trees or groups of trees that are periodically harvested, creating a multi-age-class forest. Selection methods such as single tree removal and patch cutting create small gaps and are used to promote regeneration of shade-tolerant tree species. 12 Selectively cut stands typically retain much of the forest bird community, but their numbers are often lower (Thompson III et al. 1993). Moreover, several studies have provided evidence that uneven-aged forest management has fewer negative effects than even-aged management on area-sensitive songbirds (Oliarnyk and Robertson 1996, Jones and Robertson 2001, Weakland and Wood 2005, Roth and Islam 2008, Register and Islam 2008). It is likely that uneven-aged treatments such as single tree removal mimic canopy gaps and openings that naturally occur in mature forests (Flashpohler 1993). In an attempt to better understand the effects of forest management practices on native plants and animals of Indiana, a consortium of state agencies and Universities initiated a 100-year study known as the Hardwood Ecosystem Experiment (HEE). Launched in 2006, the project encompasses nine management units (~282-363 ha) in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana that provide an exceptional opportunity for the study of silviculture treatments and their effects on native wildlife and plant regeneration. Uneven and even-aged harvests were implemented in the fall of 2008 across six of nine managements units; three control sites received no treatment. Control units were left unharvested, while uneven-aged units were treated with single tree and patch cut harvests, and even-aged units were treated with shelterwood and clear-cut harvests (HEE online). Cerulean Warblers were studied on the HEE management units in Southern Indiana to determine if and how silviculture treatments are affecting this population. 13 Specifically, this study examined changes in territory size and number among even-aged, uneven-aged, and control units between pre- and post-harvest years. Vegetation structure was also compared among territories and randomly selected non-use areas. Forest harvesting began in late summer 2008 and lasted until early April 2009. I hypothesized that the total number of territories would decrease significantly after the harvest and that the size of territories would increase due to reduced conspecific competition. Lastly, I predicted that territories would have significantly larger tree diameters, steeper slope, and be closer to streams and roads than non-use areas. Materials and Methods Background Research was conducted in nine management units established by the HEE project in Brown, Morgan, and Monroe counties of Southern Indiana (Figure 1). Four units were located in Morgan-Monroe and five units in Yellowwood state forests. Uneven-aged units were comprised of four 0.4 ha, two 1.2 ha, and two 2 ha openings (group cuts) and several small patch cuts with a target basal area of 16.1 – 23.0 m²/ha (Figure 2). Even-aged units had four openings (two shelterwoods and two clearcuts) each a total of 4 ha (Figure 3). Shelterwood cuts removed a range of 45-53 m²/ha in canopy cover and will be re-harvested after understory regeneration is complete (unknown date). All control units received no harvest and were used to monitor Cerulean Warblers in the absence of forest management (Figure 4). Harvests occurred 14 on slopes in each of the four cardinal directions (HEE online). Habitat consisted of oakhickory deciduous forest with steep drainages, rolling hills, and ephemeral creeks. The study site lies within the largest block of contiguous forest in Southern Indiana and fits descriptions of quality Cerulean Warbler habitat. I collected data on territory size in the spring and summer of 2010 and 2011. These data were compared with two years of pre-harvest data (2007 and 2008) and one year of post-harvest data (2009) from Prichard’s (2007) study and Kaminski (2010) and MacNeil’s (2010) theses. These data provide long-term, baseline information regarding Cerulean Warbler population response to forest harvesting. Point Count Surveys One hundred-meter fixed radius point count surveys were conducted throughout the month of May to locate singing males who were establishing territories. Surveys were conducted within a 259 ha sample grid consisting of 49 bird survey points (7x7; 1.96 km2 with a 50 m forest buffer). These points were arranged in seven northsouth transect lines where there was a 200 m distance between each point to rule out the possibility of dual detection (Jones et al. 2000, Hamel et al. 2009). Surveys were conducted between 05:30 and 10:30 EST and none took place on windy or rainy days. One point count was conducted for each survey point and lasted five minutes. The first two minutes were spent listening for singing males. This was followed by one minute of Cerulean Warbler song broadcasting via an MP3 player with an external speaker to elicit 15 a vocal response from any males in the immediate area. The last two minutes were again spent listening without playback (Falls 1981). When a bird was detected, its approximate distance from the point and compass direction were recorded. Territory Delineation Points where birds were initially heard were revisited to delineate territory boundaries. Male Cerulean Warblers were located by their incessant singing, and trees in which birds sang repetitively were identified to species and marked with flagging tape. A Garmin 76 handheld Global Positioning System (GPS) unit was used to mark the location of the tree in a Universal Transverse Mercator (UTM) coordinate system. A territory was considered complete when between 5 and 12 trees were recorded for an individual. Territory locations and sizes were determined by creating minimum convex polygons in ArcMap (ArcGIS 10). Vegetation Sampling Vegetation sampling began at the beginning of July when detection proved difficult due to a substantial decrease in singing rate. Sampling was conducted at the center of each territory where a 0.04 ha circular plot was marked. Using the methods of James and Shugart (1970), aspect was assessed at the plot’s origin and slope was measured with a clinometer in each cardinal direction. Presence and absence of canopy and ground cover were recorded with a GRS densitometer at 2 m intervals along each cardinal direction of the plot. Woody plants with a diameter at breast height (DBH) ≤3 16 cm and 3-10 cm were identified to species within a 5 m radius of the plot’s center. In each quadrant, mature trees (>10 cm DBH), both living and dead, were identified to species and a rangefinder was used to measure maximum canopy height in each quadrant. GIS Analysis A geodatabase was created in ArcMap (ArcGIS 10) to display and analyze all geospatial data. UTM coordinates were imported to ArcMap and polygons were constructed in a two-step process: 1) triangular irregular networks (TINs) were created to display third dimension attributes in vector format and 2) an attribute domain was then created to ensure data integrity. Each domain represents a separate territory and includes information about the shape’s length and area. Domains were added to a larger feature class depending on their make-up. For example, separate feature classes were created for territories and non-use areas for multiple years. These layers were superimposed onto GIS layers of the nine management units with associated landscape features and harvest areas. These, along with raster data such as aerial photographs and a digital elevation model (DEM) were obtained from the Indiana Spatial Data Portal (Kaminski 2010). These files were used to analyze differences in elevation, slope, and aspect of Cerulean Warbler territories that were derived from the vector polygons using the zonal statistics tool under Zonal in Spatial Analyst Tools. 17 Territory centroids were created for polygon feature classes using the feature to point tool. Distances were measured to and from the centroids of polygon features by generating a Near Table under Proximity in Analysis Tools. Statistical Analyses A general linear model (GLM) was used to test for post-harvest changes in territory size and number among the treatments and a subsequent one-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used to test for differences between silviculture treatments. Two-sample t-tests were used to test for differences between territories and non-use areas in spatial attributes such as distances to landscape features, slope, aspect, and elevation. I investigated whether there were any differences in vegetative characteristics between territories and random non-use areas by using Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA, Sharma 1996). Following significant multivariate results, I examined each response variable separately using a one-way ANOVA in order to determine which variable(s) were statistically significant. Bonferroni adjustments were made for all multiple comparisons of variables that were not considered independent. All tests were performed with Minitab (Version 16) at α = 0.05. All data were checked for normality and non-normal data were log-transformed. In cases where the data were extremely positively skewed, the non-parametric MannWhitney U-test was used because of its higher power in these situations (LeBlanc 18 personal communication). Therefore, median values are presented occasionally throughout. Results I detected a total of 93 male Cerulean Warblers in 2010 and 130 in 2011 (Table 1). Of these total detections, 50 territories in 2010 and 101 in 2011 were demarcated. This compares to the 65, 55, and 76 territories demarcated in 2007 (Prichard Young 2007), 2008, and 2009, respectively (Kaminski 2010, MacNeil 2010). There was no difference in the mean number of territories between pre- and post-harvest years (F1, 40 = 0.11, p = 0.741, log transformed) but there was a significant difference among treatment groups (F2, 40 = 5.65, p = .007, log transformed). A subsequent ANOVA was used to test for differences in number of territories among experimental units over all five years and a post-hoc Tukey’s HSD test determined that mean number of territories in even-aged areas was significantly greater than in control areas (F2, 41 = 5.82, p = 0.006) but mean number of territories in uneven-aged areas was not significantly different from control or even-aged areas (Figure 5). Territory sizes ranged from 0.01 ha to 1.75 ha, with an overall mean of 0.28 ha (Table 2, see Appendix III). There was no difference in mean territory size between preand post-harvest years (F1, 340 = 1.87, p = 0.173, Figure 6) but a significant difference existed among treatment groups (F2, 340 = 3.56, p = .029). A subsequent ANOVA was used to test for differences in mean territory size among experimental units over all five 19 years and a post-hoc Tukey’s HSD test determined that even-aged areas had territory mean sizes that were significantly smaller than territories in control areas (F2, 341 = 3.39, p = 0.035) but territory means in uneven-aged areas were not significantly different from mean territory sizes in either control or even-aged areas (Figure 7). Results of the MANOVA indicated that use and non-use areas differed significantly with respect to two vegetative habitat characteristics (Wilks’ λ, F11,191 = 6.416, p < 0.001). Subsequent ANOVAs (Table 3) with Bonferroni adjustments showed that territories had significantly more woody species between 0 and 3 cm DBH (p = 0.028), than non-use areas. Alternatively, non-use areas had significantly more woody species between 3 and 10 cm DBH than territories (p = 0.001, Table 4). When Cerulean Warbler territories were compared to random non-use areas, I found that the median distance to streams was significantly shorter for territories than it was for non-use areas (p ˂0.001, Mann-Whitney U-test). These data also suggest that there is a significant difference in median distance to roads between use sites and nonuse sites (p = 0.074, Mann-Whitney U-test). Territories were significantly closer to conifer patches than non-use sites (p˂0.001, Mann-Whitney U-test) and there was no difference found in median distance to harvest areas (p = 0.277, Mann-Whitney U-test, Table 4). Surprisingly, there was no difference in median slope (p = 0.522; MannWhitney U-test) although there was a significant difference in median aspect between territories and non-use sites (p˂0.001, Mann-Whitney U-test), showing that Cerulean Warblers preferred eastern facing slopes (109.47° E/SE). Finally, the data suggest that 20 there is a difference in mean elevation between use and non-use sites (t = -1.79, p = 0.075, 2-sample t-test). Territories appear to be located at lower elevations than nonuse sites. All significant differences remained significant after Bonferroni corrections were made. Discussion Territory sizes were smaller than those presented by Oliarnyk (1996) who showed that territories in Southern Ontario ranged in size from 0.38 ha to 2.4 ha. However, Robbins et al. (1992) noted that territories smaller than 0.38 ha are possible. In fact, mean territory size of 51 Cerulean Warbler territories demarcated at Big Oaks National Wildlife Refuge in Southern Indiana was 0.21 ha (Islam and Roth 2004). Furthermore, previous research on Cerulean Warblers in the Hoosier National Forest, Yellowwood State Forest, and Morgan-Monroe state forest documented a range in size from 0.04 ha to 1.43 ha, with a mean of 0.34 ha (Islam and Basile 2002). Data presented here show that there was no significant change in territory size or number post-harvest, rejecting my hypothesis. My prediction that the even-aged harvest would reduce the number of birds, alleviating competition and making territories larger was not supported. Instead, when all five years were compared disregarding the harvest, even-aged units contained a greater number of territories with smaller sizes than control sites. 21 Cerulean Warblers appear to be settling in and using areas close to canopy gaps in these forests. My results show that these birds are settling closer to streams and roads, two landscape features that create small canopy openings in the forest. These findings support previous studies claiming that Cerulean Warblers are associated with canopy gaps (Wood et al. 2005b, Oliarnyk and Robertson 1996, Hamel 2000). Territory placement closer to conifer patches was confirmed, supporting the findings of Kaminski (2010). Because there is no other indication of this species using coniferous habitats in the literature, my interpretation is that there is a correlation between planted conifer patches and streams and it is the latter feature that attracts this species. This reasoning can be used in the interpretation of finding Cerulean Warbler territories at lower elevations at the bottom of ravines where streams are most prevalent. Similar to previous studies, aspect was shown to influence Cerulean Warbler settlement, with principal use of eastern facing slopes (Wood et al. 2005b, Kaminski 2010). Other research suggests that forest growth in the northern hemisphere is frequently greatest on north-to-east-facing slopes (Beers et al. 1966). Several studies have found that Cerulean Warbler males perch in trees with diameters that are significantly larger than average trees available to them in their territories (Robbins et al. 1992, Jones and Islam 2006). Larger trees and greater structural diversity on eastern aspects could provide Cerulean Warblers with ideal song perch posts. Surprisingly, I did not find any differences in slope between territories and non-use areas. This contrasts 22 from the findings of other studies (Roth and Islam 2007, Kaminski 2010, Buehler et al. 2008). Although not significant, most pronounced increases in number of territories post-harvest occurred in even-aged areas. It is possible that Cerulean Warblers are selecting habitat based on within-territory characteristics and are not negatively affected by harvests in surrounding areas of the forest. In West Virginia, Cerulean Warbler abundance in unharvested stands directly adjacent to clear-cut and two-age treatments (gradual removal of stand) was similar to that in unharvested control stands away from the cuts (Wood et al. 2005b). In Southern Ohio, Bakermans and Rodewald (2009) examined nest success in relation to adjacency of regenerating clear-cuts and found that variation in local habitat (within territory) features played more of a role in nest productivity than the nearness or size of the clearcuts. Furthermore, the regenerating systems did not contribute significantly to edge-related nest predation events. They concluded that there is no evidence that Cerulean Warblers are sensitive to intensive timber harvesting in the immediate landscape surrounding mature forest sites (Bakermans and Rodewald 2009). Cerulean Warblers may find something attractive about even-aged units and settle in them because of it. Boves (2011) found that Cerulean Warbler territory density was highest after intermediate and heavy disturbance, suggesting that this species may seek out habitat altered by large-scale disruptions like fire or landslides rather than 23 single tree-fall gaps. Additionally, he found that males occupying disturbed areas had greater body mass (better body condition) than those in control areas (Boves 2011). Even-aged areas promote the succession of small woody species, a variable that seems to be important in Cerulean Warbler habitat selection. Dense woody understory that colonizes clearcuts has been shown to provide protection from predators and food resources for mature forest birds during the post-fledging period (Marshall et al. 2003, Vitz and Rodewald 2006). I observed both male and female Cerulean Warblers foraging in smaller trees on the edges of clearcuts as well as fledglings being led by adults through thick, woody understory in even-aged plots. In one of the management units, the largest aggregation of birds was located directly within a shelterwood cut (Figure 8). Several studies have shown that breeding bird abundance and diversity increases in two-age stands (shelterwoods) and that they are attractive to many late-successional forest species (Boardman and Yahner 1999, Duguay et al. 2001). A study on songbird nest survival rates in West Virginia concluded that two-age treatments placed within extensively forested areas do not result in the type of edge effects (population sinks) observed in areas fragmented by agriculture. Two-age treatments can also provide a more aesthetic and viable conservation alternative to clearcutting where brood parasitism is not a concern (Duguay et al. 2001). McDermott and Wood (2009) found that 19-26 year-old two-age harvests were beginning to provide habitat for some species of mature forest songbirds that were absent or uncommon previously. Conclusions and Management Recommendations 24 Cerulean Warblers occur in greater numbers with smaller territory sizes in evenaged management units throughout Morgan-Monroe and Yellowwood state forests. Despite appearing benign or even beneficial, it would be dangerous to speculate that even-aged silviculture could be used in future management efforts for this species without accounting for survival or reproduction. The assessment of logging effects by measuring bird populations in the absence of measurements of effects on reproductive success can be misleading (Brawn and Robinson 1996, Van Horne 1983). Therefore, it is essential that future research continues to monitor Cerulean Warbler fecundity in and near the harvests to more effectively determine the long-term consequences of evenaged silviculture. Songbirds have a tendency to exhibit a lag response to habitat disturbance (Brooks et al. 1999) and it is possible that large clearcuts and shelterwood harvests could be associated with more insidious threats that are not immediately perceived. Many species of birds have been shown to be attracted to areas where they experience poor survival or reproductive success, especially in human-altered landscapes (Gates and Gysel 1978, Wilcove 1985). In the Appalachian Mountains, Boves (2011) found that although Cerulean Warbler territory density and body condition was highest after intermediate and heavy forest disturbance, reproductive success was significantly lower. Cerulean Warblers in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests had lower reproductive success in both even and uneven-aged areas than control areas (Wagner 25 unpublished data). Even-aged areas could be serving as ecological traps that could ultimately have a negative impact on the population (Pärt et al. 2007, Boves 2011). Even-aged silviculture may have different effects on Cerulean Warbler reproduction and survival in different landscapes. The HEE treatments removed a total of 75.7 ha of timber, meaning that only 6.43% of the forest within the six treated sites was harvested (Kaminski 2010). This is a relatively small-scale harvest, but it was done in two state forests that lie within the highly fragmented agricultural Midwest. Similar harvests in more contiguously forested habitats may contribute to greater reproductive success in those areas. 26 Literature Cited Annand, E. M. and F. R. Thompson III. 1997. Forest bird response to regeneration practices in central hardwood forests. Journal of Wildlife Management 61:159 171. Bakermans, M. H. and A. D. Rodewald. 2009. Think globally, manage locally: The importance of steady-state forest features for a declining songbird. Forest Ecology and Management 258:224-232. Beers, T. W., P. E. Dress, and L. C. Wensel. 1966. Notes and observations. Aspect transformation in site productivity research. Journal of Forestry 64:691-692. Bent, A. C. 1953. Life Histories of North American Wood Warblers, parts I and II. Dover Publications, New York. Birdlife International. 2004. Threatened Birds of the World. CD-ROM. Birdlife International, Cambridge, UK. Boardman, L. A., and R. H. Yahner. 1999. Wildlife communities associated with even aged reproduction stands in two state forests of Pennsylvania. Northern Journal of Applied Forestry 16:89-95. Boves, T. J. 2011. Multiple responses by Cerulean Warblers to experimental forest disturbance in the Appalachian Mountains. PhD dissertation, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN. Brawn, J. D. and S. K. Robinson. 1996. Source-sink population dynamics may complicate the interpretation of long-term census data. Ecology 77:3-12. Brooks, T. M., S. L. Pimm, and J. O. Oyugi. 1999. Time lags between deforestation and bird extinction in tropical forest fragments. Conservation Biology 13:1140-1150. Buehler, D. A., J. J. Giocomo, J. Jones, P. B. Hamel, C. M. Rogers, T. A. Beachy, D. W. Varble, C .P. Nicholson, K. L. Roth., J. Barg, R. J. Robertson, J. R. Robb, and K. Islam. 2008. Cerulean Warbler Reproduction, Survival, and Models of Population Decline. Journal of Wildlife Management 72:646-653. Duguay, J. P., P. B. Wood, and J. V. Nichols. 2001. Songbird Abundance and Avian Nest Survival Rates in Forests Fragmented by Different Silvicultural Treatments. Conservation Biology 15:1405-1415. Falls, J. B. 1981. Mapping territories with playback: an accurate census method for songbirds. Studies in Avian Biology 6:86-91. 27 Flaspohler, D. J. 1993. Wisconsin Cerulean Warbler Recovery Plan. Bureau of Endangered Resources Technical Publication #96. Wisconsin Department of Natural Resources, Madison, WI. Hamel, P. B. 2000. Cerulean Warbler (Dendroica cerulea). The Birds of North America, No. 511 (A. Poole and F. Gill, eds.). The Birds of North America, Inc., Philadelphia, PA. Hamel, P. B., D. K. Dawson, and P. D. Keyser. 2004. How we can learn more about the Cerulean Warbler (Dendroica cerulea). Auk 121:7-14. Hamel, P. B., M. J. Welton, C. G. Smith, III, and R. P. Ford. 2009. Test of Partners in Flight effective detection distance for cerulean warbler. Pp. 328–333 in Rich, T. D., C. Arizmendi, D. W. Demarest, and C. Thompson (eds). Tundra to tropics: connecting birds, habitats, and people. Proceedings of the 4th international Partners in Flight conference 13-16 February 2008, McAllen, TX. Hardwood Ecosystem Experiment. Online http://www.fnr.purdue.edu/HEE/Overview/Overview.htm Islam, K., and C. Basile. 2002. Relative abundance and habitat selection of Cerulean Warblers in Southern Indiana. Final report submitted to U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Fort Snelling, MN, December 2002. Department of Biology Technical Report No. 1, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana 76 pp. Islam, K., and K. L. Roth. 2004. Habitat Selection and Reproductive Success of Cerulean Warblers in Southern Indiana. Final report submitted to U.S. Fish & Wildlife Service, Fort Snelling, MN, December 2004. Department of Biology Technical Report No. 4, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana 51 pp. James, F. C. and H. H. Shugart. 1970. A Quantitative Method of Habitat Description. Audubon Field Notes 24:727-736. Jones, J., P. Ramoni-Perazzi, E. H. Carruthers, and R. J. Robertson. 2000. Sociality and foraging behavior of the Cerulean Warbler in Venezuelan shade coffee plantations. Condor 102:958-962. Jones, J., R. D. DeBruyn, J. J. Barg, and R. J. Robertson. 2001. Assessing the effects of natural disturbance on a Neotropical migrant songbird. Ecology 82:2628– 2635. Jones, J. and R. J. Robertson. 2001. Territory and nest-site selection of Cerulean Warblers in eastern Ontario. Auk 118:727-735. Jones, K. and K. Islam. 2006. Selection of song perches by Cerulean Warblers. Proceedings of the Indiana Academy of Science 115:37-43. 28 Kaminski, K. J. 2010. Cerulean Warbler Initial Response to Silviculture Treatments in Southern Indiana. M.S. Thesis, Ball State University, Muncie, IN. LeBlanc, D., 2008. Statistics: concepts and applications for science. XanEdu Publishers, Michigan. Lynch, J.M. 1981. Status of the cerulean warbler in the Roanoke River basin of North Carolina. Chat 45, 29-35. MacNeil, M. 2010. Does timber harvesting affect Cerulean Warbler foraging ecology? M.S. Thesis, Ball State University, Muncie, IN. Marshall, M. R., J. A. Dececco, A. B. Williams, G. A. Gale, and R. J. Cooper. 2003. Use of regenerating clearcuts by late-successional bird species and their young during the post-fledging period. Forest Ecology and Management 183:127-135. McDermott, M. E., and P. B. Wood. 2009. Short and long-term implications of clearcut and 2-age silviculture for conservation of breeding forest birds in the central Appalachians, USA. Biological Conservation 142:212-220. Oliarnyk, C. J. 1996. Habitat selection and reproductive success in a population of Cerulean Warblers in southeastern Ontario. M.S. Thesis, Queen’s University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada. Oliarnyk, C. J. and R. J. Robertson. 1996. Breeding behavior and reproductive success of Cerulean Warblers in Southeastern Ontario. Wilson Bulletin 108:673-684. Pärt, T., Arlt, D., and Villard, M-.A. 2007. Empirical evidence for ecological traps: a twostep model focusing on individual decisions. Journal of Ornithology 148:S327 S332. Register, S. M. and K. Islam. 2008. Effects of Silvicultural Treatments on Cerulean Warbler (Dendroica cerulea) Abundance in Southern Indiana. Forest Ecology and Management 255:3502-3505. Robbins, C. S., J. W. Fitzpatrick, and P. B. Hamel. 1992. A warbler in trouble: Dendroica cerulea. In: Ecology of Conservation of Neotropical Migrant Landbirds. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington DC, pp. 549-562. Roth, K. L. and K. Islam. 2008. Habitat Selection and Reproductive Success of Cerulean Warblers in Indiana. Wilson Journal of Ornithology 120:105-110. Sauer, J. R., J. E. Hines and J. Fallon. [online]. 2008. The North American Breeding Bird Survey,Results and Analysis 1966-2007 version 5.15,2008. USGS Patuxent Wildlife Research Center, Laurel, Maryland, USA. 29 Sharma, S. 1996. Applied Multivariate Techniques. John Wiley and Sons, Inc., New York. Thompson, F. R., J. R. Probst, and M. G. Raphael. 1993. Silvicultural options for Neotropical migratory birds. In: D. M. Finch, P. W. Stangel, (eds.). Status and management of neotropical migratory birds: September 21-25, 1992, Estes Park, Colorado. General Technical Report RM-229. Fort Collins, CO: Rocky Mountain Forest and Range Experiment Station, U.S. Dept. of Agriculture, Forest Service:353-362. U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service. 2008. Birds of conservation concern. United States Department of Interior, Fish and Wildlife Service, Division of Migratory Bird Management, Arlington, VA, USA. Van Horne, B. 1983. Density as a misleading indicator of habitat quality. Journal of Wildlife Management 47:893-901. Vitz, A. C., and A. D. Rodewald. 2006. Can regenerating clearcuts benefit mature-forest songbirds? An examination of post-breeding ecology. Biological Conservation 127:477-486. Weakland, C.A. and P. B. Wood. 2005. Cerulean Warbler (Dendroica cerulea) microhabitat and landscape level habitat characteristics in southern West Virginia. Auk 122:497-508. Wood, P. B., S. B. Bosworth, and R. Dettmers. 2005a. Cerulean Warbler abundance and occurrence relative to large-scale edge affect habitat characteristics. Condor 108:154-165. Wood, P. B., J. P. Duguay, and J. V. Nichols. 2005b. Cerulean warbler use of regenerated clear-cut and two-age harvests. Wildlife Society Bulletin 33:851 858. Young, L. C. 2007. Distribution and foraging ecology of Cerulean Warblers in Southern Indiana. M.A. Thesis, Ball State University, Muncie, IN. 30 ©Cortney Mycroft 2008 Figure 1. Hardwood Ecosystem Experiment (HEE) management units used to study Cerulean Warblers in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. 31 Figure 2. Cerulean Warbler territories and random vegetation plots mapped in 2010 and 2011 in an uneven-aged unit (Unit 7) within Yellowwood state forest in Southern Indiana. 32 Figure 3. Cerulean Warbler territories and random vegetation plots mapped in 2010 and 2011 in an even-aged unit (Unit 3) within Morgan-Monroe state forest in Southern Indiana. 33 Figure 4. Cerulean Warbler territories and random vegetation plots mapped in 2010 and 2011 in a control unit (Unit 5) within Yellowwood state forest in Southern Indiana. 34 Territory Number Among Experimental Units 1.6 Log Territory Number 1.4 1.2 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 Control Even Experimental Unit Uneven Figure 5: Number of Cerulean Warbler territories on a logarithmic scale among all three experimental units at Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. Over all five years, even-aged units had significantly more territories than control units but did not differ significantly from uneven-aged units. 35 Territory Size in Even-aged Units Log Territory Size (ha) 0.0 -0.5 -1.0 -1.5 -2.0 Pre Post Figure 6. Cerulean Warbler territory sizes on a logarithmic scale of even-aged units preand post-harvest at Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. Despite the significant increase in the number of territories (Figure 5), territory size did not decrease significantly. 36 Territory Sizes Among Experimental Units Log Territory Size (ha) 0.0 -0.5 -1.0 -1.5 -2.0 -2.5 Control Even Experimental Unit Uneven Figure 7. Cerulean Warbler territory sizes on a logarithmic scale among all three experimental units at Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. Territories in even-aged units were significantly smaller than control units but did not differ significantly from uneven-aged units. 37 Figure 8. Cerulean Warbler territories demarcated in 2010 and 2011 aggregated near a shelterwood cut in Unit 9 at Yellowwood state forest in Southern Indiana. 38 Table 1. Numbers of Cerulean Warblers detected in 2010 and 2011 in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. Notice that Units 3 and 8 consistently have greater numbers of birds than other units. R.A. = Relative Abundance. Harvest Type Control Control Control Total Even-aged Even-aged Even-aged Total Uneven-aged Uneven-aged Uneven-aged Total Total Unit # 2 4 5 3 3 6 9 3 1 7 8 3 9 2010 5 3 7 15 22 13 16 51 1 8 18 27 93 R.A. 2.55 1.53 3.57 2.55 11.22 6.63 8.16 8.67 0.51 4.08 9.18 4.59 2011 7 8 19 34 19 26 11 56 1 10 29 40 130 R.A. 3.57 4.08 9.69 5.78 9.69 13.27 5.61 9.52 0.51 5.1 14.8 6.8 39 Table 2. Number of Cerulean Warbler territories and their mean sizes from pre-harvest years (2007-2008) and post-harvest years (2009-2011) at Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. Values are presented as the mean ± 1 SD. Terr. = Territories. Harvest Site 2007 (# of terr.) Area (ha) 2008 (# of terr.) Area (ha) 2009 (# of terr.) Area (ha) 2010 (# of terr.) Area (ha) 2011 (# of terr.) Area (ha) Uneven 1 7 8 0 7 24 0 0.18 ± 0.08 0.18 ± 0.17 1 7 21 0.44 ± 0 0.86 ± 0.59 0.19 ± 0.10 0 6 26 0 0.37 ± 0.26 0.22 ± 0.21 0 3 16 0 0.14 ± 0.05 0.25 ± 0.33 0 6 31 0 0.38 ± 0.27 0.20 ± 0.14 Even 3 6 9 8 6 9 0.18 ± 0.14 0.16 ± 0.15 0.13 ± 0.13 13 0 5 0.28 ± 0.16 0 0.31 ± 0.27 21 6 7 0.17 ± 0.23 0.26 ± 0.24 0.09 ± 0.03 16 5 14 0.16 ± 0.10 0.27 ± 0.21 0.15 ± 0.15 13 14 13 0.20 ± 0.22 0.16 ± 0.19 0.23 ± 0.26 Control 2 4 5 1 4 6 0.11 ± 0 0.4 ± 0.46 0.11 ± 0.07 3 2 3 0.4 ± 0.11 0.44 ± 0.04 0.69 ± 0.24 0 3 7 0 0.19 ± 0.11 0.24 ± 0.14 2 0 2 0.38 ± 0.05 0 0.37 ± 0.05 2 10 12 0.59 ± 0.49 0.33 ± 0.25 0.16 ± 0.15 Total: 65 55 Summary Statistics Variable 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 N 65 55 76 58 101 Mean SE Mean 0.1774 0.0236 0.3628 0.0462 0.2102 0.0251 0.2046 0.0287 0.2276 0.0226 StDev Minimum Maximum 0.1899 0.0147 1.1827 0.3426 0.0190 1.7513 0.2186 0.0197 0.8867 0.2187 0.0032 1.2076 0.2266 0.0067 1.0813 76 58 101 40 Table 3. Results of ANOVAs that examined how vegetative characteristics differed between Cerulean Warbler use and non-use sites in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. Highlighted cells show F and p-values that were considered significant after Bonferroni adjustments were made. Values are presented as the mean ± 1 SD. Response variable Canopy height Number of woody species Species of woody species Number of woody species (0-3 cm) Number of woody species (3-10 cm) Number of trees Species of trees Tree diameter at breast height (DBH) ANOVA Mean (use) Mean (non-use) 0.83 (0.365) 24.03 ± 3.82 24.47 ± 3.70 5.16 (0.024) 26.57 ± 17.11 21.5 ± 14.38 4.87 (0.028) 5.74 ± 2.53 4.99 ± 2.23 8.35 (0.004) 24.02 ± 17.28 17.63 ± 13.81 11.14 (0.001) 2.55 ± 2.5 3.88 ± 3.14 4.29 (0.040) 12.10 ± 4.58 13.42 ± 4.43 2.07 (0.152) 5.51 ± 2.02 5.14 ± 1.70 0.25 (0.615) 28.25 ± 5.71 27.87 ± 5.01 41 Table 4. Spatial attributes of Cerulean Warbler territories (use) and random non-use areas in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. Results of 2-sample t-tests that were used to compare all attributes among areas are reported below. Bonferroni adjustments were made for all multiple comparisons of variables that were not considered independent. Attribute Conclusion Use (n) Distance to stream Distance to road Distance to harvest Distance to conifer patch Slope Aspect Elevation S NS NS S NS S NS 159 159 159 159 159 159 159 Non-use (n) Median (use) Median (non-use) 91 91 91 91 91 91 91 157.1 78 252 192.2 36.6 109.47 755.22 284.1 111.2 209 407.4 37.7 226.77 773.52 P < 0.001 0.074 0.277 < 0.001 0.522 <0.001 0.21 Use (n) = number of territories. Non-use (n) = number of random non-use areas. Median (use) = median distance of territories to attribute in m, with the exception of slope and aspect (degrees). Median (non-use) = median distance of random non-use areas to attribute in m, with the exception of slope and aspect (degrees). S = significant. NS = not significant -Chapter 2Conspecific Social Cues Strongly Influence Cerulean Warbler Male Settlement Patterns in a Managed Forest Ryan H. Dibala 43 Abstract Population age-structure and differential settlement patterns may have important consequences for reproductive output and population dynamics in bird populations, especially those that are in sharp decline. The Cerulean Warbler (Setophaga cerulea) is one species that has declined drastically throughout its range. This species was monitored in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana to address questions pertaining to age-specific male settlement patterns and clustered territoriality. A total of 130 male Cerulean Warblers were detected and 101 territories were demarcated across eight of nine study sites. Thirty of 96 males (31%) were classified as second year (SY) birds, 66 of 96 (69%) were classified as after second year (ASY) birds, and five birds remained of unknown age. Using a nearest neighbor analysis, 80 of 101 (79.21%) territories located within five of eight study sites were clustered. ASY males were no more likely to cluster than SY males (p = 0.258). SY male mean distance to ASY males was significantly closer than mean distance to other SY males (p = 0.001). Similarly, ASY male mean distance to other ASY males was significantly closer than mean distance to SY males (p = 0.001), showing that both ageclasses have a clear preference for settling closer to experienced males. There were no differences in vegetation or distance to landscape features between the age-classes, supporting social explanations for settlement. These results provide evidence that Cerulean Warbler settlement patterns are strongly influenced by social cues. Keywords: Cerulean Warbler, Setophaga cerulea, social attraction, age-structure, cluster 44 Introduction Habitat selection of forest songbirds has been well studied (reviewed by Jones 2001 and Johnson 2007), perhaps most effectively by examining male settlement patterns concomitantly with reproductive success in territorial species (Landmann and Kollinsky 1995, Morris and Lemon 1988, Ficken and Ficken 1967, Holmes et al. 1996). These studies have depicted the importance of accounting for both habitat components and social mechanisms while evaluating habitat quality. Despite several informative studies (Betts et al. 2008, Wagner 1997, Cornell and Donovan 2010, Muller et al. 1997), the relative importance of habitat and social cues still remains largely unknown. Instead of measuring territory quality directly in ways that would be difficult or time-consuming, birds may need to rely on an indirect assessment of territory value, such as using social cues from conspecifics (Stamps 1987). In fact, for many short-lived species, relying on personally gathered information may be very costly (Stamps and Krishnan 2005). Many studies have provided evidence that males may be attracted not only to certain habitat characteristics but to various conspecific social cues (Collias and Collias 1969, Wagner 1997, Muller et al. 1997). More recently, research has focused on proximate cues provided by conspecifics (reviewed in Ahlering and Faaborg 2006) and a growing body of evidence supports the hypothesis that social cues play an important role in settlement (Muller et al. 1997, Danchin et al. 2000, Ward and Schlossberg 2004, Betts et al. 2008, Cornell and Donovan 2010). For example, researchers have shown 45 that conspecific songs can have an effect on bird settlement patterns (Alatalo et al. 1982, Mills et al. 2006, Betts et al. 2008, Ward and Schlossberg 2004). All-purpose territories are sometimes aggregated in dense concentrations, or clusters, across the landscape leaving seemingly suitable habitats vacant (Stamps 1987). This pattern has been observed in many species of New and Old World wood warblers, including Black-throated Blue Warbler (Setophaga caerulescens, Holmes et al. 1996), Wood Warbler (Phyloscopus sibilatrix, Herremans 1993), Yellow Warbler (Setophaga petechial, Clark and Robertson 1979) and Kirtland’s Warbler (Setophaga kirtlandii, Mayfield 1960, Burgoyne and Ryel 1978). At least eight hypotheses have been proposed to explain clustering behavior in animals (see Tarof and Ratcliffe 2004), the most common of which are the material resource hypothesis and the predation hypothesis (Igor Dias et al. 2009). The former predicts clustered settlement to occur in response to the patchiness of ecological resources such as vegetation or food (Kiester and Slatkin 1974). The presence of conspecifics could signal the availability of quality habitat at a particular location (Stamps 1987, Danchin and Wagner 1997). The predation hypothesis proposes that clustering reduces predation due to increased vigilance and communication among conspecifics (Hamilton 1971). A third explanation, known as the hidden-lek hypothesis, proposes that the clustering of territories results from female pursuit of extra-pair copulations from high quality males that are already socially paired (Wagner 1997). Additionally, large aggregations could easily attract numerous females, making it easier for males to secure 46 a mate (Danchin and Wagner 1997). These explanations imply that because of their naivety, young individuals should rely more heavily on conspecific cues than older, more experienced individuals (Stamps 1987, Danchin et al. 2000). Thus, when studying male settlement patterns it is essential to differentiate between naïve settlers from those who are returning to previously used habitats (Muller et al. 1997). In many species of birds, territory quality and associated breeding success increases with age (Ficken and Ficken 1967, Hill 1988, Lozano et al. 1996). It has been well documented that older males arrive on the breeding grounds earlier and are more successful breeders than yearling males (Brown 1969, Fretwell and Lucas 1970, Hill 1988, Lozano et al. 1996, Muller et al. 1997). This late arrival could be an adaptive response by young males to avoid intense competition early in the season (Hill 1988, Morton and Derrickson 1990). Older birds have more familiarity with the breeding area and by arriving earlier have the advantages of minimal competition for territory placement, greater mate availability, and a longer overall breeding season. Conversely, yearling males must establish territories wherever they can and therefore often occupy lower quality habitat patches (Wooller and Coulson 1997, Newton 1988). Examining differential settlement patterns and age-specific reproductive output can provide important demographic information about breeding bird populations, especially those that are in sharp decline. One species in particular, the Cerulean Warbler (Setophaga cerulea), has become increasingly scarce across much of North America. This long-distance Nearctic-Neotropical migrant has been described as a 47 “loosely colonial” species (Hamel 2000, Roth and Islam 2007) that engages in aggressive territorial defense against conspecifics, providing an excellent study subject for questions regarding intraspecific sociality and age-specific habitat differences. It is likely that territory establishment and settlement patterns are influenced by both structural habitat cues and social mechanisms. Cerulean Warblers are an area-sensitive species that depend on large, contiguous blocks of mature deciduous forest and tall trees for successful breeding (Lynch 1981, Hamel 2000, Jones and Robertson 2001, Roth and Islam 2008). It appears that a heterogeneous, three-dimensional structure of the canopy within the forest is an important landscape feature for this species (Lynch 1981, Hamel 2000, Jones et al. 2001). In Ohio, Cerulean Warbler density and nesting success were positively associated with canopy openness, numbers of large-diameter trees, and number of grapevines (Bakermans and Rodewald 2009). However, if physical habitat components were the only reason for clustering, then territories would be spaced more evenly throughout the landscape (Roth and Islam 2007). Roth and Islam (2007) observed that enough suitable and available habitats are left unoccupied by Cerulean Warblers that social aspects must play an important role in settlement. Yearling, or second year (SY) male fecundity and settlement patterns have not been documented for the Cerulean Warbler and yet are critical in the evaluation of breeding habitat and age-specific demography. Throughout the range of the Blackthroated Blue Warbler, Graves (1997) found a disproportionate number of yearling males in geographically separated and peripheral habitats, possibly leading to the 48 formation of distinct “sink” populations. A study of breeding American Redstart (Setophaga ruticilla) males showed that SY males were forced into marginal breeding habitat by older males (Ficken and Ficken 1967, Sherry and Holmes 1989). In Southern Ontario, Jones et al. (2004) thought 15% of Cerulean Warbler males were SY birds and in Michigan, Rogers (2006) found Cerulean Warbler age structure to be strongly biased toward older males and suggested that further research be conducted on habitat specificity and reproductive success of age-structured breeding populations. Investigating conspecific social dynamics along with site-specific vegetation structure can help identify preferred Cerulean Warbler habitat. The purpose of this study was to determine whether or not there were differences in settlement patterns, territory size, reproductive success, and vegetation structure between second year (SY) and after second year (ASY) males. The following questions were addressed: 1) Where do SY and ASY males occur on the landscape? 2) What is the age ratio of this particular population? 3.) What proportion of SY and ASY males are clustered? 3) Is there a difference in territory sizes between age-classes? 4) Are there differences in reproductive success between age-classes? 5) Are there differences in physical habitat structure between age-classes? Based on a priori published information indicating that experienced adults return to breeding sites earliest and presumably have greater reproductive potential, I predicted that Cerulean Warbler ASY males would account for the largest proportion of clustered territories and reproductively successful individuals. I expected that ASY birds would defend larger 49 territories because of their greater level of experience and consequent resource-holding potential. I also predicted that ASY birds would be associated with areas inhabited by larger trees, grape vine, steeper slopes, and shrub cover, four variables that have been shown to be important in Cerulean Warbler habitat selection (Hamel 2000, Bakermans and Rodewald 2009, Kaminski 2010, Dibala unpublished data). Materials and Methods Focal Species and Study Area The Cerulean Warbler is a Nearctic-Neotropical migrant songbird that breeds in mature deciduous forests of eastern and central North America and winters on the Andean slopes of northwestern South America (Hamel 2000). Due to loss of contiguous forest cover and habitat fragmentation, this species is facing immediate threats in both its breeding and wintering ranges. It is now considered North America’s fastest declining wood warbler, as populations are estimated to have dropped by approximately 70% since 1966 (Sauer et al. 2008). This decline is so alarming that the species has been listed on the International Union for the Conservation of Nature’s (IUCN) red list as “vulnerable” (Birdlife International 2004) and is considered stateendangered in Indiana. In fact, the threat is so high that a sense of urgency attends the study of this species of concern (Hamel et al. 2004). I studied Cerulean Warblers in 2010 and 2011 at nine management units established by the Hardwood Ecosystem Experiment (HEE) in Brown, Morgan, and 50 Monroe counties of Southern Indiana. Launched in 2006, the HEE is a 100-year study involving a consortium of state agencies and Universities to determine the effects of forest management practices on native plants and animals of Indiana (HEE online). The project encompasses nine management units (~282-363 ha) in Yellowwood and Morgan- Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana (Figure 1, Chapter 1) and is comprised of silviculture treatments grouped into one of two classifications: even-aged and uneven-aged management (Thompson III et al. 1993). Harvests took place in the summer of 2008, providing an excellent opportunity to monitor Cerulean Warbler population response to human disturbance. The study site lies within the largest block of contiguous forest in Southern Indiana and fits descriptions of quality Cerulean Warbler habitat. Point Count Surveys One hundred-meter fixed radius point count surveys were conducted throughout the month of May to locate singing males that were establishing territories. Surveys were conducted within a 259 ha sample grid consisting of 49 bird survey points (7x7; 1.96 km2 with a 50 m buffer). These points were arranged in seven north-south transect lines with 200 m between each point to rule out the possibility of dual detection (Jones et al. 2000, Hamel et al. 2009). Surveys were conducted between 05:30 and 10:30 EST and none took place on windy or rainy days. One point count was conducted for each survey point and lasted five minutes. The first two minutes were spent listening for singing males. This was followed by one minute of Cerulean Warbler 51 song broadcasting via an MP3 player and an external speaker to elicit a vocal response from any males in the immediate area. The last two minutes were again spent listening without playback (Falls 1981). When a bird was detected, its approximate distance from the point and compass direction were recorded. Territory Demarcation Points where birds were initially heard were revisited to delineate territory boundaries in 2010 and 2011. Male Cerulean Warblers were located by their incessant singing, and trees in which birds sang repetitively were identified to species and marked with flagging tape. A Garmin 76 handheld Global Positioning System (GPS) unit was used to mark the location of the tree in a Universal Transverse Mercator (UTM) coordinate system. A territory was considered complete only when between 5 and 12 trees had been recorded for an individual. Territory locations and sizes were analyzed by creating minimum convex polygons in ArcMap (ArcGIS 10) and territory boundaries were assessed based on their distance to landscape features and harvests. Age-class Identification Throughout the territory demarcation process, the resident male was viewed with binoculars as many times as possible to begin assessing its age-class. Once a minimum of five trees were marked for any particular territory, I navigated to the approximate center of the territory and broadcasted a recording of a male song with an MP3 player with an external speaker for one minute. The following two minutes were 52 spent listening and watching for the male and photographs were taken when possible. If the bird did not approach within 15 m to make visual observations, another minute was spent advertising the song. The last minute was spent listening and watching for the resident male. To avoid creating a bias, playback song selection was kept consistent across all territories. Additionally, playback was used conservatively so as not to overstimulate and increase aggressive interaction among neighboring birds. All behavioral and physical observations were recorded for each territory. Males were identified as either second year (SY) or after second year (ASY) by documenting a combination of physical characteristics based off of field observation. SY males were identified by the presence of a white superciliary stripe, incomplete or faint breast band, and reduced streaking on the back. ASY males lack a prominent white supercilium, possess a full, dark breast band, and are azure blue above with prominent black streaking (Dunn and Garrett 1997). It is important to note that ASY males may possess a small white remnant patch of the supercilium at the back of the eye. Therefore, in order to confidently conclude that a male belonged to a particular ageclass, a combination of at least two of these three physical characteristics were confirmed. All birds were observed from a distance of at least 15 m. Birds designated to a particular age-class based on only one physical characteristic were excluded from the data (See Appendix II). To eliminate potential observer bias, I made all final male age-class identifications. Reproductive Output 53 Nest searching commenced in late April of 2011. Direct observation of a female on a male’s territory and her general behavior often exposed the location of the nest. Audible cues were used readily in pinpointing the nest location, as a paired female would often communicate to a nearby male with a high-pitched chip note and a mated male would sing softly in close proximity to the nest (Barg et al. 2007). When a nest was found, it was monitored closely at least once every two days and was determined successful if at least one nestling fledged (Mayfield 1975). Additionally, all territories where Cerulean Warbler fledglings were seen begging or being fed by adults were considered successful. Because all territories were monitored closely throughout the season and the twitter of fledglings was quite conspicuous, it is unlikely that fledglings went undetected (Jones et al. 2004, Rogers 2006). Therefore, in addition to nests that failed, I considered territories unsuccessful if no nest and/or fledglings were found. Thus, I defined nesting success as: Successful nests + territories with fledglings/unsuccessful nests + unknown. Vegetation Sampling Vegetation sampling began at the beginning of July when detection proved difficult due to a substantial decrease in male singing rate. Sampling was conducted at the center of each territory where a 0.04 ha circular plot was marked. Using the methods of James and Shugart (1970), aspect was assessed at the plot’s origin and slope was measured with a clinometer in each cardinal direction. Presence and absence of canopy and ground cover were recorded with a GRS densitometer at 2 m intervals along 54 each cardinal direction of the plot. Woody plants with a diameter at breast height (DBH) ≤3 cm and 3-10 cm were identified to species within a 5 m radius of the plot’s center. In each quadrant, mature trees (>10 cm DBH) both living and dead were identified to species and a rangefinder was used to measure the maximum canopy height in each quadrant. The presence or absence of grapevine (Vitis sp.) within each plot was also noted. GIS Analysis ArcMap (ArcGIS 10) was used to create a geodatabase in which all geospatial data were displayed and analyzed. UTM coordinates were imported to ArcMap and polygons were constructed using a two-step process: 1) triangular irregular networks (TINs) were created to display three dimensional attributes in vector format and 2) an attribute domain was then created to ensure data integrity. Each domain represented a separate territory and included metadata about the shape’s length and area. Domains were added to a larger feature class depending on individual datasets. For example, separate feature classes were created for SY/ASY males as well as for clustered/nonclustered and reproductively successful/unsuccessful Cerulean Warblers. These layers were superimposed onto GIS layers of the nine management units with associated landscape features and harvest areas. These, along with raster data such as aerial photographs and a digital elevation model (DEM) were obtained from the Indiana Spatial Data Portal (Kaminski 2010). These files were used to analyze differences in 55 elevation, slope, and aspect of Cerulean Warbler territories that were derived from the vector polygons using the zonal statistics tool under Zonal in Spatial Analyst Tools. Territory centroids were created for polygon feature classes using the feature to point tool. Distances were measured to and from the centroids of polygon features by generating a Near Table under Proximity in Analysis Tools. Distances to landscape features such as harvest areas, roads, streams, conifer stands, and neighboring territories were calculated for SY and ASY males and clustered and non-clustered territories. Additionally, territory sizes were compared between SY and ASY males and clustered and non-clustered males. Statistical Analyses A Fisher’s Exact Test was used to determine differences in mating success between males from the two age-classes. I used a two-sample t-test to determine whether successful SY males could contribute their success to settling closer to any ASY male, regardless of whether the ASY male was successful. Differences in vegetative characteristics between the age-classes were investigated by using Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA, Sharma 1996). Clustering on the landscape was determined statistically by using the Average Nearest Neighbor tool under Analyzing Patterns in Spatial Statistics tools. This tool calculates the distances between the centroids of each territory and the nearest neighboring territory and compares these values to the expected neighboring distances 56 if territories were dispersed randomly throughout the study area (Krebs 1989). The area of each management unit (1.96 km2) was specified for each analysis and a z-test at α = 0.05 was used to determine if the spatial pattern of territories within each management unit was clustered (Figure 1). Pooled data are presented for settlement patterns observed in 2010 and 2011. Two sample t-tests were used to test for differences (LeBlanc 2008) in spatial attributes such as distances to landscape features (streams, roads, harvests), slope, aspect, and elevation between age-classes. A paired t-test was used to compare the distance of nearest neighboring SY and ASY males to members of each age class. Additionally, nearest neighbor distance values that pooled both age-classes were calculated for all territories and a Mann-Whitney U-test was used to test for differences in settlement proximity. Bonferroni adjustments were made for multiple comparisons of variables that were not considered independent. All data were checked for normality and non-normal data were log-transformed. In cases where the data were extremely positively skewed, the non-parametric MannWhitney U-test was used because of its higher power in these situations (LeBlanc personal communication). Therefore, median values are presented in some cases. All tests were performed with Minitab (Version 16) at α = 0.05. Results Age-class structure and Reproductive Success 57 A total of 130 male Cerulean Warblers were detected and 101 territories were demarcated across eight of nine management units. Cerulean Warblers were absent in the one unit when revisited and therefore no territories were marked in that unit. Thirty of 101 males (29.70%) were classified as SY birds, 66 of 101 (65.35%) were classified as ASY birds, and five birds (4.95%) remained of unknown age-class (Figure 2). These five birds were excluded from all age-class comparison analyses. Thirty-one territories (10 SY and 21 ASY) had either a successful nest or fledglings present. SY birds made up nearly half of all successful males and no difference was found in reproductive success between ASY and SY males (p = 1.000, Fisher’s Exact Test, Figure 3). Additionally, reproductively successful SY males were no closer to ASY males than to reproductively unsuccessful SY males (t = -1.37, p = 0.182, df = 27, 2-sample t-test with a log transformation). Vegetation There were no differences in vegetative characteristics between SY and ASY male Cerulean Warbler territories (Wilks’ λ, F11,37 = 0.794, p = 0.645). Clustering A total of 80 out of 101 (79.21%) territories located within five of eight management units were clustered (Table 1). This is almost identical to clustering patterns in 2010 where 46 of 58 (79.31%) territories and three of seven management units were clustered. ASY males were no more likely to cluster than SY males (p = 0.258, 58 Fisher’s Exact Test). However, a Mann-Whitney U-test of nearest neighbor values showed that ASY male median distance to nearest neighbor (74.8 m) was significantly closer than SY male median distance to nearest neighbor (103 m, p = 0.030). There was no significant difference in reproductive success between clustered and non-clustered sites (p = 1.000, Fisher’s Exact Test). However, there was a significant difference in median territory size between non-clustered territories (0.191 ha) and clustered territories (0.143 ha, p = 0.0235, Mann-Whitney U-test). No differences existed between clustered and non-clustered territories in median distances to streams (p = 0.843), roads (p = 0.642), and harvest areas (p = 0.256). Similarly, no differences were found between the two groups in mean slope (p = 0.367), mean aspect (p = 0.891, with a log transformation), and mean elevation (p = 0.240). Settlement Patterns Interestingly, SY male mean territory size was significantly larger than ASY mean territory size (t = -2.09, p = 0.039, df = 94, 2-sample t-test with a log transformation, Figure 4). SY male mean distance to ASY males (176.2 m) was significantly closer than mean distance to other SY males (328.5 m, t = 3.58, p = 0.001, Figure 5). Similarly, ASY male mean distance to other ASY males (123.3 m) was significantly closer than mean distance to SY males (215 m, t = 3.33, p = 0.001, Figure 6), showing that both age-classes have a clear preference for settling closer to experienced males. No differences were found between age-classes in median distances to streams (p = 0.545), roads (p = 0.701), and harvest areas (p = 0.530). Likewise, there were no significant differences in 59 mean slope (p = 0.384) and elevation (p = 0.369), however median aspect differed significantly between SY (90.46°) and ASY (116.53°) male territories (p = 0.013, MannWhitney U-test). SY males appear to be using easterly facing slopes while ASY males are using southeasterly facing slopes (Table 2). Discussion Age-class Structure I found that 29.70% of Cerulean Warbler males in Yellowwood and MorganMonroe state forests were SY birds. This is similar to the 27% SY proportion presented by Boves (2011) in his four year study in the Appalachian Mountains. In Southern Ontario, Jones et al. (2004) concluded that 15% of Cerulean Warbler males were SY birds. At a study site in Michigan, Rogers (2006) claimed that SY birds made up 10-20% of the male population. Multiple studies have suggested that there is wide variation in SY birds among years. Holmes et al. (1992) found that numbers of ASY male Blackthroated Blue Warblers remained relatively stable whereas numbers of SY males fluctuated widely over a four year period (31-56% of all males). In their study, the ratio of SY/ASY males varied among years, with the highest percentage of SY males during years of highest Black-throated Blue Warbler densities. Graves (1997) indicated that Black-throated Blue Warbler yearling males exhibited substantial variation among years; however, he found that peripheral habitats where relative abundance was lower had a higher proportion of SY males. In a study conducted by Bayne and Hobson (2001), the proportion of SY male Ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla) was higher in farm fragments 60 than in forest fragments or contiguous forest. He suggested that a skewed age-ratio among landscapes could mean that older males are forcing yearlings out of optimal breeding habitats and into inferior areas. Although there were no obvious examples of habitat segregation between the age-classes on a local scale in Cerulean Warblers, larger landscape-level factors could be playing a role in age-class structure. Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests are large forested tracts of land that are located in the heart of the agricultural Midwest. These forests could be acting as sink habitats for younger, less-experienced individuals that are excluded from optimal breeding areas by adults (Cornell and Donovan 2010). Buehler et al. (2008) reviewed Cerulean Warbler breeding studies throughout its range and claimed that sites in non-forested landscapes such as Indiana may remain as sinks without major landscape restoration. They conjectured that populations in these areas are being maintained by immigration from other regions. This hypothesis is not supported by the findings of Boves (2011), who investigated the age structure of Cerulean Warblers in several areas throughout the Appalachian Mountains and found that 27% of all males were SY birds. Considering that this area is known to harbor the highest density of successfully breeding Cerulean Warblers in North America (Buehler et al. 2008), the higher proportion of SY males could suggest greater levels of annual recruitment, indicating the availability of higher quality habitat. Future research is needed to investigate the age structure of Cerulean 61 Warbler populations within managed forests of the Midwest and other highly fragmented landscapes and to determine how it compares to more contiguous forests. Reproductive Output There was no difference in reproductive success between the age-classes or nonclustered and clustered units. Jones et al. (2004) modeled SY fecundity as 80% that of ASY males based on findings from other studies because no data existed for SY male Cerulean Warbler reproductive success (Nolan 1978, Saether 1990, Forslund and Part 1995, Holmes et al. 1996). My research showed that nearly half (47.62%) of successful Cerulean Warbler territories belonged to SY males. Since males of both age-classes settled in similar areas, they likely had equal access to females. It may be likely that mature adult males allow yearlings to settle in close proximity in order to increase their opportunity for extra-pair copulations with the mates of yearling males (Barg et al. 2006, Boves 2011). Further study is needed to address age-specific fecundity in both sexes, as the age of the female could play an even greater role in reproductive success than the age of the male. Vegetation Both SY and ASY male Cerulean Warblers appear to be using similar habitats based on vegetation structure. This supports the conclusions of Sherry and Holmes (1989) who showed that there were no age-related habitat preferences among ageclasses in American Redstarts. Harrison and Green (2010) found no link between 62 vegetation characteristics and territory settlement patterns in the northern part of Brewer’s Sparrow’s (Spizella breweri) breeding range. Instead, birds settled in areas where the previous year’s reproductive success was high, indicating lower levels of predation. Similarly, vegetation characteristics appeared to have little or no effect on settlement patterns in Least Flycatchers Empidonax minimus (Tarof and Ratcliffe 2004), Kirtland’s Warblers (Morse 1989), and Lazuli Buntings Passerina amoena (Green et al. 1996). However, several studies contradict these findings (Hill 1988, Holmes et al. 1996), showing that physical habitat characteristics differ between age-classes. It is evident that the importance of habitat structure as it relates to age-specific settlement may vary among species. Settlement Patterns My results confirm earlier findings that the Cerulean Warbler is a “loosely colonial” species, likely using the presence of conspecifics as habitat-specific cues (Roth and Islam 2007). I found that both ASY and SY males located their territories significantly closer to ASY males than they did to other SY males. These findings suggest that Cerulean Warblers are relying on the experience of older males when settling on the landscape. Males may be attracted to conspecifics because of the greater likelihood of females’ settling in places with higher densities of territorial males (Collias and Collias 1969, Wagner 1997, Stamps 1991). A dense concentration of females could cause latearriving SY males to settle near mature adults for access to mates. Another possibility is that the presence of experienced individuals could be an indicator of high quality habitat 63 (Ward and Schlossberg 2004). In many species, individuals are more likely to return to a particular site if they have reproduced successfully there the previous year (Bollinger & Gavin 1989, Haas 1998). Therefore, settling closer to site-faithful males may be the best way to seek out high quality habitat (Alatalo et al. 1982, Stamps 1991, Ward and Schlossberg 2004). A number of published studies provide similar evidence. Muller et al. (1997) found that newly breeding House Wrens (Troglodytes aedon) select nest boxes that are significantly closer to established adult male territories. A study on Black-throated Blue Warblers found that males were four times more likely to settle in areas where playback was used than in control areas (Betts et al. 2008). Additionally, they tested the settlement response of both males and females to broadcasted vocalizations across a gradient of vegetation types and found that neither shrub cover nor tree density predicted warbler occurrence and that location cues trumped the use of vegetation cues. Most interesting is their documentation of the apparent use of public information to aide in settlement decisions the following spring. Beecher et al. (2007) proposed that successful individuals sing more frequently later in the breeding season to influence song development in offspring and thus provide valuable information to onlookers. Similarly, habitats devoid of singing males late in the season may infer poor habitat quality to prospecting conspecifics (Betts et al. 2008). The smaller size of clustered territories was probably due to density-dependent factors such as resource competition. A study of Dendroica warblers in spruce forests of 64 Maine found that species with dense populations had smaller territory sizes and vice versa (Morse 1976). One of the many benefits of settling in a populated area is that birds have to expend less energy when defending a smaller territory perimeter. Territory sizes of SY males were significantly larger than those of ASY males. Assuming ASY males occupy territories with greater food availability, this finding is consistent with a study that analyzed the stomach contents of ovenbirds and showed that territory size was inversely proportional to the amount of food available (Stenger 1958). Intuitively, the most dominant birds would occupy the highest quality habitat and therefore, obtain equitable benefits from defending a smaller territory. However, several studies have found either no difference in territory size or ASY territories to be larger than SY territories (Benham et al. 2008, Ralph and Pearson 1971). More research is needed to confirm if there are noticeable differences in territory size between the age-classes in this species. There were no differences in distance to important habitat features between the age-classes and clustered and non-clustered areas. If ASY males settled significantly closer to streams, a habitat feature shown to be important to this species (see Chapter 1), one could make a solid case for physical habitat differences between age-classes. However, similar settlement proximity to habitat features negates this explanation and further supports social explanations for settlement. Conclusion and Management Implications 65 I have provided statistical evidence that both SY and ASY Cerulean Warbler males are attracted to and settle closer to ASY males than other SY males. Social cues appear to be playing an important role in settlement, a behavior that could have significant conservation implications (Smith and Peacock 1990), and should be considered when managing for this species. Kress (1997) first showed that seabird restoration on uninhabited islands was possible when models, mirrors, and playbacks were used to create the impression of a successful colony. Later, studies showed that social attraction was possible for territorial species and that it was worth pursuing the use of playback and models to influence settlement for theoretical and management reasons (Mills et al. 2006, Alatalo et al. 1982, Muller et al. 1997). It is possible that vulnerable songbirds like the Cerulean Warbler could be coaxed into settling on protected lands where predators and brood parasites can be controlled and breeding success may be higher. Ward and Schlossberg (2004) used playbacks to attract Black-capped Vireos (Vireo atricapilla) to experimental plots where they were able to control Brown-headed Cowbird (Molothrus ater) populations and thus increase the success rate relative to a nearby unmanipulated population. Moreover, they showed that birds returned to experimental plots the following year without the use of playback. With that said, intraspecific sociality is rendered useless if adequate habitat is limited or unavailable. Attention should be given to the characteristics of ASY male territories, especially in areas that are re-occupied annually. Forests should be manipulated in ways to promote those characteristics on the landscape. For example, 66 knowing that this population of ASY males most frequently settled on southeasterly facing slopes, timber harvesting in these areas should be avoided where Cerulean Warbler densities are high. Just as birds use public information to make informed settlement decisions, researchers should observe bird behavior to help identify highly productive areas. Likewise, logging should be avoided directly in those habitats. The relatively high proportion of SY males and low overall fecundity of Cerulean Warblers in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests leads me to believe these management areas are serving as habitat for a sink population. If yearlings are more prevalent in modified and managed forests, the smaller number of more experienced individuals may affect breeding productivity in such habitats (Holmes et al. 1992). However, the importance of these forests should not be undermined. Less-preferred areas provide refuge for less-competitive individuals and create buffer areas that can act to mitigate population losses in source habitats (Bernstein et al. 1991). A myriad of questions are yet to be asked regarding Cerulean Warbler age-class structure and reproductive output. Future research should further investigate age-class pairing success, where pairing occurs on the landscape and what Cerulean Warbler density looks like where pairing is greatest. Also, frequency of site occupancy should be monitored to determine whether ASY male territories are being used more frequently than SY male territories. Additional reproductive and demographic data for Cerulean Warblers in a variety of landscapes are needed to test the validity of the results presented here. Continual monitoring of demographic dynamics in all areas of this 67 species’ range will be critical in deciding where and how to implement cost-effective and feasible conservation strategies in the future. With this knowledge, appropriate management decisions can be made to ensure that this denizen of the deciduous forest has a place to return to year after year. 68 Literature Cited Ahlering, M. A., and J. Faaborg. 2006. Avian habitat management meets conspecific attraction: If you build it, will they come? Auk 123:301-312. Alatalo, R. V., A. Lundberg, and M. Bjorklund. 1982. Can the song of male birds attract other males? An experiment with the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca. Bird Behaviour 4:42-45. Bakermans, M. H. and A. D. Rodewald. 2009. Think globally, manage locally: The importance of steady-state forest features for a declining songbird. Forest Ecology and Management 258:224-232. Barg, J. J., J. Jones, M. K. Girvan, and R. J. Robertson. 2006. Within-pair interactions and parental behavior of Cerulean Warblers breeding in eastern Ontario. Wilson Journal of Ornithology 118:316–325. Barg, J. J., J. Jones and P. B. Hamel. 2007. Cerulean Warbler (Denroica cerulea) breeding guide and nest searching tips (draft). Bayne, E. M. and K. A. Hobson. 2001. Effects of habitat fragmentation on pairing success of Ovenbirds: importance of male age and floater behavior. Auk 118:380-388. Beecher, M. D., J. M. Burt, A. L. O’Loghlen, C. N. Templeton, and S. E. Campbell. 2007. Bird song learning in an eavesdropping context. Animal Behavior 73:929-935. Benham, P. M., M. T. Hallworth, and L. R. Reitsma. 2008. Does age influence territory size, habitat selection, and reproductive success of male Canada Warblers in Central New Hampshire? The Wilson Journal of Ornithology 120.3: 446+. Bernstein, C., J. R. Krebs, and A. Kacelnik. 1991. Distribution of birds amongst habitats: theory and relevance to conservation. Bird Population Studies: Relevance to Conservation and Management (eds C. M. Perrins, J. –D. Lebreton and G. J. M. Hirons), pp. 317-345. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Betts, M. G., A. S. Hadley, N. Rodenhouse, and J. J. Nocera. 2008. Social information trumps vegetation structure in breeding-site selection by a migrant songbird. Proceedings of the Royal Society 275:2257-2263. Birdlife International. 2004. Threatened Birds of the World. CD-ROM. Birdlife International, Cambridge, UK. 69 Bollinger, E. K., and T. A. Gavin. 1989. The effects of site quality on breeding-site fidelity in Bobolinks. Auk 106:584–594. Boves, T. J. 2011. Multiple responses by Cerulean Warblers to experimental forest disturbance in the Appalachian Mountains. PhD dissertation, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN. Brown, J. L. 1969. Territorial behavior and population regulation in birds: a review and re-evaluation. Wilson Bulletin 81:293-329. Buehler, D. A., J. J. Giocomo, J. Jones, P. B. Hamel, C. M. Rogers, T. A. Beachy, D. W. Varble, C .P. Nicholson, K. L. Roth., J. Barg, R. J. Robertson, J. R. Robb, and K. Islam. 2008. Cerulean Warbler Reproduction, Survival, and Models of Population Decline. Journal of Wildlife Management 72:646-653. Burgoyne, G. E., Jr., and L. A. Ryel. 1978. Kirtland’s Warbler numbers and colonies, 1977. Jack Pine Warbler 56:185-190. Collias, N. E., and E. C. Collias. 1969. Size of breeding colonies related to attraction of mates in a tropical passerine bird. Ecology 50:481-488. Clark, K. L., and R. J. Robertson. 1979. Spatial and temporal multi-species nesting aggregations in birds as anti-parasite and anti-predator defenses. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 5:359-371. Cornell, K. L., and T. M. Donovan. 2010. Scale-dependent mechanisms of habitat selection for a migratory passerine: an experimental approach. Auk 127:899 908. Danchin, E., Boulinier, T., and F. Helfstein. 2000. Colonies as byproducts of commodity selection. Behavioral Ecology 11:572-573. Danchin, E., and R. H. Wagner. 1997. The evolution of coloniality: the emergence of new perspectives. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 12:342-347. Dunn, J. and K. Garrett. 1997. A field guide to warblers of North America. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin Co. Falls, J. B. 1981. Mapping territories with playback: an accurate census method for songbirds. Studies in Avian Biology 6:86-91. Ficken, M. S., and R. W. Ficken. 1967. Age-specific differences in the breeding behavior and ecology of the American Redstart. Wilson Bulletin 79:188-199. 70 Forslund, P., and T. Pärt. 1995. Age and reproduction in birds: Hypotheses and tests. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 10:374-378. Fretwell, S. D., and H. L. Lucas, Jr. 1970. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. I. Theoretical development. Acta Biotheoretica 19:16–36. Graves, G. R. 1997. Geographic clines of age ratios of Black-throated Blue Warblers (Dendroica caerulescens). Ecology 78:2524-2531. Green, E., V. R. Muehter, and W. Davison. 1996. Lazuli Bunting (Passerina amoena). In The Birds of North America, no. 232 (A. Poole and F Gill, Eds.). Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, and American Ornithologists’ Union, Washington D.C. Haas, C. A. 1998. Effects of prior nesting success on site fidelity and breeding dispersal: an experimental approach. Auk 115:929–936. Hamel, P. B. 2000. Cerulean Warbler (Dendroica cerulea). The Birds of North America, No. 511 (A. Poole and F. Gill, eds.). The Birds of North America, Inc., Philadelphia, PA. Hamel, P. B., D. K. Dawson, and P. D. Keyser. 2004. How we can learn more about the Cerulean Warbler (Dendroica cerulea). Auk 121:7-14. Hamel, P. B., M. J. Welton, C. G. Smith, III, and R. P. Ford. 2009. Test of Partners in Flight effective detection distance for cerulean warbler. Pp. 328–333 in Rich, T. D., C. Arizmendi, D. W. Demarest, and C. Thompson (eds). Tundra to tropics: connecting birds, habitats, and people. Proceedings of the 4th international Partners in Flight conference 13-16 February 2008, McAllen, TX. Hamilton, W. D. 1971. Geometry of the selfish herd. Journal of Theoretical Biology 31:295-311. Hardwood Ecosystem Experiment. Online. http://www.fnr.purdue.edu/HEE/Overview/Overview.htm 71 Harrison, M. L., and D. J. Green. 2010. Vegetation influences patch occupancy but not settlement and dispersal decisions in a declining migratory songbird. Canadian Journal of Zoology 88:148-160. Herremens, M. 1993. Clustering of territories in the Wood Warbler Phylloscopus sibilatrix. Bird Study 40:2-23. Hill, G. E. 1988. Age, plumage brightness, territory quality, and reproductive success in the Black-headed Grosbeak. Condor 2:379-388. Holmes, R. T., T. W. Sherry, P. P. Marra, and K. E. Petit. 1992. Multiple brooding and productivity of a Neotropical migrant, the Black-throated Blue Warbler (Dendroica caerulescens) in an unfragmented temperate forest. Auk 109:321 333. Holmes, R. T., P.P. Marra, and T. W. Sherry. 1996. Habitat-specific demography of breeding Black-throated Blue Warblers (Dendroica caerulescens): Implications for population dynamics. Journal of Animal Ecology 65:183-195. Igor Dias, R., M. Kuhlmann, L. R. Lourenco, and R. H. Macedo. 2009. Territorial clustering in the Blue- black Grassquit: Reproductive strategy in response to habitat and food requirements? Condor 111:706-714. James, F. C. and H. H. Shugart. 1970. A Quantitative Method of Habitat Description. Audubon Field Notes 24:727-736. Johnson, M. D. 2007. Measuring habitat quality: a review. Condor 109: 489-504. Jones, J. 2001. Habitat selection studies in avian ecology: a critical review. Auk 118:557-562. Jones, J. and R. J. Robertson. 2001. Territory and nest-site selection of Cerulean Warblers in eastern Ontario. Auk 118:727-735. Jones, J., R. D. DeBruyn, J. J. Barg, and R. J. Robertson. 2001. Assessing the effects of natural disturbance on a Neotropical migrant songbird. Ecology 82:2628-2635. Jones, J., W. J. McLeish, and R. J. Robertson. 2000. Density influences census technique accuracy for Cerulean Warblers in eastern Ontario. Journal of Field Ornithology 71:46-56. 72 Jones, J., J. J. Barg, T. S. Sillett, M. L. Veit, and R. J. Robertson. 2004. Minimum estimates of survival and population growth for Cerulean Warblers (Dendroica cerulea) breeding in Ontario, Canada. Auk 121:15-24. Kaminski, K. J. 2010. Cerulean Warbler Initial Response to Silviculture Treatments in Southern Indiana. M.S. Thesis, Ball State University, Muncie, IN. Kiester, A. R., and M. Slatkin. 1974. A strategy of movement and resource utilization. Theoretical Population Biology 6:1-20. Krebs, C.J. 1989. Ecological Methodology. University of British Columbia, Harper and Row, New York, U.S.A. Kress, S. W. 1997. Using animal behavior for conservation: case studies in seabird restoration from the Maine coast, USA. Journal of the Yamashina Institute for Ornithology 29:1–26. Landmann, A., and C. Kollinsky. 1995. Age and plumage related territory differences in male Black Redstarts - the (non)-adaptive significance of delayed plumage maturation. Ethology Ecology and Evolution 7:147-167. LeBlanc, D. 2008. Statistics: concepts and applications for science. XanEdu Publishers, Michigan. Lozano, G. A., S. Perreault, and R. E. Lemon. 1996. Age, arrival date and reproductive success of male American Redstarts Setophaga ruticilla. Journal of Avian Biology 27:164-170. Lynch, J.M., 1981. Status of the cerulean warbler in the Roanoke River basin of North Carolina. Chat 45, 29-35. Mayfield, H. 1960. The Kirtland’s Warbler. Cranbrook Institute of Science, Bloomfield Hills, MI. Mayfield, H. 1975. Suggestions for calculating nesting success. Wilson Bulletin 73:255 261. Mills, A. M., J. D. Rising and D. A. Jackson. 2006. Conspecific attraction during establishment of Least Flycatcher clusters. Journal of Field Ornithology 77:34-38. Morris, M. M. J., and R. E. Lemon. 1988. Mate choice in American Redstarts: by territory quality? Canadian Journal of Zoology 66:2255-2261. 73 Morse, D. H. 1976. Variables affecting the density and territory size of breeding spruce woods warblers. Ecology 57:290-301. Morse, D. H. 1989. American Warblers. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Morton, E. S. and K. C. Derrickson. 1990. The biological significance of age-specific return schedules in breeding purple martins. Condor 92: 1040-1050. Muller, K. L., J. A. Stamps, V. V. Krishnan, and N. H. Willits. 1997. The effects of conspecific attraction and habitat quality on habitat selection in territorial birds (Troglodytes aedon). American Naturalist 150:650-661. Newton, I. 1988. Age and reproduction in the sparrowhawk. – In: Clutton-Brock, T. (ed.) Reproductive Success. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 201-219. Nolan, V., Jr. 1978. The ecology and behavior of the Prairie Warbler Dendroica discolor. Ornithological Monographs, no. 26. Ralph, C. J., and C. A. Pearson. 1971. Correlation of Age, Size of Territory, Plumage, and Breeding Success in White-Crowned Sparrows. Condor 73:77-80. Rogers, C. M. 2006. Nesting success and breeding biology of Cerulean Warblers in Michigan. Wilson Journal of Ornithology 118:145-151. Roth, K. L. and K. Islam. 2008. Habitat Selection and Reproductive Success of Cerulean Warblers in Indiana. Wilson Journal of Ornithology 120:105-110. Roth, K. L., and K. Islam. 2007. Do Cerulean Warblers (Dendroica cerulea) exhibit clustered territoriality? American Midland-Naturalist 157:345-355. Sæther, B.-E. 1990. Age-specific variation in reproductive performance of birds. Current Ornithology 7:251-283. Sauer, J. R., J. E. Hines and J. Fallon. [online]. 2008. The North American Breeding Bird Survey,Results and Analysis 1966-2007 version 5.15,2008. USGS Patuxent Wildlife Research Center, Laurel, Maryland, USA. Sharma, S. 1996. Applied Multivariate Techniques. John Wile and Sons, Inc., New York. Sherry, T. W., and R. T. Holmes. 1989. Age-specific social dominance affects habitat use by breeding American Redstarts (Setophaga ruticilla): a removal experiment. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 25:327-333. 74 Smith, A. T., and M. M. Peacock. 1990. Conspecific attraction and the determination of metapopulation colonization rates. Conservation Biology 4:320-323. Stamps, J. A. 1987. Conspecifics as cues to territory quality: A preference of juvenile lizards (Anolis aeneus) for previously used territories. American Naturalist 129:629-642. Stamps, J. A. 1991. The effects of conspecifics on habitat selection in territorial species. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 28:29–36. Stamps, J. A., and V. V. Krishnan. 2005. Nonintuitive cue use in habitat selection. Ecology 86:2860-2867. Stenger, J. 1958. Food habits and available food of Ovenbirds in relation to territory size. Auk 75:335-346. Tarof, S. A. and L. M. Ratcliffe. 2004. Habitat characteristics and nest predation do not explain clustered breeding in Least Flycatchers (Empidonax minimus). Auk 121:877-893. Thompson, F. R., J. R. Probst, and M. G. Raphael. 1993. Silvicultural options for Neotropical migratory birds. In: D. M. Finch, P. W. Stangel, (eds.). Status and management of neotropical migratory birds: September 21-25, 1992, Estes Park, Colorado. General Technical Report RM 229. Fort Collins, CO.: Rocky Mountain Forest and Range Experiment Station, U.S. Dept. of Agriculture, Forest Service:353-362. Wagner, R. H. 1997. Hidden leks: sexual selection and the clustering of avian territories. Ornithological Monographs 49:123-146. Ward, M., and S. Schlossberg. 2004. Conspecific attraction and the conservation of territorial songbirds. Conservation Biology 18:519-525. Wooller, R. D., and J. C. Coulson. 1977. Factors affecting the age of first breeding of the kittiwake, Rissa tridactyla. Ibis 119:339-349. 75 ESRI 2011. ArcGIS Desktop: Release 10. Redlands, CA: Environmental Systems Research Institute. Figure 1. A sampling distribution of z-scores derived from the nearest neighbor tool in ArcMap (ArcGIS 10) that is calculated by measuring the distances between the centroids of each territory and comparing them to the expected values if the centroids were randomly dispersed throughout the site (Krebs 1989). 76 Cerulean Warbler Male Age-class Structure in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe State Forests 4.95% 29.70% 65.35% ASY SY UNK Figure 2. Proportions of second year (SY), after second year (ASY), and unknown-age male Cerulean Warblers recorded in the summer of 2011 in Morgan-Monroe and Yellowwood state forests in Southern Indiana. 77 Figure 3. Differences in mating success between successful and unsuccessful second year (SY) and successful and unsuccessful after second year (ASY) male Cerulean Warblers in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests, IN in the summer of 2011. SY males make up nearly half of all successful territories. 78 Age-specific Territory Size Log Territory Area (ha) 0.0 -0.5 -1.0 -1.5 -2.0 -2.5 Log(SY) Log(ASY) Figure 4. Differences in territory size (log transformed) between second year (SY) and after second year (ASY) male Cerulean Warblers in Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests, IN in the summer of 2011. SY male territories are significantly larger than ASY male territories. 79 SY Settlement To Both Age-Classes Distance to nearest territory (m) 1000 800 600 400 200 0 SY-SY SY-ASY Figure 5. Second year (SY) male territory centroid distances to nearest territory centroid of each age-class at Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests, IN in the summer of 2011. SY males settle significantly closer to ASY males than they do to other SY males. 80 ASY Settlement To Both Age-Classes Distance to nearest territory (m) 1000 800 600 400 200 0 ASY-SY ASY-ASY Figure 6. After second year (ASY) male territory centroid distances to nearest territory centroid of both age-classes at Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests, IN in the summer of 2011. ASY males settle significantly closer to other ASY males than they do to SY males. 81 Table 1. Nearest neighbor analysis values for Cerulean Warbler (Setophaga cerulea) territories in 2011 at Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. The area of each management unit (1.96 km2) was specified for all cluster analyses. Management Unit Value z p Status n Mean distance SE Unit 2 1.26 0.69 0.4902 Random 2 593.77 120.68 Unit 4 Unit 5 0.61 0.54 -2.35 -3.04 0.0186 0.0023 Clustered Clustered 10 12 129.29 104.4 82.28 88.74 Unit 3 0.86 -0.93 0.3514 Random 13 160.49 25.07 Unit 6 Unit 9 0.45 0.49 -3.91 -3.54 0.0001 0.0004 Clustered Clustered 14 13 81.18 90.41 97.63 95.15 Unit 1 NA NA NA NA 0 NA NA Unit 7 0.88 -0.55 0.5828 Random 6 241.13 32.01 Unit 8 0.75 -2.67 0.0075 Clustered 31 90.02 30.15 82 Table 2. Spatial attributes of second year (SY) and after second year (ASY) male Cerulean Warbler territories in 2011 at Yellowwood and Morgan-Monroe state forests in Southern Indiana. Results of 2-sample t-tests that were used to compare all attributes among age-classes are reported below. Highlighted cells show p-values considered significant after Bonferroni adjustments were made. Attribute Distance to stream Distance to road Distance to nearest neighbor Distance to harvest Slope Aspect Elevation Conclusion NS NS S NS NS S NS SY (n) 30 30 30 30 30 30 30 ASY (n) SY (median) ASY (median) 66 135 155 66 81.94 78.93 66 103 74.8 66 341 277 66 34.39 36.51 66 90.46 116.5 66 742.4 757.9 P 0.545 0.701 0.03 0.53 0.384 0.013 0.369 SY (n) = number of second year birds. ASY (n) = number of after second year birds. Median SY = median distance of second year male territories to attribute in m, with the exception of slope and aspect (degrees). Median ASY = median distance of after second year male territories to attribute in m, with the exception of slope and aspect (degrees). NS = not significant. S = significant. 83 Appendix I: UTM coordinates for Cerulean Warbler territories demarcated in the summers of 2010 and 2011. East. = Easting and North. = Northing. * = Birds with less than 5 trees that were not included in analyses. 2010 UNIT 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 NAME PAN PAN PAN PAN PAN DON* DON* DON* DON* KOZ KOZ KOZ KOZ KOZ SUN* SUN* SUN* SUN* BUD BUD BUD BUD BUD NIK* NIK* NIK* NIK* RED RED RED RED RED RED East. 548918 548910 548958 548913 548865 547936 547944 547885 547825 547093 547129 547091 547183 547154 547920 547856 547841 547843 548078 548039 548023 548047 548038 548301 548320 548341 548333 548369 548384 548361 548372 548390 548341 North. 4354674 4354671 4354622 4354585 4354692 4355241 4355185 4355194 4355187 4352109 4352046 4352057 4352021 4352073 4352159 4352141 4352187 4352165 4352062 4352055 4352071 4352059 4352071 4352065 4352075 4352096 4352097 4352112 4352103 4352158 4352165 4352162 4352099 2011 UNIT 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 NAME JDAN JDAN JDAN JDAN JDAN ERIC ERIC ERIC ERIC ERIC ERIC ERIC CLAY CLAY CLAY CLAY CLAY CLAY ZORO ZORO ZORO ZORO ZORO LUIS LUIS LUIS LUIS LUIS LUIS LUIS LUIS LUIS LUIS East. 548270 548294 548328 548302 548263 547826 547943 547906 547861 547809 547923 547782 548000 547993 547963 547989 548022 548025 547886 547842 547864 547871 547866 547200 547215 547163 547152 547211 547211 547181 547294 547282 547108 North. 4354888 4354896 4354856 4354867 4354903 4355359 4355251 4355245 4355252 4355261 4355219 4355331 4352179 4352170 4352162 4352152 4352162 4352129 4352532 4352517 4352529 4352541 4352591 4352017 4352029 4352061 4352051 4352045 4352007 4352046 4352104 4352073 4352088 84 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 BRO BRO BRO BRO BRO TAZ TAZ TAZ TAZ TAZ JAH JAH JAH JAH JAH FRO FRO FRO FRO FRO FRO SAX SAX SAX SAX SAX ACE ACE ACE ACE ACE FLY FLY FLY FLY FLY LUV LUV LUV LUV LUV LUV 548373 548381 548412 548414 548396 548015 548022 548080 547998 547999 548433 548420 548396 548403 548423 548393 548380 548377 548357 548360 548356 548307 548314 548342 548331 548347 548279 548276 548304 548244 548257 547796 547820 547801 547759 547743 548392 548356 548345 548420 548405 548369 4352107 4352081 4352079 4352066 4352093 4352133 4352084 4352136 4352146 4352119 4351974 4351928 4351936 4351968 4351927 4351963 4351963 4351972 4351952 4351964 4351958 4352378 4352354 4352358 4352351 4352330 4352366 4352384 4352351 4352409 4352338 4352430 4352410 4352377 4352386 4352353 4352280 4352275 4352275 4352267 4352322 4352323 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 ROKO ROKO ROKO ROKO ROKO PACO PACO PACO PACO PACO PACO ZEEK ZEEK ZEEK ZEEK ZEEK ZEEK ZEEK NEPI NEPI NEPI NEPI NEPI NEPI BIRD BIRD BIRD BIRD BIRD BIRD LARY LARY LARY LARY LARY LARY NICK NICK NICK NICK NICK NICK 547975 547985 548003 547999 547968 547903 547904 547940 547936 547913 547923 548372 548369 548367 548386 548402 548401 548405 548301 548300 548312 548340 548309 548335 547795 547797 547799 547776 547827 547801 547991 547983 547952 548001 548003 547980 548344 548342 548332 548338 548340 548344 4352401 4352386 4352389 4352420 4352378 4352279 4352294 4352323 4352314 4352270 4352375 4351964 4351988 4351993 4352001 4352011 4352006 4352038 4351979 4351986 4352002 4351991 4352019 4352002 4352535 4352512 4352523 4352556 4352454 4352490 4352725 4352735 4352750 4352734 4352773 4352675 4352082 4352091 4352094 4352051 4352058 4352052 85 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 KAM KAM KAM KAM KAM PAT PAT PAT PAT PAT KIM KIM KIM KIM KIM GUS GUS GUS GUS GUS GUS ROY ROY ROY ROY ROY ROY MOE MOE MOE MOE MOE JET JET JET JET JET TAO TAO TAO TAO TAO 547711 547705 547752 547705 547738 548353 548321 548348 548302 548332 548359 548379 548367 548391 548387 554646 554593 554653 554660 554633 554612 553910 553889 553873 553891 553927 553895 555230 555229 555300 555206 555209 554923 554906 554857 554867 554880 554671 554688 554613 554634 554710 4352392 4352404 4352458 4352397 4352393 4352228 4352257 4352268 4352256 4352302 4352227 4352218 4352208 4352278 4352211 4339292 4339393 4339282 4339330 4339382 4339342 4339950 4339935 4340022 4340071 4340018 4340020 4330420 4330407 4330393 4330406 4330412 4330632 4330626 4330619 4330594 4330636 4330771 4330782 4330811 4330797 4330774 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 NICK AMOS AMOS AMOS AMOS AMOS FRIZ FRIZ FRIZ FRIZ FRIZ JEFF JEFF JEFF JEFF JEFF JEFF BART BART BART BART BART BART BART ZEUS ZEUS ZEUS ZEUS ZEUS ZEUS THOR THOR THOR THOR THOR THOR THOR ARUN ARUN ARUN ARUN ARUN 548320 547969 547994 547957 547950 547969 548093 548070 548058 548088 548086 549136 549151 549151 549156 549154 549141 549153 549136 549164 549155 549121 549133 549118 550270 550238 550236 550223 550235 550228 550144 550095 550088 550127 550111 550158 550082 549047 549091 549160 549162 549126 4352063 4351765 4351723 4351698 4351673 4351678 4352046 4352071 4352079 4352098 4352067 4350665 4350655 4350691 4350625 4350664 4350677 4350703 4350722 4350725 4350732 4350724 4350726 4350719 4351215 4351271 4351219 4351256 4351205 4351212 4351201 4351271 4351282 4351346 4351337 4351309 4351221 4350909 4350933 4350876 4350809 4350845 86 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 TAO TAO SAM SAM SAM SAM SAM BLU BLU BLU BLU BLU BLU BAM BAM BAM BAM BAM BAM ZIP ZIP ZIP ZIP ZIP SKY SKY SKY SKY SKY SKY SKY SKY BOB BOB BOB BOB BOB BOB TIM TIM TIM TIM 554707 554696 555223 555157 555158 555203 555204 555026 555057 554991 555020 555048 555095 558618 558626 558639 558620 558601 558601 558268 558258 558235 558223 558232 558559 558559 558583 558576 558586 558564 558528 558542 558293 558285 558276 558255 558227 558278 558399 558403 558375 558391 4330741 4330734 4330780 4330800 4330816 4330786 4330835 4330609 4330673 4330702 4330716 4330749 4330753 4331826 4331842 4331882 4331863 4331846 4331835 4331321 4331346 4331328 4331336 4331265 4331648 4331673 4331679 4331673 4331648 4331641 4331643 4331662 4329264 4329241 4329228 4329212 4329227 4329213 4329349 4329324 4329262 4329257 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 ARUN MARS MARS MARS MARS MARS HALF HALF HALF HALF HALF NEWB NEWB NEWB NEWB NEWB NEWB NEWB STAN STAN STAN STAN STAN HANK HANK HANK HANK HANK HUGH HUGH HUGH HUGH HUGH JAZZ JAZZ JAZZ JAZZ JAZZ JAZZ NORM NORM NORM 549130 550241 550226 550222 550201 550243 550355 550344 550347 550390 550343 550136 550068 550079 550022 550028 550084 550162 550218 550288 550356 550336 550266 550280 550204 550242 550281 550269 553992 553986 553992 553983 553983 554008 554015 554052 554054 554060 554100 554175 554156 554148 4350834 4351171 4351120 4351158 4351165 4351158 4351224 4351175 4351189 4351260 4351267 4350724 4350705 4350701 4350724 4350710 4350761 4350704 4350423 4350458 4350528 4350592 4350479 4350343 4350370 4350383 4350370 4350354 4338971 4338963 4338958 4338962 4338973 4339007 4339026 4339006 4339016 4339027 4339024 4339999 4340036 4340016 87 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 TIM TIM TIM TED TED TED TED TED TED WES WES WES WES WES JOE JOE JOE JOE JOE JOE BIL BIL BIL BIL BIL SLY SLY SLY SLY SLY BEN BEN BEN BEN BEN JIM JIM JIM JIM JIM JIM ABE 558359 558370 558390 559195 559197 559192 559122 559147 559170 558932 558942 558952 558959 558973 559145 559155 559164 559251 559240 559251 559149 559145 559162 559111 559160 558358 558344 558346 558346 558349 558312 558346 558322 558289 558292 558924 558968 558968 558984 558981 558974 558560 4329257 4329308 4329310 4329648 4329642 4329633 4329659 4329702 4329677 4329637 4329629 4329625 4329604 4392614 4329661 4329671 4329648 4329603 4329544 4329577 4329660 4329681 4329691 4329735 4329752 4329356 4329354 4329348 4329364 4329368 4329259 4329297 4329305 4329281 4329255 4329669 4329666 4329642 4329640 4329626 4329617 4329457 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 NORM NORM MORT MORT MORT MORT MORT TIKI TIKI TIKI TIKI TIKI SMIT SMIT SMIT SMIT SMIT SMIT SMIT YORK YORK YORK YORK YORK YORK HAWK HAWK HAWK HAWK HAWK HAWK HAWK THUG THUG THUG THUG THUG BONO BONO BONO BONO BONO 554166 554180 554167 554175 554180 554181 554199 554665 554665 554683 554643 554634 554716 554712 554691 554685 554704 554735 554742 554823 554838 554818 554821 554817 554829 554836 554830 554815 554867 554863 554906 554775 555070 555138 555053 555074 555099 554717 554715 554751 554754 554745 4339967 4339953 4340072 4340072 4340063 4340060 4340071 4339416 4339429 4339388 4339398 4339408 4339282 4339279 4339253 4339273 4339325 4339323 4339310 4339677 4339686 4339680 4339674 4339699 4339679 4339729 4339736 4339730 4339706 4339733 4339731 4339713 4339403 4339251 4339268 4339307 4339269 4339227 4339188 4339158 4339189 4339186 88 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 ABE ABE ABE ABE MRT MRT MRT MRT MRT MRT MRT MRT MRT MRT VOZ VOZ VOZ VOZ VOZ VOZ VOZ VOZ PAC PAC PAC PAC PAC MAN MAN MAN MAN MAN BUB BUB BUB BUB BUB STU STU STU STU STU 558575 558600 558565 558576 558721 558759 558744 558741 558754 558750 558853 558861 558851 558848 558311 558316 558319 558271 558261 558300 558309 558378 558105 558094 558104 558103 558108 558081 558078 558066 558075 558061 558124 558149 558142 558147 558144 558147 558153 558173 558171 558152 4329438 4329435 4329422 4329431 4329440 4329381 4329373 4329362 4329364 4329431 4329320 4329327 4329338 4329220 4329531 4329472 4329442 4329450 4329463 4329451 4329376 4329438 4329558 4329550 4329362 4329536 4329559 4329509 4329524 4329523 4329536 4329544 4329553 4329553 4329564 4329557 4329569 4329504 4329512 4329495 4329482 4329522 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 LARS LARS LARS LARS LARS NERO NERO NERO NERO NERO NERO NERO DOUG DOUG DOUG DOUG DOUG DOUG SHAQ SHAQ SHAQ SHAQ SHAQ SHAQ SHAQ SHAQ TOVE TOVE TOVE TOVE TOVE TOVE GRIZ GRIZ GRIZ GRIZ GRIZ BEAR BEAR BEAR BEAR BEAR 554173 554139 554153 554121 554138 554671 554659 554675 554681 554693 554644 554668 554581 554569 554590 554595 554568 554585 554744 554740 554709 554719 554735 554784 554815 554714 554551 554545 554546 554558 554545 554532 554839 554861 554851 554855 554791 554332 554323 554323 554340 554335 4338829 4338810 4338793 4338866 4338869 4339319 4339293 4339331 4339287 4339320 4339317 4339362 4330679 4330659 4330655 4330656 4330663 4330659 4330461 4330492 4330494 4330483 4330366 4330342 4330360 4330492 4330670 4330660 4330651 4330641 4330641 4330639 4330619 4330653 4330657 4330667 4330622 4330821 4330799 4330806 4330788 4330781 89 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 TOM* TOM* TOM* TOM* NED NED NED NED NED PAW PAW PAW PAW PAW DAN DAN DAN DAN DAN RON RON RON RON RON TEK TEK TEK TEK TEK BAL* BAL* BAL* JIB JIB JIB JIB JIB MAX* MAX* MAX* MAX* JER 560684 560678 560635 560627 560606 560578 560558 560564 560622 560831 560847 560867 560850 560830 560787 560780 560805 560828 560840 560879 560850 560862 560840 560835 560725 560743 560766 560778 560750 560926 560919 560926 560814 560809 560791 560771 560784 560594 560588 560588 560625 560597 4332583 4332609 4332610 4332616 4332312 4332272 4332293 4332367 4332346 4332401 4332411 4332385 4332402 4332381 4332622 4332637 4332568 4332570 4332580 4332733 4332779 4332747 4332745 4332700 4332510 4332522 4332543 4332533 4332563 4332459 4332449 4332468 4332691 4332703 4332712 4332692 4332704 4332912 4332899 4332859 4332922 4332743 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 BEAR RICK RICK RICK RICK RICK RICK KING KING KING KING KING FATS FATS FATS FATS FATS FATS WADE WADE WADE WADE WADE WADE WADE FRED FRED FRED FRED FRED BRET BRET BRET BRET BRET BRET MOON MOON MOON MOON MOON SEAN 554351 554911 554891 554919 554995 554891 554932 555039 555040 555094 555103 555060 554542 554516 554512 554485 554485 554509 554653 554661 554664 554653 554662 554656 554665 554509 554508 554497 554499 5554500 554584 554562 554560 554567 554568 554575 554509 554509 554512 554520 554531 554613 4330790 4330598 4330601 4330648 4330624 4330613 4330638 4330614 4330596 4330627 4330657 4330577 4330893 4330775 4330824 4330819 4330784 4330759 4330495 4330499 4330480 4330459 4330447 4330470 4330489 4330651 4330652 4330650 4330640 4330645 4330575 4330573 4330556 4330540 4330544 4330580 4330601 4330664 4330670 4330650 4330674 4330595 90 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 JER JER JER JER JER JER ASH* ASH* ASH* PIP PIP PIP PIP PIP PIP PIP PIP YEN YEN YEN YEN YEN DRU* DRU* DRU* DRU* 560611 560557 560577 560606 560512 560582 560532 560579 560544 560742 560758 560720 560713 560741 560749 560758 560774 560696 560698 560751 560684 560675 560746 560735 560732 560739 4332718 4332744 4332826 4332786 4332727 4332776 4332888 4332905 4332916 4332557 4332525 4332560 4332562 4332589 4332587 4332597 4332586 4332829 4332872 4332851 4332834 4332830 4332616 4332621 4332640 4332635 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 SEAN SEAN SEAN SEAN SEAN MARK MARK MARK MARK MARK MARK ZIGI ZIGI ZIGI ZIGI ZIGI ZIGI OZZY OZZY OZZY OZZY OZZY ZAGI ZAGI ZAGI ZAGI ZAGI ALEX ALEX ALEX ALEX ALEX ALEX ALEX LEON LEON LEON LEON LEON LEON LEON LEON 554605 554592 554575 554556 554581 554615 554625 554635 554630 554641 554615 558763 558747 558731 558708 558741 558760 558807 558792 558798 558776 558821 558787 558795 558804 558836 558842 558727 558705 558694 558748 558738 558700 558713 558593 558608 558642 558566 558630 558607 558623 558608 4330588 4330586 4330601 4330669 4330616 4330542 4330555 4330586 4330570 4330537 4330515 4331243 4331317 4331279 4331322 4331326 4331380 4331424 4331350 4331349 4331422 4331470 4331231 4331271 4331232 4331173 4331171 4331675 4331688 4331639 4331671 4331665 4331660 4331700 4332008 4331983 4332058 4332004 4331938 4331960 4331872 4331888 91 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 LEON ZITO ZITO ZITO ZITO ZITO ZITO HANS HANS HANS HANS HANS CHAD CHAD CHAD CHAD CHAD KURT KURT KURT KURT KURT SLIM SLIM SLIM SLIM SLIM HICK HICK HICK HICK HICK RIFF RIFF RIFF RIFF RIFF RIFF RIFF RIFF RIFF JAKE 558617 558197 558200 558243 558261 558218 558205 558940 558947 558943 558946 558953 558790 558794 558801 558815 558806 558871 558900 558876 558860 558843 558633 558623 558643 558644 558632 558605 558592 558579 558565 558574 558643 558664 558698 558682 558650 558657 558633 558628 558632 558584 4331798 4331193 4331202 4331266 4331307 4331187 4331080 4329517 4329510 4329530 4329496 4329527 4329427 4329424 4329417 4329442 4329439 4329554 4329564 4329548 4329565 4329547 4329127 4329084 4329089 4329081 4329079 4329096 4329057 4329087 4329092 4329084 4329139 4329208 4329178 4329141 4329087 4329080 4329090 4329101 4329111 4329357 92 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 JAKE JAKE JAKE JAKE FRAN FRAN FRAN FRAN FRAN FRAN FRAN FRAN FRAN FRAN FRAN FRAN GREG GREG GREG GREG GREG MIKE MIKE MIKE MIKE MIKE MIKE MIKE MIKE CHAN CHAN CHAN CHAN CHAN CHAN CHAN CHAN REXX REXX REXX REXX REXX 558607 558615 558534 558551 559055 559054 559037 559057 559055 559054 559037 559057 559081 559082 559085 559046 559077 559062 559074 559074 559097 558133 558111 558133 558088 558107 558116 558120 558118 558131 558123 558110 558114 558090 558096 558064 558160 558166 558155 558176 558139 558169 4329314 4329355 4329320 4329331 4329635 4329640 4329654 4329649 4329635 4329640 4329654 4329649 4329655 4329633 4329620 4329605 4329639 4329653 4329673 4329662 4329727 4329277 4329245 4329250 4329250 4329262 4329226 4329183 4329280 4329139 4329147 4329148 4329135 4329138 4329162 4329105 4329114 4329186 4329161 4329189 4329220 4329195 93 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 REXX JACK JACK JACK JACK JACK JACK JUAN JUAN JUAN JUAN JUAN JUAN ERNY ERNY ERNY ERNY ERNY BERT BERT BERT BERT BERT TINY TINY TINY TINY TINY TINY TINY ANDY ANDY ANDY ANDY ANDY BABE BABE BABE BABE BABE BABE BABE 558121 558070 558061 558049 558066 558000 557997 558274 558323 558321 558320 558296 558302 558525 558563 558570 558563 558566 558466 558487 558528 558529 558470 558520 558550 558558 558537 558578 558594 558518 558543 558567 558565 558512 558591 557859 557838 557893 557860 557851 557868 557908 4329203 4329075 4329051 4329035 4329030 4329057 4329050 4329228 4329199 4329177 4329284 4329252 4329248 4329520 4329521 4329527 4329542 4329555 4329950 4330064 4330025 4330057 4329991 4329994 4329964 4330007 4329976 4329935 4329943 4329985 4329091 4329099 4329092 4329116 4329082 4329150 4329164 4329163 4329109 4329148 4329151 4329140 94 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 RALF RALF RALF RALF RALF RALF RALF RALF RALF STUD STUD STUD STUD STUD DAVE DAVE DAVE DAVE DAVE ZACK ZACK ZACK ZACK ZACK ZACK MELO MELO MELO MELO MELO MELO MELO TATE TATE TATE TATE TATE TATE TATE ALFE ALFE ALFE 557914 557914 557884 57892 557922 557927 557925 557880 557831 559072 559067 559036 559030 559023 558881 558831 558859 558830 558804 559092 559095 559104 559124 559082 559046 558935 558941 558950 558946 558925 558920 558936 558742 558747 558753 558729 558731 558622 558617 557841 557888 557923 4329053 4329047 4329089 4329087 4329175 4329053 4329046 4329123 4329094 4329932 4329931 4329941 4329936 4329995 4329651 4329578 4329657 4329604 4329610 4329810 4329805 4329783 4329788 4329832 4329854 4329665 4329658 4329669 4329662 4329700 4329676 4329638 4329418 4329415 4329407 4329414 4329389 4329454 4329458 4329346 4329302 4329343 95 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 ALFE ALFE CHIP CHIP CHIP CHIP CHIP MARV MARV MARV MARV MARV MARV MARV DUKE DUKE DUKE DUKE DUKE DUKE YUNG YUNG YUNG YUNG YUNG JOSH JOSH JOSH JOSH JOSH JOSH JOSH JOSH JOSH BILL BILL BILL BILL BILL BILL CAIN CAIN 557855 557865 557979 557950 557969 557927 557945 558413 558433 558437 558393 558455 558487 558472 557999 558022 558040 557996 557978 558023 558341 558366 558362 558369 558373 558833 558834 558795 558859 558858 558869 558874 558879 558915 560536 560526 560544 560546 560513 560500 560701 560692 4329356 4329292 4329133 4329162 4329147 4329149 4329161 4329371 4329394 4329424 4329387 4329379 4329360 4329321 4329075 4329013 4329012 4328995 4328973 4328980 4329353 4329365 4329379 4329410 4329438 4330083 4330065 4330052 4330106 4330086 4330098 4330143 4330163 4330124 4332781 4332779 4332799 4332805 4332820 4332818 432516 4332499 96 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 CAIN CAIN CAIN CAIN WALT WALT WALT WALT WALT MATT MATT MATT MATT MATT LUKE LUKE LUKE LUKE LUKE LUKE PETE PETE PETE PETE PETE PETE PAUL PAUL PAUL PAUL PAUL PAUL PAUL PAUL KENT KENT KENT KENT KENT KENT ABEL ABEL 560715 560677 560715 560697 560543 560551 560550 560530 560573 560777 560771 560785 560762 560801 560718 560708 560694 560720 560724 560712 561000 560994 560977 560950 560894 560873 560804 560749 560736 560735 560731 560725 560729 560766 560853 560791 560794 560804 560812 560835 560675 560707 4332516 4332507 4332519 4332531 4332632 4332638 4332620 4332648 4332654 4332709 4332701 4332721 4332746 4332774 4332613 4332607 4332611 4332644 4332659 4332636 4332172 4332186 4332144 4332198 4332122 4332148 4332672 4332679 4332694 4332674 4332672 4332671 4332675 4332699 4332683 4332683 4332687 4332670 4332681 4332666 4332484 4332493 97 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 9 ABEL ABEL ABEL ABEL ABEL JOHN JOHN JOHN JOHN JOHN RYAN RYAN RYAN RYAN RYAN RYAN RYAN ADAM ADAM ADAM ADAM ADAM ADAM ADAM ADAM JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE JOSE 560711 560712 560721 560737 560779 560792 560817 560845 560812 560788 560560 560576 560577 560602 560604 560614 560581 560725 560728 560730 560745 560719 560713 560740 560721 560650 560648 560663 560670 560657 560634 560654 560699 560709 560723 560735 560762 560637 560648 560663 560676 560693 4332498 4332518 4332521 4332538 4332466 4332571 4332594 4332599 4332651 4332637 4332782 4332781 4332787 4332791 4332808 4332740 4332785 4332521 4332540 4332541 4332552 4332555 4332541 4332566 4332570 4332473 4332452 4332466 4332484 4332464 4332482 4332470 4332380 4332373 4332374 4332365 4332436 4332398 4332402 4332420 4332452 4332452 98 Appendix II Protocol for Observing and Recording Male Age-class Objective: To determine if there is a significant difference between the numbers of successfully nesting adult males and first spring males. Ho: Adult males have no reproductive advantage over first spring males. Ha: Adult males have a significant reproductive advantage over first spring males. Methods: Throughout the territory demarcation process, the resident male should be viewed in binoculars as many times as possible to begin assessing its age-class. Once a minimum of five trees have been marked for any particular territory, researchers will navigate to the approximate middle of the territory and a playback recording of a male song will be played for one minute. The next two minutes will be spent listening and watching for the male, where photographs will be taken of the individual, if at all possible. If the bird does not approach within an adequate distance to make visual observations, another minute will be spent playing the song recording. The last minute will again be spent listening and watching for the male. To avoid creating a bias, ensure that playback song selection is consistent across all territories. Also, use caution not to overuse the playback, as it can increase aggressive interaction among neighboring birds 99 and complicate the age-class identification process. All behavioral and physical observations should be recorded, along with song rate in the territory data sheet. Males will be identified as either second year (SY) or after second year (ASY) by documenting a combination of physical characteristics. SY males will be identified by the presence of a white superciliary stripe, incomplete or faint breast band, and reduced streaking on the back. ASY males lack a prominent white supercilium, possess a full, dark breast band, and are azure blue above with prominent streaking (Dunn and Garrett 1997). It is important to note that mature adults may possess a small white remnant patch of the supercilium at the back of the eye. Therefore, in order to confidently claim that a male belongs to a particular age-class, a combination of at least two of these three physical characteristics must be confirmed. Birds allocated to a particular ageclass based on only one physical characteristic will be excluded from the data. To eliminate potential observer bias, all final decisions regarding male age-class must be made by the principal investigator. Below are pictures of first spring and mature adult males. Often times, physical characteristics can be nebulous or enter a “gray zone.” Investigators should use the images below as an aid in age-class identification in the field. 100 SY males are on the left and ASY males are on the right 101 102 103 APPENDIX III TERRITORY SIZES (HA) FOR ALL YEARS 2007 0.445265 0.020797 0.187022 0.039364 0.061989 0.21123 0.027715 0.204284 0.191496 0.103618 0.014701 0.132308 0.149518 0.287423 0.304575 0.058176 0.215181 0.177808 0.102048 0.019304 0.131958 0.264194 1.1827 0.04036 0.168313 0.176491 0.185058 0.035873 0.131264 0.508268 0.113206 0.037915 0.36023 0.045918 2008 0.835524 0.118704 0.190432 0.081238 0.35086 0.197855 0.165846 0.790938 0.820591 1.751274 1.668189 0.613087 0.044386 0.082035 0.339199 0.188786 0.213 0.206114 0.161497 0.126469 0.311869 0.303584 0.260849 0.239235 0.264297 0.038891 0.19712 0.32699 0.019045 0.369683 0.136684 0.129905 0.121125 0.433945 2009 0.064153 0.14154 0.197289 0.085385 0.351376 0.746663 0.432932 0.119464 0.053862 0.117407 0.069389 0.120301 0.381034 0.302855 0.044478 0.235291 0.054505 0.434414 0.067244 0.166851 0.584743 0.262545 0.811675 0.096735 0.145744 0.221811 0.261958 0.027356 0.118867 0.687774 0.057566 0.435347 0.150944 0.027549 2010 0.0645 0.1931 0.0556 0.00315 0.08355 0.1497 0.2588 0.6743 0.1592 0.06435 0.02 0.04355 0.14605 0.3324 0.0146 0.24415 0.0355 0.32005 0.11385 0.6079 0.13015 0.05255 0.14185 0.4409 0.13355 0.1172 0.3492 0.28785 0.0292 0.09115 0.0865 1.20765 0.3567 0.02385 2011 0.0921 0.0471 0.00675 0.05075 0.0744 0.0973 0.2676 0.2091 0.3223 0.7826 0.03325 0.1475 0.1465 0.23785 0.2997 0.08275 0.0705 0.08415 0.10495 0.1475 0.5882 0.2076 0.23055 0.0189 0.2006 0.02675 0.11335 0.1322 0.1749 0.154 0.6726 0.11235 0.21585 0.0734 104 0.062314 0.030109 0.086568 0.12928 0.052527 0.358803 0.093666 0.064119 0.018591 0.075999 0.151295 0.579369 0.124592 0.121603 0.0674 0.167097 0.105875 0.066704 0.096838 0.58575 0.055391 0.234375 0.598717 0.202585 0.035184 0.174209 0.108794 0.082003 0.385695 0.103135 0.179217 0.630813 1.003724 0.405993 0.478461 0.125858 0.480834 0.27 0.141954 0.448344 0.118858 0.368953 0.173099 0.184865 0.683803 0.148273 0.265699 0.279651 0.43802 0.543706 0.263139 0.402794 0.131834 0.111999 0.288592 0.215376 0.12459 0.147233 0.190521 0.561567 0.019706 0.154985 0.03406 0.123081 0.313104 0.077735 0.09402 0.039401 0.027303 0.522665 0.078543 0.11915 0.847187 0.265743 0.031689 0.117891 0.019864 0.031275 0.215459 0.043263 0.352908 0.07883 0.140095 0.805657 0.076727 0.051258 0.033789 0.18616 0.099133 0.021646 0.140318 0.030479 0.01845 0.43135 0.1828 0.0676 0.23135 0.21515 0.16305 0.42625 0.19855 0.0752 0.1406 0.0148 0.04345 0.1726 0.237 0.2715 0.2316 0.1117 0.227 0.0681 0.90925 0.0366 0.1613 0.20175 0.06165 0.08265 0.5335 0.0608 0.0662 0.06125 1.02245 0.0929 0.1626 0.09745 0.266 0.3413 0.1795 0.2319 0.08915 0.24675 0.08 0.1256 0.2607 0.36245 0.07545 0.0724 0.05265 0.18105 0.1513 0.276 0.2144 0.2385 0.43175 0.0754 0.0236 0.06205 1.0813 0.15065 0.0384 0.1432 0.0578 0.3923 0.4189 0.9205 105 0.048532 0.886691 0.1914 0.184 0.08805 0.9185 0.13625 0.19375 0.0108 0.5997 0.4619 0.7592 0.1403 0.12185 0.3421 0.08705 0.426 0.29055 0.0601 0.43685 0.63475 0.2474 0.2286 0.02375 0.2585 0.0774 0.1448 0.09795 0.1185