Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification Procedures:

advertisement
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification Procedures:
Follow-up on the Test of Sequential versus Simultaneous Procedures (Study One)
and
An Experimental Study of the Effect of Photo Arrays on
Evaluations of Evidentiary Strength by Key Criminal Justice Decision Makers (Study Two)
REPORT UNDER REVISION
Karen L. Amendola, Ph.D.
Maria D. Valdovinos, B.A.
Meghan G. Slipka, M.A.
Earl Hamilton, M.A.
Mary Sigler, B.A.
Adam Kaufman, B.A.
POLICE FOUNDATION
1201 Connecticut Avenue, N.W.
Suite 200
Washington, DC 20036
www.policefoundation.org
Chief James Bueermann (ret.)
President
February 25, 2014
This project includes a follow-up of the 2011 American Judicature Society’s Eyewitness ID Field Studies Project
and was supported by generous grants from the Laura and John Arnold Foundation, the Open Society Foundations,
and the former JEHT Foundation.
The findings and discussion herein represent those of the authors and not necessarily those of the funding sources.
Table of Contents
Table of Contents...................................................................................................... i
List of Tables and Figures ..................................................................................... iii
Acknowledgments...................................................................................................iv
Executive Summary................................................................................................ . 1
Introduction ........................................................................................................... 12
Laboratory versus Field Studies ................................................................ 14
Test of the Sequential versus Simultaneous Method ................................. 16
Study One: Examining the Relationship Between the AJS’ EWID
Field Studies and Case Outcomes .................................................. 18
Part A: Analysis of Case Outcomes (“Tracking Case Dispositions”) ................. 19
Method ................................................................................................................... 20
Results ................................................................................................................... 21
Case Dispositions across Three Study Sites ............................................. 21
Relationship between Photo Array Presentation Methods and
Case Dispositions? ......................................................................... 22
Relationship between Photo Array Pick Types and
Case Dispositions? ......................................................................... 23
Part B: Analysis of Case Outcomes (“The Evidentiary Strength Study”) ........... 27
Approximating Ground Truth ................................................................................ 28
DNA as a Measure of Eyewitness Accuracy and Approximation of
Ground Truth ............................................................................................. 30
Measurement and Interpretation of Strength of Evidence by Key
Decision Makers ........................................................................................ 33
Instrument Development ....................................................................................... 35
Initial Validation of The Strength of Evidence Scale ............................................ 39
Method ................................................................................................................... 42
Site Selection ............................................................................................. 42
Case Selection............................................................................................ 43
Participants ................................................................................................ 46
Training ..................................................................................................... 47
Study Oversight and Monitoring ............................................................... 48
Consensus Process ..................................................................................... 48
Results ................................................................................................................... 50
Evidentiary Strength Ratings by Presentation Method, Case
Dispositions, and Pick Types..................................................................... 50
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification i Police Foundation Presentation Methods and Evidentiary Strength ................................................... 51
Relationship between Evidentiary Strength and Case Dispositions ...................... 52
Overall Evidentiary Strength for Guilty vs. Not Adjudicated Cases
within Pick Types ..................................................................................... 53
Study Two: An Experimental Study of the Effect of Photo Arrays on
Evaluations of Evidentiary Strength by Key Criminal Justice
Decision Makers ............................................................................. 56
Method ................................................................................................................... 57
Procedure .................................................................................................... 59
Part A: Experimental Results ............................................................................... 59
Impact of Photo Array Knowledge on Evidentiary Strength for
Specific Types of Evidence ....................................................................... 60
Part B: Impact of Photo Array Knowledge on Evidentiary Strength by
Case Dispositions .................................................................................... 65
Part C: Qualitative Case Analysis ........................................................................ 67
Results ................................................................................................................... 68
Cases with no differences in scores when photo array was included ......... 69
Cases in which scores decreased when the photo array was included ....... 69
Cases in which scores increased when the photo array was included........ 70
Part D: Group Differences in Evaluations of Evidentiary Strength ..................... 71
Limitations of the Present Study ........................................................................... 76
Overall Results and Discussion ............................................................................. 79
Conclusion ............................................................................................................. 87
Recommendations for Practice and Future Research ............................................ 94
References ............................................................................................................. 95
Appendices .......................................................................................................... 108
A. Police Foundation Strength of Evidence Scale.................................. 108
B. Final Rater Evaluation Form ............................................................... 116
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
ii
Police Foundation
List of Tables and Figures
TABLES
Table 1. Number of Cases with Dispositions Provided by Research Site ................................... 21
Table 2. Frequencies of Case Dispositions by Lineup Presentation Methods............................. 22
Table 3. Frequencies of Case Dispositions by Pick Types .......................................................... 23
Table 4. Differences in Case Disposition Proportions within Pick Type by Presentation
Method ........................................................................................................................... 27
Table 5. Crime Types in 114 Police Cases ................................................................................. 46
Table 6. Differences in Evidentiary Strength by Presentation Method within Pick Types ......... 52
Table 7. Overall Evidentiary Strength Ratings for Guilty vs. Not Adjudicated Cases within
Pick Type ...................................................................................................................... 53
Table 8. Mean Overall Evidentiary Strength Ratings by Pick Type and Disposition ................ 53
Table 9. Mean Ratings of Overall Evidentiary Strength by Knowledge of Photo Array/Pick
Type ............................................................................................................................... 60
Table 10. Mean Ratings for Strength of Physical Evidence by Knowledge of Photo Array
Within Pick Types ......................................................................................................... 61
Table 11. Mean Evidentiary Strength Ratings for Identification Information Bb Knowledge
of Photo Array ............................................................................................................... 62
Table 12. Mean Ratings of Strength of Suspect Statement by Knowledge of Photo
Array/Pick Type ............................................................................................................ 63
Table 13. Mean Ratings of Strength of Suspect History by Knowledge of Photo Array/Pick
Type ............................................................................................................................... 63
Table 14. Mean Evidentiary Strength Ratings for Victim Characteristics by Knowledge
of Line Up Decision ...................................................................................................... 64
Table 15. Mean Ratings of Witness Characteristics by Knowledge of Line Up Decision ......... 65
Table 16. Overall Evidentiary Strength Ratings by Knowledge of Lineup Decision for Cases
Not Prosecuted .............................................................................................................. 65
Table 17. Overall Evidentiary Strength Ratings by Knowledge of Lineup Decision for Cases
Adjudicated Guilty ........................................................................................................ 66
Table 18. Mean Overall Evidentiary Strength Ratings by Judicial Outcomes within
Pick Type ................................................................................................................... 67
Table 19. Impact of Photo Array on Ratings of Overall Evidentiary Strength Across
Pick Types ..................................................................................................................... 68
Table 20. Group Differences in Ratings of Overall Evidentiary Strength .................................. 72
Table 21. Group Differences in Ratings of Physical Evidence .................................................... 72
Table 22. Group Differences in Ratings of Suspect Statement Evidence .................................... 73
Table 23. Group Differences in Ratings of Suspect History ......................................................... 73
Table 24. Group Differences in Ratings of Victim Characteristics .............................................. 73
Table 25. Group Differences in Ratings of Witness Characteristics ............................................ 74
Table 26. Group Differences in Ratings of Identification Information ....................................... 75
FIGURES
Figure 1. Example of evaluation form for rating a type of evidence............................................ 43
Figure 2. Case Selection Process .................................................................................................. 46
Figure 3. Example of scores across raters for consensus discussion ............................................ 51
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
iii
Police Foundation
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
As with any large-­‐scale project, so many people contributed to these studies and the development of the Evidentiary Strength Scale (Amendola & Slipka, 2009) used herein, that it would be impossible to mention all of them. Nevertheless, a number of people were instrumental in the project from its conception to completion. First and foremost we would like to acknowledge those organizations and individuals who provided financial and programmatic support for this comprehensive effort, as the projects described herein, would not have been possible without them. We are very thankful to the JEHT Foundation. While no longer in operation, JEHT and its staff provided resources to numerous organizations focused on justice-­‐related issues. We are particularly grateful to Scott Matson, M.A. and now Senior Policy Advisor at the U.S. Department of Justice for his initial guidance in our effort. Next, we greatly appreciate the fact that the Open Society Foundations (OSF) recognized the importance of this work already in progress, and committed to provide funding to see that it was completed. We especially thank the oversight provided by Terrance Pitts, J.D., our program officer at the Justice Fund at OSF. Finally, we are sincerely grateful to the Laura and John Arnold Foundation as they made it possible for us to conduct our study in Austin, Texas. These organizations represent the best of the best in terms of their commitment to justice system improvements nationwide and abroad. Because our experimental study was carried out in Travis County, Texas, we would like to extend a heartfelt thank you to the staff and leadership of the Travis County, Texas District Attorneys’ Office, and in particular, District Attorney Rosemary Lehmberg, who afforded us the opportunity to work with her agency to ensure we could carry out the study. She carefully guarded confidential data and information, while allowing us access to staff and case file information necessary for this effort. It is not easy for any agency head to take on the task of supporting scientific research for the sake of improving field practices. Her willingness to participate in this effort was the essential element in making this project a success. Former ADA Claire Dawson-­‐Brown, also of the Travis County DA’s Office, lent tremendous support to this effort by providing necessary case information, providing assistance throughout our three weeks on site in Austin, and by recruiting police investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges from the immediate area to serve as our study participants/subject matter experts. While we are highly grateful to all of those individuals in Austin, our confidentiality agreement does not allow us to mention each by name, but we thank them anonymously. We are also very grateful for the insights provided by both First Assistant District Attorney John Neal and ADA Bryan Case in the Travis County DA’s office. In addition, we acknowledge Chief Art Acevedo of the Austin Police Department who willingly participated in our study and provided the necessary staff and commitment to ensure project success. We also thank Assistant Chief David Carter, Commander Julie O’Brien, Lieutenant Mark Spangler, and Commander Troy Gay in providing us with the much-­‐needed data, information and rapid response to all inquiries. Numerous individuals in all of the project sites supported these efforts substantively. In San Diego, Chief William Lansdowne and key staff of the San Diego Police Department including Captain James Collins, Lieutenant Kevin Ammon, and Karen Goodman were very supportive of our efforts, as was Deputy District Attorney Tia Quick and the San Diego District Attorney’s Office. In Mecklenburg County, NC we are grateful for the support of Chief Rodney Monroe of the Charlotte Mecklenburg Police Department (CMPD) as well as former Chief Darrel Stephens (ret.), who both provided necessary data and guidance. We also appreciate the assistance provided by Major Kevin Wittman (ret.) and Major Paul Zinkann III, both of the CMPD. We wish to acknowledge the support of the Mecklenburg County District Attorney’s Office and ADA Bart Menser for assisting us with conducting the pilot test. Finally, all present and former police investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges from the surrounding area who participated as case evaluators for our pilot test, are sincerely thanked for their terrific insights into our Strength of Evidence Scale, and the process of conducting consensus discussions. While our confidentiality agreement does not allow us to name them individually, we nevertheless thank those whose participation was invaluable to subsequent study efforts. Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
iv
Police Foundation
We also thank Lisa Judge, Principal Assistant City Attorney of the Tucson City Prosecutor’s Office for providing us with case disposition data for the cases in that site. We are pleased to acknowledge the tremendous effort of our partners, the American Judicature Society (AJS) and the Innocence Project who supported us in carrying out an evaluation of outcomes from the Wells, Steblay, and Dysart (2011) study of sequential and simultaneous presentation methods. Former AJS project manager Dani (Danielle) Mitchell was an enormous help in assuring data convergence between our project and that of AJS. Her tireless efforts to support our project, especially the pilot test we conducted in Charlotte, were exceptional at every step. We would especially like to thank Barry Scheck, JD, Co-­‐Founder of the Innocence Project, whose dedication to equal justice and steadfast commitment to evidence-­‐based practices is unwavering. His efforts to ensure we conducted an objective and comprehensive examination were unmatched. In addition, Executive Director Maddy deLone at the Innocence Project provided us with extensive program support and demonstrated a unique commitment to this project every step of the way by helping us in troubleshooting issues as those arose. Importantly, we are very grateful to the efforts of Professor Gary L. Wells, PhD, Nancy K. Steblay, PhD, and Jennifer E. Dysart, the scientists who conducted the AJS National Eyewitness Identification Field Studies. We especially thank Gary for his leadership of that effort and his assistance in helping us to reconcile issues that arose in our study. In addition, we are very grateful to Nancy Steblay who worked with our team to ensure that all necessary data were provided to support our efforts. Many individuals worked with us to develop the Evidentiary Strength Scale, indeed, too many to name. Most notably was James M. Doyle, J.D. a current fellow at the National Institute of Justice, who helped us to gain perspective on evidence and evidentiary standards, and to think more broadly about these issues. In addition, Dr. Jon M. Shane of John Jay College of Criminal Justice and former police captain in Newark, NJ provided us with considerable information about the context of policing. We also thank the following individuals for their participation in this effort: Captain Sam Cogen of the Baltimore (MD) City Sheriff’s Office; Rachel E. Cogen, District Court Judge in Baltimore (MD); Anne M. Corbin, JD; Judge Gregory Jackson (DC); Christopher Ortiz, PhD, City of Glen Cove (NY) Police Department; Chief LeRoy O’Shield (ret.); Professor Alfred Slocum (Rutgers School of Law); Lieutenant LaTesha Watson of the Arlington (TX) Police Department; Judge William Walls (NJ District Court); Assistant Prosecutor Gwendolyn Williams, Newark (NJ). We also thank all those who participated in completing our survey to help generate data useful in our establishing content-­‐oriented validity for the Strength of Evidence Scale. Furthermore, we gratefully acknowledge Jon Gould, J.D., Ph.D. of American University in Washington (DC) and Julia Carrano, J.D. (formerly of American University) for their reviews and commentary on the Strength of Evidence Scale and involving us in their work on wrongful convictions. We would like to make a special note of appreciation to Christine C. Mumma, J.D. of the NC Commission on Actual Innocence and Adjunct Professor at the UNC School of Law (NC) for her many insights into this effort. Special thanks to both Patricia Riley and Renata Cooper, Special Counsel to the U.S. Attorney, United States District Court (DC) and Kristine Hamann, J.D., currently a visiting fellow at the Bureau of Justice Assistance of the U.S. Department of Justice, for providing insights into the challenges of prosecutors, guidelines, and best practices. The authors of this report would like to especially thank Professor Nancy Steblay (Augsburg College), Emily West, Ph.D. (Research Director at the Innocence Project), and Professor David Weisburd (George Mason University) among others for their thoughtful review, comments, and recommendations for improving this report. The Principal Investigator also wishes to thank Professor Weisburd for his ongoing mentorship and contributions to the Police Foundation. Finally, the PI extends a warm thank you to Chief Jim Bueermann (ret.), President of the Police Foundation for providing an environment where scientific research, innovation, and evidence-­‐based practices are recognized as paramount to the field of policing. Finally, the PI thanks former President, Hubert Williams who believed strongly in applying research to practice and was therefore supportive of this important effort. Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
v
Police Foundation
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification Procedures
Executive Summary
Eyewitness identification procedures have been the subject of much scientific and public
policy controversy in recent years (Clark, 2012; Cutler & Kovera, 2008; Garrett, 2008). A recent
experimental field test conducted by the American Judicature Society (AJS) in four sites, was
designed to test the effect of police departments’ methods for presenting photo arrays of suspects
on the identification choices made by victims and other eyewitnesses (Wells, Steblay, & Dysart,
2011). The presentation methods compared in that study included the simultaneous procedure
(all photos of potential suspects are shown at one time) versus the sequential approach (showing
photos of potential suspects one at a time). Known as the American Judicature Society National
Eyewitness Identification Field Studies (hereinafter referred to as the AJS’ EWID Field Studies),
that effort stands as the first multi-site field-test of sequential and simultaneous presentation
methods employing a range of procedures (known as the Greensboro Protocols1) to reduce bias
and standardize lineup protocols.
Despite the fact that there are numerous other influences on the reliability, validity, and
accuracy of victims/witnesses such as viewing opportunity, characteristics of the offender,
characteristics of the crime, and time lag since the crime, etc., the findings demonstrated that the
sequential method resulted in significantly fewer misidentifications (picking a “filler” over the
police-identified suspect) though fewer overall picks (Wells, Steblay, & Dysart, 2011), findings
consistent with much of the accumulated evidence from laboratory studies.
Nevertheless, little is known about whether that finding (the simultaneous presentation
1
The Greensboro Protocols, as they are known, represent a series of reforms for guiding the design and rigor of
future field studies as decided by a group of lawyers, prosecutors, police investigators, and leading behavioral
scientists in the field of eyewitness identification that convened in Greensboro, NC in September 2006.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification 1 Police Foundation method leads to more filler picks) actually makes a difference in terms of the dispositional
outcomes of those cases. Indeed, there are many other things including other forms of evidence
that lead to case dispositions. As such, the Police Foundation research team conducted a followup study of the Wells, et al. (2011) study to examine the relationship between presentation
methods and case outcomes. Referred to as “Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
Procedures,” the goals of this study were to examine the extent to which photo array
presentation methods and their outcomes (pick types) were associated with case dispositions
(adjudicated guilty or not2) across the three field sites, as well as evidentiary strength as rated by
police investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges in Austin, Texas. The latter is
particularly important as these criminal justice practitioners are responsible for interpreting
evidence and facilitating outcomes for the more than 90% of cases that are adjudicated without
juries; primarily, those cases that result in plea deals or, on occasion, bench trials.
One of the important elements of the studies we present in this paper, was the
development of the “Evidentiary Strength Scale” (Amendola & Slipka, 2009, see Appendix A),
an objective and scientifically derived instrument used by key criminal justice decision makers in
this study to rate evidentiary strength in the cases. Evidence of the scale’s reliability and initial
content-oriented validity were collected through a series of procedures3 prior to the
implementation of these studies and is the subject of a separate manuscript (Amendola,
forthcoming). The inclusion and almost exclusive input of police, prosecutors, defense attorneys,
and judges (as key subject matter experts) in the development of that instrument, renders it
particularly relevant to real-world cases and criminal justice proceedings. While the instrument
2
Adjudicated guilty means by plea or finding, not adjudicated means insufficient evidence and/or not prosecuted.
Scale development is described elsewhere (Amendola, forthcoming). In addition, the scale has since been used in
other research by Gould, J.B, Carrano, J., Leo, R. and Young, J. Predicting Erroneous Convictions: A Social Science
Approach to Miscarriages of Justice, Final Report, Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Justice, National Institute
of Justice, December 2012, Grant No. 2009-IJ-CX-4110.
3
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
2
Police Foundation
was informed by other research and scientific knowledge about instrument development and
validation, the content was that of the subject matter experts alone. Rated evidentiary strength
was used as a proxy for ground truth (actual guilt or innocence), as it is clear that case
dispositions may result from extra-evidentiary factors, such as the willingness of victims and/or
witnesses to testify (e.g. witness intimidation); the exclusionary rule (that may prevent a guilty
person from being convicted); personal biases and/or the “stories” or hypotheses about the
suspect’s involvement and the events of the crime constructed by prosecutors (that may cause an
innocent person to be convicted); and alternative theories presented by defense attorneys (again,
that may result in putting a guilty person back on the streets).
In addition, we present a second experimental study in Austin, Texas, the site from the
AJS’ EWID Field Studies that generated the greatest number of photo arrays in which we
examined the impact of including photo arrays and the pick types made on evaluators’ ratings of
the overall evidentiary strength of the cases, as well as any specific type of evidence in the case.
This was achieved by providing one group of evaluators the complete cases, and the other group
with the same cases in which the photo arrays and pick types were fully redacted.
Findings
The presentation method did not relate to the case dispositions, suggesting that the
method may not make a difference in terms of case outcomes. The Wells et al. (2011) finding
that simultaneous photo arrays lead to substantially more filler picks, ultimately did not matter in
these cases in terms of outcomes. In essence, because the misidentifications were those in which
known innocents (fillers or “foils”) were selected, it may indicate that “filler picks” are not
necessarily representative of the more consequential error of picking a suspect in a lineup where
the actual perpetrator is missing. Practitioners and scientists alike have acknowledged and
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
3
Police Foundation
asserted that fillers picked from lineups are very unlikely to be prosecuted, as the fillers are
individuals whom, with exceedingly few exceptions, are “known innocents.” At the same time, it
is important to note that as a result of an increasing number of cases in which DNA evidence has
exonerated individuals erroneously convicted by eyewitness evidence, or perhaps with other
advances and/or increasing public pressure over time, many policy and practice changes have
occurred in prosecutors’ offices, as well as police departments regarding the unreliability of
eyewitness id evidence, and its relative importance in establishing guilt or innocence.
In terms of case outcomes, the results of this study show that the evidentiary strength
ratings were virtually the same (no statistically significant differences) across the groups who
had the photo identification information and those who did not, when considering cases in which
fillers were picked and in which no picks were made. And while the evidentiary strength in the
cases that resulted in guilty outcomes were rated higher when suspects were picked, that only
mattered for the cases in which the evidence was already particularly strong.
Our examination of observational data from the Wells et al. (2011) study revealed a
strong association between lineup choices and case dispositions in that a greater proportion of
cases with guilty findings were associated with suspect picks, as compared to those in which no
picks were made or fillers were picked. Conversely, among cases that were not adjudicated, a
greater proportion of them were associated with no picks and filler picks as compared to suspect
picks, as might be expected. However, while these relationships were strong, they do not reflect
a cause and effect relationship. Indeed there are many other factors, beyond photo array results,
that influence case dispositions and these factors, e.g. other case evidence, may actually explain
the outcomes of the cases, as is elaborated upon below.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
4
Police Foundation
To the extent that our rating scale differentiated adjudicated guilty and non-adjudicated
cases in the sample, it demonstrates that the rating scale is valid as a proxy for ground truth,
lending to the validity of the instrument in accurately representing case strength. Indeed, the
mean evidentiary strength ratings (on a 5-point scale) for those cases adjudicated guilty versus
not prosecuted were well over 4 and well below 3, respectively.
One of the key findings from this study was that the inclusion of a photo array in a case
does not appear to have a significant influence on the overall ratings of evidentiary strength by
key criminal justice decision makers. Indeed, for the cases in which photo lineup information
(including pick type) was provided to the evaluators (the “yes” experimental condition), the
evidentiary strength ratings were statistically equivalent to those from evaluators to whom no
information about a photo array or its outcomes was provided (the “no” experimental condition).
This indicates that the pick of a suspect is not likely to be the source of the increased ratings of
overall case strength; instead, evaluators saw those cases as stronger despite the inclusion of a
photo array. The one exception to that however, was that for those cases that had been
adjudicated guilty,4 evaluators assigned higher ratings when they knew there was a suspect pick
(mean of 4.33), but only in those cases where the evidence was particularly strong despite the
lineup (3.99 on a 5 point scale), and thus would have likely also resulted in conviction. In other
words, the only time that suspect picks affected case dispositions, at least in Austin, was when
the cases were already strong enough to be prosecuted at the outset.
Essentially, these findings suggest that the police in Austin most likely had identified the
actual perpetrators and not the “wrong person,” given the strength of other corroborating
evidence in the cases. Additionally, they suggest that the inclusion of a photo array does not
4Despite
the fact that evaluators did not know how the cases had been adjudicated. Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
5
Police Foundation
provide any additional meaningful benefit to the evidentiary basis for the case (neither
strengthening or weakening it) in the eyes of police, prosecutors, defense attorneys, or judges
than would be provided without a photo array, both a serendipitous and counter-intuitive finding.
This key finding does not imply, however, that photo arrays are not diagnostic among police as
photo arrays may have some investigative importance. Indeed, police may use them as a tool to
help guide their investigations.
More research is needed, however, to examine whether policies or procedures in
investigative units specify the need for a documented justification for including a potential
suspect/confirmed suspect in a lineup or photo array. In this study, the researchers observed
some cases in which the rationale for the inclusion of a particular suspect in a photo array was
not documented in the case file, and was not readily apparent. This does not necessarily mean
the investigators did not have valid reasons, simply that they were not always specified in the
case file. Without a documented justification, it may be that the administration of an array or
lineup is premature; if a suspect is picked, it may lead an investigator in one particular direction
(i.e., put too much weight on the suspect pick, despite the known problems with eyewitness
identification reliability) while at the same time, “ruling out” another viable suspect.
Furthermore, the fact that the picks do not provide any incremental value in most cases
does not mean that they would not strengthen the police and prosecutor “stories” in court cases
(heard by juries). There is no doubt that when a victim in a courtroom points to the suspect
being tried and says, “it was him,” that it has a profound effect on the jury (or any observer for
that matter) in favor of the prosecution’s case. Indeed, research with jurors/juries on the role of
eyewitness id information has regularly shown it to have significant biasing effects on juries
(Bodenhausen, 1990; Chapadelaine & Griffin, 1997; Kerr et al., 2008).
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
6
Police Foundation
The key here is that the drama-induced impact is not necessarily reflective of ground
truth. In other words, the “story” of the case may be improved when a suspect is picked from a
photo array (even when other witnesses or victims do not pick the suspect or pick a filler) or
worsened when fillers are picked or no one is picked (in favor of the defense). Nevertheless, key
decision makers in our study were not necessarily strongly influenced by the photo array
outcomes in interpreting evidence and its strength in connecting the suspect to the crime.
Indeed, an anecdotal finding by researchers in this study (both in the pilot and full study)
was that police, prosecutors, and defense attorneys typically refer to “cases” as the entirety of the
case they would present if heard by a jury, i.e. the evidence, as well as the context and prosecutor
proposed theory/story of the crime, or the plausible alternative explanations offered by the
defense, and not necessarily the objective evidence alone. This is the reality of the adversarial
system, but when no courtroom story lines are required (as is evident in the vast majority of
cases that result in plea agreements), it is very important that the key decision makers are able to
interpret the evidence in an objective manner in order to ensure justice. It is for this reason that
we were also concerned about whether the knowledge of a suspect or filler pick would bias the
interpretation of other case evidence by police, prosecutors, defense attorneys, or judges.
However, we did not find a significant biasing effect of suspect picks on interpretation of
other case evidence, suggesting that criminal justice decision makers can sufficiently separate
different types of evidence, lending validity to the “Evidentiary Strength Scale” (Amendola &
Slipka, 2009). This instrument shows promise as a tool for prosecutors (and potentially others)
to separate the individual case facts from the context or “story” about the case, which should
lend validity to the accuracy of their interpretations of evidentiary strength in delivering just
outcomes. There was evidence of strong consistency of ratings among the evaluators in this data
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
7
Police Foundation
set, suggesting that various evidentiary factors can indeed be assigned values and relative
weights (Amendola, forthcoming).
While it may be surprising that photo array outcomes did not even bias ratings of
“identification information,”5 it does underscore the fact that cases often have a range of
identification information that allows them to connect suspects to the crime, rendering the result
of a photo array a relatively unimportant factor among them. These other factors considered in
the identification information category include: a) clothing, tattoo, hair styles, and other
perpetrator descriptions that can help identify the suspect; b) details of the crime obtained
through the investigation (e.g. finding a stolen item on a suspect, etc.); c) witness id information
(e.g., detailed account of incident is given by witness consistent with other evidence, etc.); d)
third party/ complainant information (e.g. pawn shop owner knows suspect and verifies he/she
came in with stolen property, third party statement implicating the suspect, etc.); and e)
circumstances surrounding arrest (e.g. suspect hiding near crime scene, etc.); f) co-conspirator
flips, thereby implicating suspect; and g) anonymously provided information. Identification
information, therefore, may be stronger, more reliable, or more important to criminal justice
decision makers than photo arrays and their results, or it may simply stand on its own without the
need for a photo array. Importantly, all types of decision makers ranked the identification
information among the most important of the six categories of evidence (police investigators =
#2, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges = #1). Similarly, all but judges ranked the physical
evidence among the most important type of evidence,6 although judges seemed to think that
characteristics of witnesses and victims were more important than physical evidence.
5
“Identification Information” was defined by the participants in the instrument development phase of the project as
“Independent corroboration of information linking the suspect to the particular incident, regardless of source.”
6
There were six distinct categories of evidence as determined by criminal justice decision makers; highest rank
means either a “1” or a “2.”
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
8
Police Foundation
There were some distinct differences in the way that criminal justice practitioner types
evaluate evidentiary strength. Judges, for example, rated suspect histories as stronger in
implicating the suspects than did prosecutors or defense attorneys, but not police. It is possible
that judges become more convinced over time that criminal background and/or gang affiliation
of suspects makes them more likely guilty than do other groups, suggests greater skepticism on
their part about the ability of individuals to change. Interestingly, police and judges rate the
physical evidence higher than defense attorneys when a photo array is present. This is probably
because defense attorneys generally benefit their clients more from knowing that photo arrays
are unreliable, and are therefore less likely to, for example, allow a suspect pick to increase their
ratings of physical evidence. And, judges rate the physical evidence more strongly than the
prosecutors when no photo arrays were provided to either group, perhaps suggesting the
prosecutors’ are more influenced by photo arrays.
Perhaps surprisingly, police rated suspect statements lower than did prosecutors or
defense attorneys. When an ID was present, prosecutors rated the witness credibility as higher.
With regard to victims, defense attorneys tended to rate the victim’s credibility as higher than the
police when no photo array information was provided. This may indicate that prosecutors benefit
more than others from believing the witnesses. Police officers also appear more skeptical with
regard to the evidentiary value of suspect statements than defense or prosecutors; indeed, they
rated witness characteristics weaker than did judges or defense attorneys. These findings are
perhaps attributable to their general cultural tendency toward skepticism.
Extensive scientific findings over the past four decades on eyewitness unreliability,
despite our best efforts to minimize errors and improve reliability of administrative procedures,
and inclusive of the findings presented herein, suggest a different course for the future. The fact
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
9
Police Foundation
that photo arrays in this study did not add to the case’s overall interpreted evidentiary strength
(except in cases that already had particularly strong evidence), or the strength of any specific
category of evidence, suggest that continued use of lineup/photo array procedures may not
actually increase the ability to detect truth or sufficiently improve the evidentiary basis for cases
beyond that provided by other evidence. Scientists have not heretofore examined the validity of
eyewitness identification as an indicator of ground truth, despite the aforementioned, well
documented problem of unreliability and misidentification.
Our study provides some reason for the criminal justice community to question whether
or how the use of photo arrays benefits justice. In light of two facts – many people have been
exonerated by DNA evidence and the primary cause of the wrongful convictions appears to have
been the exclusion of the actual perpetrators from the lineups — it is important to consider both
the relative importance and utility of lineups in achieving justice.
Certainly, many changes have occurred in prosecutors’ offices (and probably many police
departments) regarding the use of eyewitness id evidence over the past few decades, often
requiring significant corroboration of a suspect pick in lineup procedures. In a vast majority of
wrongful conviction cases (where the DNA or other evidence exonerated the suspect), it has
been shown that the identification made by the witness or victim, was the only piece of evidence.
Indeed, the Travis County District Attorney’s office noted in 2012 that ID-only cases do not
provide sufficient justification for prosecution. Assuming this is not likely true in every
jurisdiction, despite today’s knowledge of the fallibility of witness and victim identifications, the
fact that eyewitness misidentifications have led to wrongful convictions should, at a minimum,
raise questions about the use of eyewitness id without other strong corroborating evidence to
accompany it, if not to fully re-examine its use at all as a material factor in cases. The fact that
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
10
Police Foundation
the case dispositions in the Austin cases were largely attributed to other case evidence, prevented
the miscarriage of justice in both potential wrongful convictions and failing to convict the guilty.
Future studies should be conducted to determine if this effect is representative of other
agencies. Additionally, an examination of the policies among prosecutors’ offices should be
conducted to assess the extent to which corroboration of suspect picks is necessary for
prosecutions to proceed. Also, archival analysis of criminal cases should be done to assess the
extent to which justifications are provided for including individuals in photo arrays.
There are some policy implications associated with these findings. Police agencies
should examine the evidence about photo arrays and explore new methods for improving
investigative procedures so as to not over-emphasize photo arrays, given their limitations in
improving the evidentiary strength of cases. Police agencies should also train their officers and
investigators regarding the limited utility and limitations of lineup procedures, as well as
encourage the collection of, and emphasis on, physical evidence and other forms of identification
information in order to minimize reliance on lineups (live and photographic). Police departments
may also benefit from implementing policies that require clear documentation of investigators’
justifications for including potential suspects in lineups and photo arrays so that they are not
done prematurely or lead to an overly narrow investigative focus. Finally, participants in the
criminal justice system should [continue to] emphasize corroboration when relying on lineups.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
11
Police Foundation
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification Procedures
Over the past several decades, a significant body of research has examined the reliability
and accuracy of eyewitness identification in criminal cases (Clifford & Scott, 1978; Cutler,
Penrod, Stevens, & Martens, 1987; Deffenbacher, Bornstein, Penrod, & McGorty, 2004; Sporer,
Penrod, Read, & Cutler, 1995; Wells, 1978), particularly as a result of early laboratory findings
by Loftus and colleagues that eyewitness memory was often unreliable or inaccurate (Loftus,
1975; Loftus, Miller & Burns, 1978; Loftus & Palmer, 1974). Indeed, much of the accumulated
evidence to date has been drawn from the science of cognitive and social psychology, including
studies of human memory, decision-making, and social influence processes. As researchers have
explored the issue, they have identified a number of factors influencing the accuracy and
reliability of eyewitnesses and/or victims of crimes, many of which have been grouped under the
categories of estimator and system variables. Estimator variables consist of those factors specific
to the witness or the crime scene that affect eyewitness memory (e.g. features of the perpetrator
or lighting at the crime scene, etc.) whereas system variables are those controllable factors within
the legal system which may affect eyewitness memory, such as a police officer’s questioning
style, photographic versus physical line up presentation, pre-lineup instructions, etc. (Wells,
Memon, & Penrod, 2006; The Innocence Project7, 2014).
One major research focus in the scientific exploration of the accuracy and reliability of
eyewitness identification has been on the presentation methods used in photo arrays (a form of
lineup using photographs from a variety of sources). The traditional method, known as the
simultaneous presentation method, involves presenting photos as a group, typically with six
7The Innocence Project, as mentioned in this report, refers to the New York City-­‐based non-profit legal
organization committed to exonerating the wrongly convicted through the use of DNA testing, and to reforming the
criminal justice system to prevent future injustice, affiliated with the Cardozo School of Law at Yeshiva University.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
12
Police Foundation
photos (informally referred to as a “six pack”) or nine photos) with pictures appearing side by
side typically across a few rows. The other method, now adopted in many jurisdictions, is known
as the sequential presentation method. It involves showing photos one at a time, in sequence.
Substantial scientific evidence has mounted on both of these methods; however, a series of
recent issues has resulted in significant controversy over which approach produces more accurate
and reliable results (Mecklenburg, 2006; Mecklenburg, Bailey, & Larson, 2008; Malpass, 2006;
Mickes, Flowe & Wixted, 2012; Steblay, 2011; Steblay, Dysart, Fulero & Lindsay, 2001; Wells,
Steblay, & Dysart, 2011; Wixted & Mickes, 2012). One of the more recent issues is the
examination of diagnosticity of various presentation methods. Much like diagnosticity ratios, the
traditional method relies on measures of probative value. More recently, however, Wixted and
Mickes (2012) have asserted that these forms of analysis are theoretically less relevant, and that
receiver operating characteristic analysis (consistent with theories of recognition memory,
particularly signal detection theory) are better equipped to address superiority effects in light of
balancing false positives and true positives.
At the heart of these discussions and complications regarding simultaneous and
sequential procedures is a fundamental question as to whether actual perpetrators are present or
absent in the lineups, and the importance of liberating witnesses from the assumption that the
perpetrator is even in the lineup (Malpass, 2006; Malpass, & Devine, 1981; Steblay, 1997;
Wells et al., 2011). Indeed, Cowdery (2005) aptly noted that among the reasons for wrongful
convictions in the UK, is the sometimes narrow focus of the police and prosecutors on one
suspect while ignoring other suspects. Additionally, an important and defining aspect of real
criminal investigations is that in most circumstances, the police in fact do not know whether the
person they suspect of committing a crime is actually the person who committed it (Clark &
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
13
Police Foundation
Tunnicliff, 2001, p. 200). In a policy review conducted by Malpass (2006), the author summed
up this controversy quite succinctly noting that:
“The utility of simultaneous and sequential lineups is responsive to
two factors external to their actual performance; the values that are
placed on the various eyewitness identification outcomes and the a priori
probability that the police have been able to place the actual criminal in
the identification procedure” (p. 415).
Laboratory versus Field Studies
In order to gain a greater understanding of the impact of lineup presentation methods, a
variety of approaches have been implemented in laboratory settings, and to a lesser degree, field
settings. It may seem that the key challenge associated with the appropriate interpretation and
translation of research into practice in this area of inquiry has been our inability to gain the
unique benefits of both laboratory studies (knowledge of ground truth and experimental control)
and field studies (real-world stakes and nuances).
In the late 1990s and early to mid 2000s, legal and psychological scholars, policy makers,
criminal justice decision makers (police, prosecutors, judges, etc.) and other skeptics alike, began
to question the applicability of a body of literature that was based solely on laboratory studies to
real world settings and situations. Indeed, both forms of science are valid, but each with unique
benefits and challenges (Chae, 2010; Konecni & Ebbesen, 1986; Wagstaff et al., 2003; Wright &
McDaid, 1996). While laboratory studies raise questions of external validity, field studies using
actual police case files lack the experimental control of laboratory studies. Nevertheless, field
studies represent what some have characterized as a “degree of realism and a range of variables
impossible to simulate in a laboratory setting” (Tollestrup, Turtle, & Yuille, 1994).
Laboratory studies in eyewitness identification are often conducted with staged crimes
where the perpetrators are always known to the researchers allowing them to test the effects of
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
14
Police Foundation
manipulated variables on ground truth or in other words, a “known” perpetrator. Lab studies
also allow for significant controls to be exerted, thereby limiting the influence of other factors on
the manipulated variable(s). Many legal practitioners and researchers themselves, however, have
justifiably raised questions regarding the limitations of laboratory studies, including their
inability to mimic reality in many respects, the low stakes associated with wrong choices, and the
application of those findings to real-world settings. Indeed, as Tollestrup et al. (1994) noted
some 20 years ago, the historical over reliance on findings from laboratory research has left the
field of eyewitness memory open to challenges of external validity (Clark & Tunnicliff, 2001;
McCloskey & Egeth, 1983; McKenna, Treadway, & McCloskey, 1992; Yuille & Wells, 1991).
Laboratory research testing of the sequential versus simultaneous debate has also come
under fire for a number of other reasons, including the same foils design issue (Clark &
Tunnicliff, 2001), the target to foils shift phenomenon (Clark & Davey, 2005), bias in single
versus double blind administration (Greathouse & Kovera, 2009), and a number of other issues
related to the building and discrediting of evidence speaking to either a sequential lineup
superiority effect or a simultaneous lineup superiority effect (Carlson, 2008; Lindsay & Wells,
1985; Malpass, 2006; Memon & Gabbert, 2003; Mickes et al., 2012; Steblay, Dysart, & Wells,
2011). Nevertheless, while the bulk of the scientific literature informing the sequential versus
simultaneous debate continues to come largely from laboratory settings or meta analysis of these
laboratory findings, questions of external validity and applicability to the real world have
remained to some degree.
Even though field studies are limited in many respects as described previously, they also
present an opportunity to test variables of interest in real-world settings and thereby, an
opportunity to overcome the problems inherent in laboratory studies (Schacter et al., 2008;
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
15
Police Foundation
Wagstaff et al., 2003; Wells et al., 2000; Wells et al., 2006), despite their inability to establish
ground truth in studies of eyewitness identification, an issue that will be addressed in the present
study. Nevertheless, until very recently there had not been a robust test of the sequential versus
simultaneous procedures in the field.
Test of the Sequential versus Simultaneous Method
In response to calls for a more robust field study, the American Judicature Society (AJS)
implemented the “Eyewitness Identification Field Studies” (hereafter referred to as the AJS’
EWID Field Studies) designed to compare sequential and simultaneous presentation methods in
multiple field sites (Wells, et al., 2011). That study consisted of a series of controlled field
experiments and was informed both by project partners8 who collaborated with the AJS, as well
as the Greensboro Protocols9 to establish greater scientific control in the course of conducting a
field study. The AJS’ EWID Field Studies represented perhaps the first time social scientists,
prosecutors, criminal defense attorneys and the law enforcement community had come together
in a systematic fashion to inform the testing of new ways to improve the reliability and
credibility of eyewitness evidence. Wells et al. (2011) implemented that experiment in four sites:
Charlotte-Mecklenburg County, North Carolina; Tucson, Arizona; San Diego, California; and
Austin (Travis County), Texas.
In the AJS Field Studies test of the sequential versus simultaneous variable, all factors
other than the presentation method were held constant10 following the guidelines established in
the Greensboro Protocols. In other words, at the field sites, the protocol required standardized
8
Center for Problem Oriented Policing, Police Foundation, and Center for Modern Forensic Practice at the John Jay
College of Criminal Justice (no longer in operation).
9
The Greensboro Protocols represent a series of reforms for guiding the design and rigor of future field studies as
decided by a group of lawyers, prosecutors, police investigators, and leading behavioral scientists in the field of
eyewitness identification that convened in Greensboro, NC in September 2006.
10
With some exceptions regarding blinded versus double blind procedures, see Wells, et al. (2011).
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
16
Police Foundation
instructions administered via a laptop presentation mode, ensured that all lineup administrations
were double blind11, and also required the collection of confidence statements. The lineup
presentation method itself – sequential versus simultaneous – was randomly assigned by
computer for each lineup immediately prior to viewing; therefore, it (the treatment condition)
was also blind to lineup administrators. While sample sizes were substantially lower in three of
the four sites as compared to that of Austin, Texas, there were different reasons for this.12
Nevertheless, as planned, data were combined across sites to increase the overall sample size,
increase statistical power, and to create greater ability to generalize findings regionally and
nationally.
The investigators in the AJS’ EWID Field Studies found that the sequential procedure
yielded a slightly higher rate of identification of the suspect than for the simultaneous condition
(Wells et al., 2011) although this difference did not reach statistical significance. However, in
analyzing the filler (eyewitness pick of a “known innocent”) identification rates, the researchers
found that the simultaneous condition yielded a much higher rate of filler picks than did the
sequential condition (18.1% for simultaneous compared to 12.2% for sequential), a finding that
was indeed statistically significant, and which represented almost a 50% higher rate of false
identifications for the simultaneous presentation method. The research team also found that
there were no reductions in identifications of suspects using the sequential procedure, despite the
fact that its critics have been concerned about reductions in accurate identifications. Wells, et al.
11
A double blind condition is one in which the lineup administrator does not know who the suspect is. In the AJS
field studies, a lineup was deemed not to be double-blind if the administrator acknowledged knowing the image of
the suspect, the case detective was the administrator, or the detective commented in the record that the lineup was
not performed double blind.
12
In NC, state law changed during the experiment requiring all agencies to use double-blind sequential methods. In
Tucson, AZ, data were being collected as part of a related study conducted by Nancy Steblay in collaboration with
Lisa Judge of the Tucson City Prosecutor’s Office and the methods and sample sizes were deemed appropriate for
inclusion in the AJS Field Studies. In San Diego, CA low participation resulted from compatibility between the
photo data bank and the lineup presentation software that significantly impeded full data collection.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
17
Police Foundation
(2011) acknowledged, however, that laboratory studies have generally shown a reduction in
suspect picks using a sequential procedure, adding that the fact their studies did not show the
same effect could be explained by differences in the protocols used in their study not present in
laboratory studies (e.g., a “not sure” option was provided, witnesses were permitted to review the
lineup a second time – a “second lap,” and others).
Wells and colleagues (2011) noted that: “later articles will continue to extract additional
new findings from this data set” (p. 17). While the release of additional findings has not yet
occurred, in this follow-up study we do track the cases to examine the relationship between
presentation methods and case outcomes, if any.
STUDY ONE:
Examining the Relationship between the AJS’ EWID Field Studies and Case Outcomes
This observational study consists of an examination of archival data from the AJS’ EWID
Field Studies (Wells et al., 2011) and follow-up data collection on the dispositions of cases
across three of the four study sites. In addition, because case dispositions may not always
accurately reflect actual guilt or innocence, the second and more robust follow-up analysis will
consist of an examination of the relationship between presentation methods/associated pick types
and evidentiary strength ratings made by police investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and
judges — also herein referred to interchangeably as (key) criminal justice decision makers, raters,
evaluators, or practitioners — in Austin (Travis County), TX, one of the four study sites used in
the Wells et al. (2011) study. That examination is expected to provide some understanding
regarding the accuracy and validity of the identifications made by witnesses and victims.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
18
Police Foundation
Part A: Analysis of Case Outcomes (“Tracking Case Dispositions”)
With regard to case dispositions of the AJS’ EWID Field Studies’ lineups, we address two
key questions. First, what is the relationship between the lineup presentation method (sequential,
simultaneous) and the case dispositions, if any? Importantly, as the AJS’ EWID Field Studies
showed, the presentation method affected the type of picks made by the witnesses/victims. While
random assignment of sequential or simultaneous presentation methods allowed for an
independent assessment of the impact of presentation method on the pick types in the AJS’
EWID Field Studies,13 the relationship between the presentation method and the case disposition
is highly dependent on other factors beyond photo array results. As such, we would not expect
there to be a direct relationship between the lineup presentation method and the case disposition
independent of the outcome of the photo array (suspect pick, no pick, filler pick), although that
relationship should be ruled out first.
The second key question related to tracking case dispositions is: What is the relationship
between the pick types in the Wells et al. (2011) study, and the case dispositions? Clearly, many
other case factors (other evidence and context) were likely present that may or may not have
been known to the witnesses when they reviewed the photo array, and importantly, those case
factors may have had more to do with the case outcomes than did the pick types (suspect, filler,
or no pick). Additionally, it is possible that other case factors such as physical evidence may
have outweighed lineup results thereby diminishing their importance in the case outcomes. As
such, this analysis is observational, not causal in nature (i.e. it is not possible to determine which
factors were most influential in the case dispositions, nor the relative weight of each in
13This assertion presumes that the range of case conditions and estimator variables were not different across groups.
While we are not aware of whether an analysis was done to examine the extent to which the case characteristics
were randomly distributed across the groups, random chance would suggest that the groups (sequential and
simultaneous) had equal distributions of various case conditions and estimator variables. Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
19
Police Foundation
determining case dispositions due to the nature of these data and the design of both the AJS’
EWID Field Studies and the present study). Nevertheless, it was important that we establish the
relationships between the presentation methods, pick types, and case dispositions.
Method
In order to ensure that the cases associated with the lineups from the AJS’ EWID Field
Studies (Wells et al., 2011) had reached disposition, we required that at least one year pass since
the lineups were presented.14 In order to assess the relationship between lineup presentation
methods and case dispositions, we conducted an archival analysis with data collected from the
AJS’ EWID Field Studies (Wells et al., 2011). We received disposition data from all four sites,
and while the agencies were not able to provide us with dispositions for every case, we examined
the data for all but one site.15 Because the descriptions of the outcomes varied by agency, we
were only able to categorize the dispositions as having been adjudicated guilty (by plea or
judgment) or not prosecuted.16 While the results of the Wells’ et al. (2011) studies indicated a
causal relationship between presentation methods (sequential, simultaneous) and selection
outcomes (a suspect was picked, no one was picked, or a “filler” was picked), our examination of
the relationships between those variables and case dispositions is not cause and effect. Instead,
we analyzed the associative relationships between lineup presentation method and case
dispositions as well as that between the pick type and case dispositions.
14
Research team members and their partners agreed that a 12-month lag would, in most cases, be sufficient for the
case to reach disposition. In fact, in most cases, the follow up period was significantly greater than 12 months, and
other than those for which a status could not be determined or made available to the researchers, most cases had
reached disposition by the time this analysis was conducted.
15
Dispositions from Charlotte-Mecklenburg County were not used because the study was prematurely discontinued
based on changes in state law mandating the double-blind sequential procedure for lineup presentation.
16
Due to differences in disposition types across the three sites (Austin, San Diego, and Tucson) we were only able to
report on whether cases were adjudicated guilty (by plea or judgment) or not prosecuted (not referred by police, not
accepted for prosecution due to insufficient evidence, or other reason).
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
20
Police Foundation
Results
Case Dispositions across Three Study Sites
This analysis included cases from Austin, Texas, the AJS’ EWID Field Studies’ site
generating the greatest amount of data from lineups,17 as well as San Diego and Tucson. The
cases for which dispositions were reported by the agencies are presented in Table 1 below. As is
shown in the Table, the adjudication rate among these cases is 38%, with Austin having the
highest (48%) as compared to just 25% in Tucson and 21% in San Diego18. The adjudication
rates appear much lower than the national average of 78% in state courts, where the vast majority
Table 1. Number of Cases with Dispositions Provided by Research Site
Agency (Study Site)
N
Adjudicated Not Prosecuted
Total
Austin, Texas
143 (61%)
67 (47%)
76 (52%)
143 (100%)
San Diego, California
24 (10%)
5 (21%)
19 (79%)
24 (100%)
Tucson, Arizona
69 (29%)
17 (25%)
52 (75%)
69 (100%)
236 (100%)
89 (38.1%)
147 (62.3%)
Total
236
of all felony convictions in the U.S. occur (BJS, 2003).19 One possible explanation for the
differences in conviction rates is that our data set primarily consisted of stranger crimes (suspect
17
We captured case dispositions for only the subset of cases in Austin (there were 455 photo arrays administered in
the Wells et al., 2011 study) that were selected for the “Evidentiary Strength Study” and the “An Experimental
Study of the Effect of Photo Arrays on Evaluations of Evidentiary Strength by Key Criminal Justice Decision
Makers” (both described later in this report) so as not to over-represent the effects obtained from Austin in this
dispositional analysis.
18
Could possibly be the result of the sample size of just 29 cases.
19Drawn from a geographically representative sample of the approximately 3,100 counties in the U.S. in 2000.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
21
Police Foundation
and victim unknown to each other),20 whereas in non-stranger crimes, the victim or witness
provides the name of the perpetrator and his/her relationship to the victim, rendering a lineup
unnecessary. Also, estimates from other studies more closely align with the data in our study. For
example, one study demonstrated that only 37% of rape cases are prosecuted (Kilpatrick,
Resnick, Ruggiero, Conoscenti, & McCauley, 2007), although our study had considerably fewer
sexual assault cases (in Austin they were excluded altogether). Additionally, Garner and
Maxwell (2009) reported that only 30% of domestic violence cases investigated by police led to
the filing of criminal cases by prosecutors (a rate closer to that which we found).
Relationship between Photo Array Presentation Methods and Case Dispositions.
The data shown in Table 2 appear show that there are far more cases “not prosecuted”
than “adjudicated.” Nevertheless, the proportions of photo arrays from the Wells et al. (2011)
study for which the suspects were adjudicated guilty or not prosecuted did not significantly differ
by presentation method21 (see Table 2). This finding confirmed our expectations that the method
of presentation would not be directly associated with case outcomes because the dispositions are,
in large part, dependent on other case factors.
Table 2. Frequencies of Case Dispositions by Lineup Presentation Methods
Disposition
Sequential
Simultaneous
Total
Not Prosecuted
59 (59%)
88 (64%)
147 (62.3%)
Guilty (plea or judgment)
41 (41%)
48 (36%)
89 (37.7%)
100 (42.4%)
136 (57.6%)
236 (100.0)
Total
2
Χ = .799 (1 df), p = n.s.
20We
identified one exception to this in our examination; while Wells, et al. (2011) required that cases for inclusion
be those where the suspect and victim were not known to each other, the officer reported that after running the
lineup, it was discovered that the victim/witness knew the suspect in the case. 21We did not examine sentencing outcomes, however.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
22
Police Foundation
Relationship between Photo Array Pick Types and Case Dispositions. For this
analysis, we examined the relationships between the pick types from the Wells et al. (2011)
study and subsequent case dispositions. While it is not possible to say that the pick types led to
the case dispositions, the data presented in Table 3 suggest strong associations between suspect
picks and guilty dispositions. A greater proportion of cases in which suspects were picked were
associated with guilty findings (68%), whereas for those in which fillers were picked or no picks
were made, less than 27% each were associated with guilty findings.
Table 3. Frequencies of Case Dispositions by Pick Types
Case Disposition
No Pick
Suspect
Filler
Total
Not Prosecuted
89 (75.4%)
22 (31.9%)
36 (73.5%)
147 (62.3%)
Guilty (plea or judgment)
29 (24.6%)
47 (68.1%)
13 (26.5%)
89 (38.1%)
118 (100%)
69 (100%)
49 (100%)
236 (100%)
Total
Χ2 = 38.429 (2 df) p ≤ .001, Cramer’s V = .404 p ≤ .001
While these observational data demonstrate a strong association (Cramer’s V = .40)
between lineup choice and the disposition of the case, it should be underscored that these are not
cause and effect relationships. The differences in proportions of pick types from the Wells, et al.
(2011) study resulted from a witness or victim viewing a lineup (in one of two presentation
formats); however, the dispositions are likely to have resulted from a host of other factors.
Although the relationship between suspect picks and guilt appear consistent with our
expectations of justice, it may not be because of obvious or intuitive reasons, i.e. a suspect
picked in the case leads to guilty findings. We cannot know, for example, which pieces of
evidence were most influential in determining the case outcomes, and cannot say that the pick
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
23
Police Foundation
types caused the case dispositions when relying solely on the data from the Wells et al. (2011)
study. As such, it is not clear the extent to which the pick type (e.g. suspect pick) and the
associated certainty level influenced the case disposition (e.g. guilty). It would be inaccurate,
then, to conclude from these data that suspect picks lead to more guilty outcomes, or that filler
picks and no picks will result in cases not being prosecuted in any given case. Other plausible
explanations may exist and whenever there are other hypotheses about or evidence in the case
beyond the photo array (as may be more typical today than 30 years ago), there could be other
factors that outweigh the photo lineup result. As such, not only are the findings above not cause
and effect, they do not provide information about the potential impact of other factors on case
dispositions (the why?).
Furthermore, in the 32 percent of cases where suspects were picked but the cases were
not adjudicated, there may be any number of plausible explanations for the outcomes as follow:
a) even though the suspect was picked, the witness, victim, and/or police got it wrong (the lineup
was absent the actual perpetrator) for a variety of reasons, or b) the suspect may actually be
guilty, but there was no plea reached or the case could not be successfully prosecuted due to
insufficient corroborating evidence, unwillingness of the victim/witness to testify, the exclusion
of inculpatory evidence, etc.
Similarly, the fact that in almost 30 percent of cases with filler picks, the suspects were
nevertheless adjudicated guilty could have resulted from many other factors including: a) even
though a filler was picked, the actual suspect may still be guilty (the victim/witness made the
‘wrong’ pick; and b) the other evidence in the case may be so strong as to outweigh the ‘wrong
pick’ by the witness/victims. Finally, similar circumstances could have explained the “no pick”
adjudications, all suggesting a range of influences or errors that could stem from the: a) victims
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
24
Police Foundation
and/or witnesses (inability to recall events, not getting a good enough “look” at the perpetrator,
unwillingness to testify, etc.); b) police (e.g., missing the actual perpetrator when constructing
the lineup, feeling pressured to close cases, failing to fully investigate other plausible suspects, or
potentially using procedures that may be overly suggestive or fail to minimize potential biasing
effects); c) the legal system (adversarial roles of prosecution and defense, the exclusionary rule
and inadmissibility of evidence, failing to fully protect victims from retaliation, etc.); d)
prosecutors (being keen to get a conviction, failure to fully investigate or audit police
investigations, or inadvertent failure to turn over Brady material); and e) defense attorneys (being
keen to get their clients off despite their dual obligation of serving justice (Espinoza & WillisEsqueda, 2008; Freedman, 1966; Hedding, 2002), among others. It should be noted that these
potential errors or biases might be rare.
Nevertheless, since the AJS’ EWID Field Studies demonstrated a linkage between
presentation methods and pick types, one might expect that the case dispositions would be
related to presentation methods when considering each pick type separately. It would also stand
to reason that if the pick types resulted, at least in part, from the presentation method, then the
presentation method may also indirectly relate to the case dispositions. However, our analysis
suggests that this was not the case as shown in Table 4 below.
For cases in which a suspect could not be identified (no pick), there were no significant
differences between proportions of suspects not prosecuted or adjudicated guilty by presentation
method (73% sequential vs. 78% simultaneous for not prosecuted and 28% sequential vs. 22%
simultaneous for adjudicated guilty). The same was true when fillers were picked (64%
sequential vs. 77% simultaneous for not prosecuted and 36% sequential vs. 23% simultaneous
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
25
Police Foundation
Table 4: Differences in Case Disposition Proportions within Pick Type by Presentation Method
Case Dispositions
No pick made
Sequential
(seq.)
Simultaneous
(sim.)
Total
Chi square
Not Prosecuted
39 (72.2)
50 (78.1)
89 (75.4)
Guilty
15 (27.8)
14 (21.9)
29 (24.6)
Χ2 = .551(1)
n.s.
54 (45.8)
64 (54.2)
118 (100)
Not Prosecuted
11 (34.4)
11 (29.7)
22 (31.9)
Guilty
21 (65.6)
26 (70.3)
47 (68.1)
32 (46.4)
37 (53.6)
69 (100)
Not Prosecuted
9 (64.3)
27 (77.1)
36 (73.5)
Guilty
5 (35.7)
8 (22.9)
13 (26.5)
14 (28.6)
35 (71.4)
49 (100)
Total
Suspect was picked
Total
Χ2 = .170(1)
n.s.
Filler was picked
Total
Χ2 = .848(1)
n.s
for adjudicated guilty), although the numbers of cases (n) within cells were particularly small.
Finally, even when suspects were picked, there were no significant differences in the
proportions of suspects not prosecuted versus adjudicated guilty by presentation method (34%
sequential vs. 30% simultaneous for not prosecuted. and 66% sequential vs. 70% simultaneous
for adjudicated guilty). These data suggest that the presentation method of the photo array is not
associated with the case dispositions for any of the types of picks made.
While there was a slightly higher rate of not prosecuted cases when no picks were made
within the simultaneous condition (as compared to the sequential condition), as well as a slightly
higher rate of guilty verdicts for those in the sequential method (as compared to the simultaneous
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
26
Police Foundation
condition), these differences were not statistically significant. In addition, while there appears to
be a slightly higher rate of convictions for those in which suspects were picked in the
simultaneous method than as compared to the sequential method, this difference was also not
statistically significant.
The many potential influences on case dispositions underscore the fact that case
dispositions are not necessarily strong and conclusive representations of actual guilt; indeed,
many guilty suspects do not end up being convicted for a variety of aforementioned reasons, and
some innocent suspects get convicted (as we know from DNA and other case exonerations or
subsequent findings of innocence). Due to these potential errors in the criminal justice system,
the next section examines the use of a proxy for actual guilt or innocence (evidentiary strength as
evaluated by police investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges, independently and
collectively) not tainted by some of the factors unrelated to actual guilt or innocence described
above including failure of witnesses to testify, improperly obtained evidence, witness
intimidation, the quality of legal counsel, statements made to defendants by police and
prosecutors, despite actual guilt or innocence.
Part B: Analysis of Case Outcomes (“The Evidentiary Strength Study”)
It has been argued here that case dispositions may not always be reflective of ground
truth about guilt or innocence of suspects in cases, and certainly not in perpetrator-absent lineups.
As such, in this part of the case outcome analysis, we assess the accuracy and validity of the
photo array decisions made by witnesses and victims in the AJS’ EWID Field Studies by relying
on a proxy of ground truth – evidentiary strength and its relationship to case dispositions from
the Wells et al. (2011) study.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
27
Police Foundation
Approximating Ground Truth
Despite the increased rigor of the methodological design afforded to the AJS’ EWID
Field Studies by the Greensboro Protocol, the inability to know ground truth about whether or
not the suspect was indeed the perpetrator of the crime remained a central concern for the AJS’
EWID Field Studies team and partners. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that witnesses/victims
frequently selected a “known innocent” person in perpetrator-absent lineups in a laboratory study
conducted by Finklea & Ebbesen (2007) suggesting a bias to make a selection in the laboratory
setting. However, the realities of actual cases are often far more nuanced than they are in
laboratory studies; hence, the argument that field studies are optimal in assessing outcomes in
real world settings is weakened by this key limitation.
While it is true that field studies provide for “known innocents” as fillers in photo arrays,
there is no way to validate whether the suspect identified by police is the actual perpetrator and
thus, no way to know ground truth about the guilt or innocence of the suspects, saving only for
conclusive DNA evidence which is available in only an estimated 10-20% of actual cases.22 And
despite the fact that it is believed DNA serves as “proof positive” of actual guilt or innocence,
many cases contain DNA of the suspect but not the victim, or conversely, the victim but not the
suspect rendering it not always conclusive. Additionally, researchers showed that among jurors
who demonstrated a higher level of pro-prosecution bias, they overestimated the weight of weak
DNA evidence (Smith & Bull, 2012). Finklea and Ebbesen (2007) found that when DNA was
used as a validity check for ground truth, witnesses were inaccurate in just 5% of cases, and
among the 95% of cases in which witnesses were accurate, two thirds of those were for sexual
assaults. This higher accuracy rate may result from prolonged exposure in sexual assault cases
22
See Myrna Raeder, J.D., L.L.M. (2012). Overturning wrongful convictions and compensating exonerees in
Springer encyclopedia of criminology and criminal justice
http://www.springer.com/social+sciences/criminology/book/978-1-4614-5689-6?detailsPage=authorsAndEditors
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
28
Police Foundation
than in other briefer encounters such as purse snatchings, robberies, or assaults, or the
availability of DNA in sex crimes.
Because we cannot be sure if the actual perpetrator is in the lineup in real world cases, the
present study included the development and inclusion of a proxy for ground truth – strength of
evidence (as evaluated by key criminal justice decision makers) – that could be used in all case
types, not just sexual assaults or those with DNA evidence. While the idea of a proxy for ground
truth is not novel, research on proxies for ground truth are scant, despite the fact that their use
may be quite effective for overcoming the limitations of field studies. As a result, researchers
continue to routinely call for more in-depth research examining the role of strength of evidence,
and its measurement in decision-making (see e.g., Adams, 1983; Smith & Bull, 2012; Wells et
al., 2006). While ratings of evidentiary strength have more traditionally been used by prosecutors
to prioritize cases for prosecution, their use in approximating ground truth in eyewitness
identification validation is very limited.
In one known study, Behrman and Davey (2001) attempted to overcome the limitation
that ground truth is typically not readily available by examining eyewitness identification and
connecting it to evidentiary strength. Following in the footsteps of Tollestrup et al. (1994)23,
these researchers attempted to alleviate the problem of perpetrator-absent lineups through the use
of various categories of extrinsic incriminating evidence. Using their own system of
categorization of case factors that had minimal or substantial probative value, these researchers
examined 271 actual police cases for suspect identifications. In the archival analysis of these
cases, suspect identifications were assessed for three different levels of extrinsic evidence that
the criminal justice practitioners themselves defined; no extrinsic evidence (no evidence was
23
Tollestrup et al. (1994) asserted that generating various evidentiary levels provided a solution to the problem of a
certain unknown percentage of real world lineups not including the perpetrator of the crime.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
29
Police Foundation
recorded), evidence of minimal probative value (evidence was incriminating but not particularly
strong), and evidence of substantial probative value (evidence was highly incriminating and thus
strong). This particular categorization scheme was one of the limitations of their approach.
Despite the rating category of “no evidence,” the rating scale was essentially dichotomous in that
evidence was characterized as either minimally probative or substantially probative, and
therefore lacked variation, precision, and utility for other purposes. Indeed, as noted by Gould,
Carrano, Leo and Young (2012) in a recent study, the quality of cases with extrinsic evidence
used by Behrman and Davey (2001) varied drastically between those deemed by the researchers
to have minimal or substantial probative value (p. 51). The assertion by Gould et al. (2012) infers
both a lack of relative ability to discriminate among levels of evidentiary quality (i.e. beyond just
“weak” or “strong”) and utility of the Behrman and Davey (2001) categorization scheme, which
is not likely reflective of actual cases. Another key limitation of that categorization scheme was
that because it was developed by the researchers, as well as used by them in the rating of cases, it
lacked necessary content-oriented validation from other experts.
DNA as a measure of eyewitness accuracy and approximation of ground truth. It
has often been overlooked that evidence often plays a limited role in predicting judicial outcomes
(see, e.g. LaFree, 1989; Peterson, Ryan, Houlden, & Mihalgovic, 1987) and that many extralegal
factors influence the criminal justice process (Adams, 1983; Alderden & Ullman, 2012; Peterson
et al., 1987), leading some to assert that “evidence plays an important but far from exclusive role
in predicting judicial outcomes” (Peterson et al., 1987). While in an ideal world it could be
assumed that a case disposition is an objective criminal justice system variable, it is clear that it
is at least partly influenced by a number of variables not related to the strength of the case. For
example, case dispositions may be influenced by legal realities described in the previous section
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
30
Police Foundation
(e.g. the exclusionary rule, the inability or unwillingness of witnesses to testify, insufficient
representation, the plea bargaining process, etc.). As such, it can be argued that rated case
strength is a suitable and potentially more accurate proxy for ground truth than case disposition
given that it can be determined for all cases regardless of other influences on the case disposition.
In rating case strength, however, there is a wide range of evidence types potentially
available, and some are better than others. In the literature on wrongful convictions, mistaken
eyewitness identification is often cited as the most common culprit (Connors, Lundregan, Miller,
& McEwen, 1996; Gross & Shaffer, 2012; Wells et al., 1998). Indeed, in nearly 75% of wrongful
convictions overturned through DNA testing (312 as of December 31, 2013 according to the
Innocence Project), mistaken eyewitness identification was found to have played a prominent
role.24 This has resulted in a vast body of research focused on identifying best practices for the
collection and handling of eyewitness evidence at the investigator level (NIJ, 1999) and also on
the evaluation of the strength and accuracy of eyewitness evidence in the courtroom, which has
largely revealed a limited understanding of the complexities associated with eyewitness
testimony by law enforcement, attorneys, judges, jurors, and the lay public (Benton, Ross,
Bradshaw, Thomas, & Bradshaw, 2006; Devenport, Penrod, & Cutler, 1997; Magnussen,
Melinder, Stridbeck, & Raja, 2010).
In response to the controversy surrounding mistaken eyewitness identification, and its
contribution to wrongful convictions, Finklea and Ebbesen (2007) set out to determine the true
rate of eyewitness accuracy using DNA to establish ground truth regarding guilt or innocence of
24
Although other contributing causes include not yet validated or improper forensics, false confessions/admissions,
forensic science misconduct, government misconduct, unreliable informants and bad lawyering, The Innocence
Project has identified eyewitness misidentification as the greatest single cause in wrongful convictions nationwide.
For more information see http://www.innocenceproject.org
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
31
Police Foundation
a defendant.25 They found that in the sample of target present lineups (n = 173), witnesses were
accurate in selecting the suspect 90.2% of the time (they rejected the lineup 8.7% of the time and
selected a foil in 1.1% of the lineups). In the sample of target absent lineups (n = 10), the
witnesses selected a suspect 100% of the time (even though absent from the lineup). Despite the
high rates of eyewitness identification accuracy noted by the authors, perhaps the more
interesting finding is the fact that in target absent lineups, witnesses still selected a suspect 100%
of the time, indicating a biasing effect of simply having a lineup and suggesting that suspect pick
rates are higher for lineups in which the police get it wrong.
Indeed, while we may be certain that the identification of a known innocent filler is
clearly an error in the field, we can never be 100 percent sure that the identification of the
suspect is correct, barring perhaps those cases with conclusive DNA evidence (and even then,
there are exceptions). Although DNA would offer the most promise here, and has served as the
basis for reversing convictions (312 DNA exonerations as of December 2013),26 the proportion
of cases for which DNA is present, collected, and processed, is estimated to be only about 10%
and there are numerous reasons for this (Gardner & Anderson, 2012; Pratt, Gaffney, Lovrich, &
Johnson, 2006; Samuels, Davies, & Pope, 2013). While Finklea and Ebbesen (2007) found that
witnesses were accurate 95% of the time in selecting the suspect in cases with DNA evidence
available (by using it as a proxy for assessing the accuracy of identifications) it should be noted
that approximately two-thirds of the cases were rapes or other sex crimes; the nature of which is
fundamentally different from say, a purse snatching on the street. Sex crimes, characterized by
close contact between the perpetrator and victim, increase the probability that DNA will be
transferred, and therefore collected (although not necessarily analyzed).
25
DNA was used establish whether lineups were target present or target absent, although it was not always possible
resulting in a sample of 71 unknown lineups.
26
http://www.innocenceproject.org/
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
32
Police Foundation
Furthermore, in describing the type of DNA samples obtained, Finklea and Ebbesen
(2007) also noted a full spectrum of sample quality. Indeed, in their archival analysis some of the
cases had DNA evidence matching a victim but not the suspect, while some had DNA matching
a suspect. Other cases had DNA matching both victim and suspect while others still had sources
of evidence with inconclusive results highlighting the fact that DNA evidence is still prone to
bias and misinterpretation of the strength of its evidential standard (Smith & Bull, 2012). In
addition to the limited cases where DNA analysis has been conducted, it is also unclear as to
whether cases with DNA are also unique in other ways that could influence the outcomes.
Few field studies on eyewitness identification procedures have attempted to overcome the
limitation that ground truth is typically not available. Those that have developed a form of proxy
for approximating ground truth (e.g. Behrman & Davey, 2001; Smith & Bull, 2012)
acknowledge that no matter how highly incriminating or strong the evidence is, perpetrator
presence in a lineup can never be assured. Finklea and Ebbesen (2007) present a very strong case
for using DNA as a proxy to establish ground truth and assess the accuracy of eyewitness
testimony outside of the lab but even so, the fact that DNA is present in a small proportion of
criminal cases limits its usefulness as a proxy for ground truth.
Measurement and interpretation of strength of evidence by key decision makers.
The findings noted by Smith and Bull (2012) regarding bias in the interpretation of weak DNA
evidence echo a particular focus by the National Research Council (NRC) Committee on
Forensic Sciences regarding the adversarial process of the American judicial system and the
ability of its members to accurately measure and interpret evidence as noted in the following:
“The adversarial process relating to the admission and exclusion of scientific
evidence is not suited to the task of finding “scientific truth.” The judicial system
is encumbered by, among other things, judges and lawyers who generally lack the
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
33
Police Foundation
scientific expertise necessary to comprehend and evaluate forensic evidence in an
informed manner….” (NRC Report on Forensic Science, 2009, p. 12).
Recently, the NRC Committee on Forensic Sciences has focused on the mounting body
of evidence indicating that there are biases and inconsistencies in the examination of various
types of evidence (particularly forensic evidence), as well as the interpretation of the strength
(and meaningfulness) of said evidence, interpretation of which is often left to the police,
prosecutors, defense attorneys, judges and juries (Kassin, Dror, & Kuckucka, 2013; NRC, 2009;
Smith & Bull, 2012; Smith & Bull, 2013). Given that the vast majority of case dispositions are
arrived at through plea bargaining, it is important to make sure that those responsible for making
initial charges (police), and offering or accepting pleas (prosecutors, defense attorneys, and
judges) are able to accurately assess the strength of evidence in cases; yet, there is reason to
believe that improvement is needed in this area.
It should be noted that while the importance of multiple perspectives in informing the
criminal justice system has long been recognized27 there continues to be a wide gap between
what the scientific literature says and what practitioners believe, and the influence of those
beliefs in informing their practice. For example, in a recent study exploring the disclosure of
forensic evidence by police, Smith and Bull (2013) surveyed 398 experienced police
interviewers regarding their use of and perceptions of the strength of various types of forensic
evidence. The researchers found that the vast majority of these interviewers had not received any
training on how to interpret or use such forensic information; yet, despite the lack of training, the
perceived strength of the forensic evidence in a case was reported to affect some participants’
27
In 1998, the National Institute of Justice of the U.S. Department of Justice assembled a multi-disciplinary working
group comprised of both researchers and practitioners (law enforcement, prosecutors and defense attorneys) to make
recommendations for the collection and preservation of eyewitness evidence. Released in October 1999, the report:
“Eyewitness Evidence: A Guide for Law Enforcement,” represents one of the first attempts at building a body of
reference that incorporates both scientific and practitioners perspectives.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
34
Police Foundation
interview strategies and more specifically, the timing of the disclosure of such evidence during
an interview.
Previously, Smith, Bull, and Holliday (2011) had found that mock jurors were able to
correctly identify the strength of various types of evidence when they were not presented with
other case information but found that the perceived strength of that same evidence was
significantly inflated when presented in the context of a criminal case (“the story model”)
especially when the evidence was of a weak or ambiguous nature. These findings echo those
from almost two decades earlier that juror interpretations of ambiguous evidence were highly
influenced by their need for cognition, the presentation order of arguments (whether pre or post
evidence presentation), and the abilities of both the prosecution and defense to persuade (Kassin,
Reddy, & Tulloch, 1990). With particular regard to eyewitness evidence, this mounting body of
evidence has also revealed a limited understanding of the complexities associated with
eyewitness memory and testimony by key decision makers within the criminal justice system
(law enforcement, attorneys, judges, and jurors) and the lay public, including students (Benton et
al., 2006; Boyce, Lindsay, & Brimacombe, 2008; Devenport et al., 1997; Lindholm, 2008;
Magnussen et al., 2010; Pozzulo, Lemieux, Wilson, Crescini, & Girardi, 2009; Wise & Safer,
2010).
Instrument development. In order to address limitations associated with prior forms of
proxies for ground truth, and evidentiary strength rating systems, researchers (Amendola &
Slipka, 2009) oversaw the development of a comprehensive, more sensitive instrument that was
seen as a necessary part of the research design in the experiment described herein. It was clear
that such an instrument should not be developed solely by the researchers and other social
scientists, but rather be fully informed by actual criminal justice decisions makers in real cases,
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
35
Police Foundation
i.e. police investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges from a range of jurisdictions.
This approach was expected to lend face validity to the instrument, and be representative of the
knowledge and information held by these critical actors in the criminal justice system.
As such, the development of the instrument was iterative in nature; for example, it relied
upon more than one group to establish the content of the instrument, including the categories of
evidence and their respective definitions, as well as specific types of information within them
(e.g. Physical Evidence would be a category and DNA evidence would be one type within that
category). In addition, it included objective reference points (exemplars) representing various
levels of evidentiary strength along a 5-point continuum. Those categories of evidence, specific
types, and exemplars were cross-validated through an iterative process in order to establish initial
content validity of the rating instrument. The final revised instrument by Amendola and Slipka,
(2009)28 is provided in Appendix A.
The Strength of Evidence Scale (Amendola & Slipka, 2009) was informed in part by a
particularly ambitious case evaluation system used by the Bronx County District Attorney
(Bronx DA) for prioritizing cases for prosecution.29 This system, developed in the 1970s, aimed
to make the subjective assessment of evidentiary strength more objective (National Center for
Prosecution Management & National District Attorneys Association, 1974, hereafter referred to
as the NCPM and NDAA). As one of the first measures of case strength, it evaluated cases along
four key case characteristics30: a) the nature of the crime charged, b) the gravity of the particular
28
Revised and completed after multiple iterations and a pilot test conducted in Charlotte-Mecklenburg County, NC.
The development of the case evaluation system for the Bronx District Attorneys Office was undertaken by the
National Center for Prosecution Management (NCPM) and the National District Attorneys Association (NDAA) at
the request of Mario Merola, District Attorney.
30
The nature of the crime charged was determined by the grade of the felony involved; the gravity of the particular
offense was determined primarily by the extent of personal injury and property loss or damage; propensity of
commit violent crime was determined via the nature of background and prior criminal record; and case strength was
determined primarily by facts, circumstances and available evidence.
29
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
36
Police Foundation
offense, c) the use of the defendant’s criminal history to determine propensity to commit violent
crime, and d) the evidentiary strength of the case (Merola, 1982 as cited in Gould et al., 2012).
A primary limitation of using the same ambitious approach to develop a rating instrument
for this study, is that the Bronx County DA case evaluation system attempted to overmathematize all aspects of cases, and that despite its rigor and sophistication given its
development some 40 years ago, it failed to address the relative level of subjectivity involved
when evaluations are made by people. While aptly arguing that objective standards would vastly
increase the utility and reliability of these systems for prosecution (NCPM & NDAA, 1974, p.
13) the Bronx County DA report did rightfully acknowledge that case evaluation systems would
never be able supplant the individual case preparation and trial expertise of the individual
prosecutor (NCPM & NDAA, 1974, p. 11). In the development of the system, however, it seems
that the Bronx County DA failed to adequately acknowledge the limitations of individual
differences in those making the evaluations or a need for training31 or evaluator concurrence.
After the development, piloting, and use of The Strength of Evidence Scale (Amendola &
Slipka, 2009) for the purposes of the experiment presented herein, Smith and Bull (2012)
published an article based on a measure they developed to identify pretrial attitudes affecting
juror perceptions of physical evidence. With the Forensic Evidence Evaluation Bias Scale
(FEEBS), the authors hoped to: a) inform legal policy about the admissibility of ambiguous
evidence, and b) contribute to the improvement of methods of presentation and explanation of
physical evidence in the courtroom (p. 799). What the authors found was that DNA evidence of a
31
While there were initial meetings with representatives from the Bronx County DA to specify the criteria and
prosecution policy that was to form the basis for referral, coding for the nature of the offense, seriousness of the
offender, and decision on whether the case should be referred to the Major Offense Bureau was conducted by the
Chief of the Major Offense Bureau. A procedures manual for use of the case evaluation form was prepared for staff
assigned to complete and review the case evaluation form. Overall, scores represented the subjective judgment of
the Major Offense Bureau personnel and prosecutorial policy.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
37
Police Foundation
weak evidential standard that was presented to juror participants who demonstrated a higher
level of pro-prosecution bias for forensic evidence was informed by that existing bias. Indeed,
despite instruction on how to objectively evaluate physical evidence, it was almost impossible to
remove the influence of bias in the interpretation of evidentiary strength for these jurors, leading
them to perceive weak DNA evidence as being of a higher probative value than it actually was.
While subjective decisions can be made to be more systematic and objective through the
development and calibration of an instrument, it is unrealistic to assume that everyone processes,
combines, and interprets information (case facts) in the same manner; indeed, it would not likely
mimic reality without some form of evaluator concurrence. As such, we utilized a multidisciplinary approach in order to ensure that the instrument reflected the perspectives of all the
major decision makers in the criminal justice system (police investigators, prosecutors, defense
attorneys, and judges). An additional component of implementing the rating scale, was the
provision of training and a calibration process among evaluators across disciplines within the
criminal justice system. Furthermore, recognizing that even with instruction it is almost
impossible to completely eliminate the influence of bias on rater assessments of evidentiary
strength, a consensus exercise was built into the case evaluation process to accompany use of this
rating scale. In these respects, our study attempted to create a more objective process for
evaluating evidentiary strength.
Indeed, in much the same way as the purpose of the Bronx DA case evaluation system,
the purpose for the “Strength of Evidence Rating Scale” developed for these studies of “Photo
Arrays in Eyewitness Identification Procedures” was to overcome the limitation of perpetrator
absence by attempting to approximate ground truth. Unlike the Bronx case evaluation system
which relied heavily on its mathematical quantification of all aspects of a case, or Smith and
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
38
Police Foundation
Bull’s (2012) forensic evidence bias scale focusing on jurors and utilizing fictional case
scenarios, the use of an alternate proxy for ground truth that relies on actual criminal justice
decision makers’ evaluations of evidentiary strength in actual police cases will provide
information about the relationship between case outcomes and actual evidence. As such, while
the proxy for ground truth developed and administered in this study is clearly not the first
attempt at coming up with such a system, it may be the first that comes from multiple criminal
justice perspectives.
Initial Validation of The Strength of Evidence Scale (Amendola & Slipka, 2009). The
Strength of Evidence Scale improves upon previous research of measures of case and/or
evidentiary strength that lacked content-oriented validity evidence (Merola, 1982; Behrman &
Davey, 2001). It does this in three respects: 1) via the generation of exemplars that serve as
objective scoring anchors; 2) via the engagement of experts in the scale development to establish
evidence of content-oriented validity; and 3) via the use of five more specified levels of
evidential strength. In addition, the use of the instrument by other researchers has set the stage
for building validity evidence. Indeed, Gould et al. (2012) characterized the Police Foundation’s
Strength of Evidence Scale, as being “a more nuanced, objective, and applicable tool” (p. 51).
In order to establish validity of a measurement tool, it is important that the process
include at a minimum, a content-oriented approach to validation. Typical of these approaches is
the establishment of items, criteria, definitions, and standards representing the domain of interest
via subject matter expert consensus. In the development of that instrument, we established
groups of subject matter experts to provide initial input and then gathered additional data from
others. Our process was thus iterative in nature; that is, it involved many repetitive steps to
ensure that experts provided input, inter-rater agreement was established, item analysis was
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
39
Police Foundation
conducted, and that the first developmental process was tested and cross-validated on two
additional groups. While initial reliability evidence was established, evidence on validity must
accumulate over time through multiple methods and multiple studies, especially that which is
predictive in nature. As a result, it is not possible to say that an instrument is in itself valid;
instead, validity evidence is accumulated over time in terms of establishing the validity of a
measurement tool for particular purposes. The resulting instrument for evaluating case strength is
presented in Appendix A. Details regarding the process for establishing evaluation criteria,
conducting reliability analysis, and collection of initial data useful for establishing validity are
provided elsewhere (Amendola, forthcoming).
While the instrument may appear overly complex, it is actually a very simple 5-point
Likert scale where a “5” means that the evidence is particularly strong in linking to the identified
suspect, and a “1” means that the evidence is exceptionally weak in linking to the identified
suspect. The scale requires ratings across six categories of evidence32 and an overall evidentiary
strength rating, but appears more complex, ironically by its simplified use of exemplars
representing consensus on what experts believed to best represent various points on the rating
scale, as shown in Figure 1 below. The purpose of the exemplars was to help guide the rater in
determining the appropriate strength of a particular piece of evidence in the case that was being
rated. The use of a Likert rating scale without this specificity would minimize its accuracy. This
example comes from the final rating instrument used in Austin, and represents the second of a
number of evidence types within the category of “physical evidence.” The description of the
type of evidence (surveillance tapes, etc. is shown on the left) followed by the five point rating
scale, “anchored” by examples of the various points on the scale (based on the average values
32
The categories of evidence consisted of physical evidence, suspect statement information, suspect history
information, victim characteristics, witness characteristics, and identification information.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
40
Police Foundation
Figure 1. Example of evaluation form for rating a type of evidence
provided by dozens of police investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges).
Anything in this evidence type that was weaker than these examples, would fall below the lowest
exemplar (here that was rated a 2.81). The box on the right is where an evaluator would register a
score if this type of evidence were available, or would have entered not applicable (n/a) if there
was no evidence.
This instrument will likely increase accuracy and meaningfulness through the inclusion
of: a) standardized definitions of the categories of evidence, b) descriptions of various types of
evidence that would be subsumed in one of the six broader categories of evidence (for example,
“surveillance tapes/photos from crime scene” as shown in above example), and c) the use of
exemplars that provide reference points (“anchors”) on the rating scale to increase objectivity.
Without these, a simple 1 to 5 rating scale would be subject to potentially strong variations in the
meaning or definition of different types of evidence, a potential halo effect or bias of specific
evidence in interpreting other evidence, and potentially strong variations in interpretation of the
strength of a specific case fact, piece of evidence, or other information where no standards had
been established. Nevertheless, the utility of this rating instrument is limited by the need to
provide training to case evaluators and to calibrate its use within a particular rating group.
In this study we relied on key criminal justice decision makers to assess the strength of
evidence; a deviation from the past research along three lines of inquiry: 1) assessing jury
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
41
Police Foundation
decision making (e.g. Smith & Bull, 2012); 2) examining evidentiary strength for prioritization
of cases for prosecution (e.g. NCPM & NDAA, 1974); and 3) relying on researchers to evaluate
evidentiary strength (e.g. Behrman & Davey, 2001). This alternative approach was seen as
necessary given the fact that juries are only involved in a small proportion of cases; indeed, it is
estimated that no more than 5 -10% of cases are decided by jury trials (Durose & Langan, 2003;
Oppel, 2011; Pastore & Maguire, 2003; Peterson et al., 1987) and the other approximately 9095% of all cases are resolved through the plea bargaining process (Glater, 2008).
The focus on police, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges to evaluate evidentiary
strength in this study underscores the fact that these key decision makers have perhaps the
greatest effect on case dispositions through plea-bargaining. Even when pleas are not reached,
these individuals have significant influence on the way in which evidence is communicated in
court. While there have been a number of key studies that have focused on the role these
influential actors play in the criminal justice system, these studies have largely tended to look at
these actors individually rather than collectively (Alderden & Ullman, 2012; Bushway & Redlich,
2011; Frederick & Stemen, 2012; Jacoby, Mellon, Ratledge, & Turner, 1982; Peterson, Hickman,
Strom & Johnson, 2013; Smith & Bull, 2013).
Method
Site Selection
The “Evidentiary Strength Study” was conducted in Austin (Travis County), Texas, the
site in the AJS’ EWID Field Studies’ (Wells et al., 2011) from which the bulk of the data were
generated. Using the “Strength of Evidence Scale” and the associated training and consensus
building process described previously, a panel of criminal justice decision makers rated the
evidentiary strength of a subset of cases in which lineups were administered in the Wells et al.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
42
Police Foundation
(2011) study. Austin was selected as the experimental site not only because the greatest number
of photo arrays were administered there, but also because restricting our study to one robust site
for the outcome evaluation allowed us to exert greater experimental control, minimizing effects
associated with potential site differences, and thereby increasing statistical power.33
Practitioners were asked to evaluate the strength of evidence across six categories of evidence
(physical, suspect statement, suspect history, victim characteristics, witness characteristics, and
identification information) and to give an overall assessment of the evidentiary strength of the
case (see Appendix B).
Case Selection
The cases were selected from the overall pool of cases in the AJS Field Studies in which
all the experimental protocols had been followed in phase one (n= 340), thereby also minimizing
the potential for error associated with potential differences in protocol adherence. The cases
included were criminal and primarily made up assaults and aggravated assaults, burglaries,
robberies, and thefts. A diagram of the case flow for selection in both the “Evidentiary Strength
Study” and the “Experimental Study of the Influence of Photo Arrays on Police Investigators’,
Prosecutors’, Defense Attorneys’, and Judges’ Evaluations of Evidentiary Strength in Criminal
Cases” (presented subsequently in this report) as described here is provided in Figure 2.
33
The three other sites were excluded from this site for a variety of reasons. First, two sites (Charlotte, NC and San
Diego, CA) had limited sample sizes. In Tucson, AZ a study had been underway for some time without District
Attorney involvement in the AJS studies, and prior to the establishment of a methodology for the outcome analysis.
Another key reason this study’s PI implemented this study in Austin alone, is that a means for controlling site
variance (if any) would have been necessary at the outset (i.e. a blocked-randomized design would have allowed for
better statistical control), and the limited sample sizes obtained in the Wells, et al. (2011) study across the remaining
sites would make the design of a new experiment less robust. While cost would have increased if the study was done
in the three remaining sites, this was not a deciding factor in the decision to implement the study in Austin alone; the
sample sizes were small enough to be an inefficient (and possibly ineffective) use of resources to address the
questions and a single study in Austin was therefore seen as the most reasonable, pure, and simple approach.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
43
Police Foundation
Total Initial Lineups (N = 340) Excluded Cases (N = 27) (6) Juvenile cases (6) Sexual Assault cases (15) County DA cases 313 Lineups Sequential (N=157) No Pick (N=95) Filler Pick (N=21) Suspect Pick (N=41) Simultaneous (N=156) No Pick (N=99) Filler Pick (N=24) Suspect Pick (N=33) Stratiiied Random Sample Selection (N=200) Sequential (N=102) No Pick (N=46) Filler Pick (N=21) Suspect Pick (N=35) Excluded Cases (N=49) Crossovers deletions (N=7) Missed Juvenile Status (N=10) Missed Sexual Assault (N=1) Isues with Suspect Mention (N=9) Inconsistent Case Details (N=10) Inconsistent Case -­‐ Lineup Info (N=6) Misc. Lineup Issues (N=4) Other Miscellaneous (N=2) Simultaneous (N=98) No Pick (N=47) Filler Pick (N=23) Suspect Pick (N=28) Analysis Sample (N=151) Simultaneous (N=76) No Pick (N=35) Filler Pick (N=19) Suspect Pick (N=22) Sequential (N=75) No Pick (N=29) Filler Pick (N=16) Suspect Pick (N=30) Figure 2. Case Selection Process
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
44
Police Foundation
From the Wells et al. (2011) study lineups, we eliminated non-pristine lineups34 resulting
in an initial sample of 340 lineups. Next, due to state law in Texas, and instructions from the
District Attorney’s Office, we also eliminated any cases involving juvenile suspects (n = 6) and
lineups associated with cases that involved sexual assault (n = 6) resulting in 328 lineups that
met the criteria of the agency and research team. Additionally, we eliminated cases that were
referred to the county attorney’s office (n = 15), resulting in a final sample of 313 eligible
lineups. We conducted a power analysis that suggested a sample of 200 lineups would be more
than sufficient to ensure a high level of power for the study. In order to obtain relative balance
among the pick types for the experimental study and to maximize our sample size, we selected
all lineups resulting in “filler picks” (n = 45) and “suspect picks” (n = 74), due to the limited
number of cases for comparison, and to ensure sufficient power for assessing the impact of
suspect picks, for a total of 119 lineup cases. Of the remaining more common outcomes “no
picks” (n = 194) we used a random number generator to select 93 “no pick”35 cases stratified
within sequential and simultaneous procedures resulting in 212 lineups with the expectation that
additional lineups might need to be excluded during the course of the study. Indeed, upon
review of case details after the initial selection, additional lineups were found to be ineligible for
inclusion by research staff (i.e., juvenile involvement, sexual assault, inconsistencies in case
details, suspect not mentioned in case, and a number of other reasons). As a result, the number of
no pick lineups was actually reduced to 64, and further reductions in the other two groups
34
Cases were deemed to be non pristine by Wells et al. (2011) if one or more of the following conditions applied: 1)
the lineup line administrator knew which person was the suspect, hence not double blind; 2) the eyewitness knew the
suspect, hence not a stranger identification; 3) the identification decision of the witness could not be clearly
determined, in other words, unclear if decision was no pick, filler pick or suspect pick; and 4) the witness saw the
suspect photo after the crime but before viewing the lineup, hence not a first time viewing.
35We attempted to achieve a total of 74 “no pick” lineups to match the number of suspect picks, but knew that
approximately 20% of the cases may become ineligible. Therefore, the inclusion of 93 cases would allow a buffer so
that at least 74 cases would result.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
45
Police Foundation
resulted in 52 suspect pick lineups, and 35 filler picks rendering a final sample in Austin of 151
lineups for evaluation, sufficient to detect medium effect sizes, according to our power analysis.
The 151 lineups selected for evaluation in this project comprised 114 distinct police cases
and 139 individual suspects. Given that the lineup was the unit of analysis for both the AJS
EWID Field Studies, and this follow-up study, any given suspect may have appeared in one or
more criminal cases or other lineups but the other lineups may not have been selected for this
study. Of the 114 distinct police cases included in this study, 78 of those cases had respective
District Attorney (D.A.) case files for which information was collected. The 114 police cases in
Phase II consisted of a variety of felony crimes (see Table 5), although more than one crime type
may have been present in a given case.
Table 5. Crime Types in 114 Police Cases
Crime Types
Total n
Crime Types
Total n
Aggravated robbery
36
Murder
4
Assault/aggravate assault
10
Robbery
8
Burglary/home invasion
14
Robbery by assault
14
Criminal mischief
1
Robbery by threat
2
Fraud/forgery
3
Stabbing
1
Harassment
1
Theft
20
TOTAL
114
Participants. Case evaluators were selected from a recruited pool of 26 criminal justice
decision makers (10 female and 16 male). In total, the pool of evaluators to choose from on any
given day was actually 36,36 as several individuals were able to serve in more than one capacity
36
A number were recently retired, one was currently employed in a neighboring jurisdiction, and several defense
attorneys and judges were still practicing on at least a limited (part time) basis.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
46
Police Foundation
on different teams.37 While in a day, we needed just eight (8) participants (two each of police
investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges), this expanded pool of evaluators
afforded the research team more flexibility to construct different teams each day, given the fact
that not every rater was available for all of the scheduled evaluation sessions.
Training. Training was provided to the participating criminal justice evaluators to
explain how the instrument was developed, what the exemplars (rating scale anchors)
represented, how they were derived, and how to rate each category of evidence independently.
The latter was discussed at length in the training, as the goal of the evaluation was to minimize
biases that may occur when the same piece of evidence was considered to fall into more than one
category, or when the strength/weakness of any type of evidence influences the interpretation of
other evidence. Because the evidence categories were established as independent with unique
definitions and unique types of evidence within each category, raters were encouraged to first
identify the appropriate category under which to evaluate a particular piece of evidence before
assigning a score. The aforementioned training steps required a training block of approximately
four to five hours.
The next step in the training was to have evaluators practice using the instrument on
actual cases provided by an independent jurisdiction. This training began with a group session in
which all of the case evaluators read the same case and came up with a rating. This was
followed by a group discussion in which the variability in ratings was discussed in order to
calibrate the ratings, so that all had an equal understanding of what constituted weak, moderate,
37
Of the available raters, eight were able to conduct evaluations in more than one capacity given their previous
experience serving the criminal justice system in different roles. For those individuals, they were asked to rate from
the specific perspective they were assigned to that day. Those eight raters’ roles consisted of: three (3) raters who
were able to serve in the capacity of either district attorney or judge; two (2) raters were able to serve in the capacity
of either district attorney or defense attorney; one (1) rater who was able to serve in the capacity of either defense
attorney or judge; and two (2) raters who were able to serve in the capacity of either district attorney, defense
attorney or judge.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
47
Police Foundation
and strong evidence, as well as how to arrive at a category score and overall case rating score.
Suffice it to say, this process was not mathematical in nature; one piece of evidence could be so
strong (e.g. DNA) that an overall case rating could be 5 even in the absence of any other
evidence. This process was expected to result in a restriction of range in scores, but at the same
time, a more objective rating of case strength based on agreement from the instrument
development teams and the rater groups on what actually represents strong or weak evidence.
The remainder of the two-day training was spent evaluating 4-5 additional cases and conducting
consensus discussions so that raters could best prepare for rating actual cases in groups of four.
Once the training was completed, the research team coordinated with the case evaluators
to establish teams and corresponding meeting dates to conduct the evaluations. Key criminal
justice decision makers were provided with case files associated with a particular suspect
associated with a lineup and case.
Study oversight and monitoring. Research team members were on site for the entire
time during which ratings were conducted in the fall of 2012. The Principal Investigator and
second author each oversaw the rating teams and assigned cases for each day, while a third team
member ensured materials were sufficient for scoring and assisted in checking in the data at the
end of each consensus session (also checking for missing data). Depending on the complexity of
the case as estimated by the researchers, approximately two (2) to thirteen (13) cases were
provided to evaluators in an eight-hour day.
Consensus process. After half of the day’s cases had been rated by all individual
evaluators (evaluators were provided with ‘morning’ and ‘afternoon’ cases) the researcher
facilitated a consensus discussion that began with raters (one at a time) providing their scores for
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
48
Police Foundation
all six categories of evidence followed by their overall case strength rating (down a column) that
were transferred to a white board by the researcher as shown below in Figure 3. The facilitator
Evidentiary Strength
Category
Rater #1: Rater #2:
(Police
(District
Investigator)
Attorney)
3
Rater #3:
((Defense
Attorney)
3
Rater 4:
(Judge)
Physical evidence
2
4
Suspect statement
information
n/a
n/a
Suspect history
2
1
2
2
Victim characteristics
4
3
4
3
Witness characteristics
3
3
2
2
Identification information
4
3
3
3
Overall evidentiary
strength
3
3
3
4
n/a
n/a
Figure 3. Example of ratings across raters for consensus discussion
and group reviewed the rows across, noting discrepancies of two points or more. The research
protocol required that when such a discrepancy was found between any two evaluators within the
team, or when the raters differed in their belief that a certain type ofevidence was present or not,
a facilitated discussion among evaluators was necessary. The purpose of this was not to force
raters to come up with the same scores38; indeed it was clear that none of these practitioners
could be “bullied” by their peers. Instead, the purpose was to ensure that all raters had seen
38This would indeed be a problem for the research team as well, as this could have led to greater restriction of range,
thereby limiting variability and the ability to detect differences in the analysis. The study already had some built-in
restriction in range as a result of using a calibrated and anchored rating scale, as well as training evaluators to rate
cases in one specific category. Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
49
Police Foundation
and/or considered all evidence thoroughly because of the limited time allotted to review the case
(which would not necessarily be the case if the evaluators were working in their formal
capacities). Finally, discussions were allowed when raters simply wanted to discuss the case
with others to clarify information or help interpret the meaning of evidence. These discussions,
however, were not allowed during the individual rating process, as raters were required to retain
the scores of the their evaluations independent of any discussion, or influence by other evaluators.
As shown in Figure 3, the two ratings for physical evidence by the police investigator and the
judge were two points apart, and therefore required discussion to determine the reasons for the
differences. If after the discussion both raters wanted to keep their original scores, they were able
to do so. However, if either one changed his/her rating, that person was required to note this on
his/her final rating form, and check the box that best explained their reason for the change (see
Appendix B).
Results
Evidentiary Strength Ratings by Presentation Method, Pick Types, and Judicial Outcomes
Operating on the assumption that ratings of evidentiary strength, as described herein, are
a better proxy for ground truth than case dispositions, the examination of the relationship
between those ratings and the presentation methods, pick types, and case dispositions, will allow
for a more detailed assessment of the accuracy and validity of the picks made by the witnesses
and victims in the Wells, et al. (2011) study. In order to validate the likely accuracy of the pick
types by witnesses in that study, we address the following three questions:
1.
Are ratings of evidentiary strength in this follow-up study associated with the
presentation methods used in the Wells et al. (2011) study?
2.
Are evidentiary strength ratings associated with case dispositions in Austin?
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
50
Police Foundation
3.
Are the overall evidentiary strength ratings in this follow-up study associated
with the pick types made in the Wells et al. (2011) study?
Presentation Methods and Evidentiary Strength
In our study, criminal justice decision makers (police, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and
judges) were not made aware of case dispositions or photo array presentation methods when
reviewing the evidence39 associated with suspects from the lineups by Wells et al. (2011). As
such, we did not expect the presentation methods to have a strong association with the ratings of
evidentiary strength. However, since the Wells et al. (2011) study indicated that presentation
method impacts the type of pick being made, we did provide evaluators with information about
the pick types (suspect, filler, or no pick) via the lineup software printout showing the photos on
a single page (vertically with all six photos listed down the page), along with the associated
names, the police-identified suspect, and which person, if any, was picked from the lineup.
There was also information included in many of the officers’ reports about the various lineups
run and the results.40
As we expected, the evidentiary strength ratings did not significantly differ across the two
photo array presentation methods within pick types for either the prosecuted or not prosecuted
cases (see Table 6 below). While it appears that the simultaneous method is associated with
higher scores when suspects are picked for the non-prosecuted cases, that difference was not
significant.
39Any
reference to presentation methods or case dispositions in the police reports or in the case file presented to
evaluators was redacted or removed.
40The officers’ narratives may have contained information about other suspects in the case as well, and/or other
lineups run with other suspects and their results, even if those suspects were not part of our sample
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
51
Police Foundation
Table 6. Differences in Evidentiary Strength by Presentation Method within Pick Types
Case Dispositions
Sequential
(seq.)
Simultaneous
(sim.)
T-Test
Not Prosecuted
1.92
2.12
n.s.
Guilty
4.40
4.36
n.s.
Not Prosecuted
2.91
3.57
n.s.
Guilty
4.29
4.38
n.s.
Not Prosecuted
2.58
2.25
n.s.
Guilty
4.19
4.42
n.s.
No pick made
Suspect was picked
Filler was picked
Relationship between Evidentiary Strength Ratings and Case Dispositions
The purpose of this section is to examine the relationship between the overall evidentiary
strength ratings within pick types by case dispositions to determine whether or not the stronger
cases were associated with guilty outcomes and the weaker ones with suspects not being
prosecuted. As shown in Table 7, the highest scoring cases (regardless of suspect pick type) on
average were associated with “adjudicated guilty” outcomes in Austin. This, despite the fact that
the evaluators had no idea if the cases were adjudicated guilty or not but saw all evidence in the
cases. Specifically, those cases with higher evidentiary strength (mean of 4.34) were associated
with the guilty verdicts and those with weaker ratings (mean of 2.50) were associated with the
non-prosecuted cases, suggesting the police probably had the correct suspects, and that the
prosecutors made accurate decisions; i.e. the system got it right.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
52
Police Foundation
Table 7.
Overall Evidentiary Strength Ratings for Guilty vs. Not Adjudicated Cases within
Pick Types
Photo array decision
by victim/witness
Adjudicated
Guilty
(1 – 5)
Not
Adjudicated
(1 – 5)
Finding and
significance
No pick made
4.38
2.03
t(60)=11.25 p ≤ .001
Filler pick made
4.32
2.34
t(25)=6.331 p ≤ .001
Suspect pick made
4.33
3.14
t(45)=5.407 p ≤ .001
Overall Evidentiary Strength by Pick Types and Case Disposition
When considering the strength of cases across photo array pick types among those in
which suspects were adjudicated guilty, there were no differences as shown in Table 8 below.
This means that for the cases with guilty outcomes, the evidence was equally as strong across all
pick types. The fact that the evidentiary strength did not vary across pick types among the
Table 8. Mean Overall Evidentiary Strength Ratings by Pick Type and Disposition
Disposition
No pick
Filler
pick
Suspect
Pick
F, p
Eta
squared
(effect
size)
Post hoc
comparisons
with significant
differences
--
--
Adjudicated
Guilty
4.38
4.32
4.33
F(2,62)
= .077
n.s.
Not
Prosecuted
2.03
2.34
3.14
F(2,69) =
.170
7.097 p < .01 (small)
no pick vs susp
p < .01
adjudicated guilty cases also suggests that the suspect picks did not add anything to the
interpreted case strength.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
53
Police Foundation
Among the non-adjudicated cases, however, the mean evidentiary strength ratings were
higher when suspects were picked (3.14) as compared to those in which no pick was made (2.03).
This difference is important when considering that both the instrument (scores ranging from 1 –
5) and the training provided to the evaluators were clear in terms of the meaning of scores less
than three or greater than three. Specifically, scores below three (3) were described as “evidence
and/or information that is weak” (in terms of connecting to the suspect) whereas assigning scores
greater than three (3) meant “evidence and/or information that is strong.” However, this finding
may suggest that even when the other case evidence is weak, key criminal justice decision
makers interpret the evidentiary strength higher when a suspect is picked. Nevertheless, the mean
rating of 3.14 for the suspect-pick cases is closer to the mean of the weak, non-prosecuted cases
of 2.5 (mean difference of .64) than to the mean of the strong adjudicated guilty cases of 4.34
(mean difference of 1.20), suggesting that those cases would not likely have been prosecuted
anyhow.
As observational data, the findings above do not tell us whether or not the raters were
influenced by the suspect picks in making their overall rating, or if the cases associated with
suspect picks were also associated with stronger overall evidence at the outset. In addition,
because these results were based on group means, we also cannot interpret findings from any
particular cases in which the scores and outcomes were anomalous, without a qualitative, caseby-case analysis. The findings thus far do not allow us to delve deeply enough into the questions
of the influence of photo arrays on key criminal justice decision makers, or the accuracy of any
particular pick by witnesses or victims in those cases. Therefore, in order to gain a better
understanding of the relationships between the presentation methods and pick types to the
evidentiary strength ratings and case dispositions, we conducted an experiment to test the
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
54
Police Foundation
differences in evaluations of evidentiary strength in which cases were reviewed by two groups,
one that had the photo array information and pick type and the other for whom that information
was redacted. Conclusions and recommendations of these studies will be provided at the end of
this report subsequent to the second study presented below.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
55
Police Foundation
STUDY TWO:
An Experimental Study of the Effect of Photo Arrays on
Evaluations of Evidentiary Strength by Key Criminal Justice Decision Makers
This experimental study was conducted in order to examine more information about a
subset of cases from the Wells et al. (2011) study of the impact of photo array presentation
methods on the pick types (suspect pick, no pick, filler pick) made by victims and witnesses.
However, the design of this experiment allowed us to examine a range of other questions
relevant to the impact of photo arrays on key criminal justice decision makers (police
investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges), and thereby add to what we, as
eyewitness id researchers, can contribute to the public policy.
Therefore, there were four goals of this experiment: a) to conduct a qualitative case-bycase review of The Evidentiary Strength Study (reported in the previous chapter) to examine
anomalies and outliers (i.e. cases in which the evaluators with the photo array information gave
scores inconsistent with the pick types);41 b) to determine if decisions made by victims and/or
witnesses in photo array procedures had a biasing effect on key criminal justice decision makers’
interpretations of evidentiary strength; b) to assess the added benefit of photo arrays, if any, on
interpretation of evidentiary strength by these decision makers; and d) to evaluate whether police
investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges may differ in how they interpret the
relative strength of various types evidence. Therefore, the basic research questions in this study
are as follow:
1. Were the pick types from the Wells et al. (2011) study seemingly accurate given the
ratings of evidentiary strength?
2. In what way do suspect picks affect the ratings of evidentiary strength, if at all?
41
These data anomalies triggered more in-depth examinations of the possible explanations for score differences as a
means for validating the accuracy of the pick types and adjudication decisions from the Wells et al. (2011) study.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
56
Police Foundation
3. Does knowledge that a suspect was picked lead to biases in interpreting other case
evidence?
4. Do police investigators, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges differ in their
evaluations of evidence in criminal cases, and if so, how?
Method
The methods used for this experimental study were precisely the same as those used for
the Evidentiary Strength Study, but with the inclusion of an experimental component. That is,
the site selection, case selection, participants, training, oversight, and consensus process were
almost the same as those from that study (which was, for all intents and purposes, embedded
within this experiment) with some exceptions. For example, the experiment required that two
teams of raters comprised of one each of the respective evaluator groups (e.g. police, prosecutor,
etc.) were assigned the same cases in different workrooms and sometimes on different days.
However, the two experimental conditions were informally counterbalanced within each team on
any given day so as to shield the groups from the purposes of the study.
The teams of case evaluators were provided with case files stripped of case dispositions,
and other necessary data, so as not to influence their determination of the case strength. Also,
cases rated with the photo array did not include information about whether the case was
presented sequentially or simultaneously. Specifically, researchers ensured that all participants
rating “yes” cases (with photo array) regardless of presentation method, included just one page in
the case file with the six photos presented vertically. This page indicated the actual suspect, and
the one picked (if any) so as not to give away that a procedure was sequential or simultaneous.42
These cases also included the officers’ full reports about the photo array procedure with
42When a photo array was presented simultaneously in Austin, the case file included the actual picture of the photo
array with suspects shown across two rows horizontally. For a sequential procedure, however, there were six
separate photos. The photo array software, however, provided a report for the police file, which was the same for
either condition showing six photos presented vertically and indicating the actual and selected photo, if any.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
57
Police Foundation
information such as why the photo array was run, and if there were multiple photo lineups,
whereas that information was redacted from the other group.
Procedure
All of the photo array cases from the “Evidentiary Strength Study” (see preceding chapter) were
assigned to two groups as follows:
1.
The first group was provided with the cases inclusive of the photo array and
associated pick type (but not the presentation method). This group’s data
served as the basis for the “Evidentiary Strength Study” described earlier in
this report.
2.
The second group examined the same cases, however, photo array information
was redacted from the case (including case details about the photo array, the
photo array printout and associated pick types).
Because all of the cases were assigned to both groups (exact matches of the cases, except for the
manipulated variable “photo array information”), random assignment was unnecessary.
Alternatively we randomly assigned both types of cases (those with the photo array and result
and those without the photo array and result) to two groups, whose members changed each day
based on their availability to participate that day. We believed this approach was both
appropriate and necessary as these groups (hereafter referred to as “teams”) would likely have
been keyed-in to the subject matter of the study had they examined only cases with id
information or without id information on any particular day, or across the entire sample of cases.
Had one group received all of the cases with the photo arrays redacted, the participants would
have – no doubt – questioned why none of their cases had photo arrays. Similarly had one group
always received the photo array cases, they would likely be on-alert that the purpose of the study
had to do with photo arrays, as in real world settings, various actors in the criminal justice
regularly see cases where the case facts and procedures vary from case to case.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
58
Police Foundation
Part A:
Experimental Results
In this section, we present the findings of our experiment in which we examined
differences in evidentiary strength ratings both for the overall case, and for each specific
category of evidence (six total). The results are based primarily on comparisons of the mean
evidentiary strength ratings for the two experimental groups by both presentation method and
pick types. The statistical tests indicate the probability of obtaining a mean difference between
the two groups by presentation methods and pick types. Our alpha level for rejection of the null
hypothesis was set at p < .05. Missing data were excluded from the analysis on a case-by-case
basis, so our n for any statistical tests includes all of the valid cases in the dataset. The analytic
approach used was an analysis of variance (ANOVA) as a test of statistical significance that
assesses whether differences in the means of the groups can lead us to reject the null hypothesis
which assumes that the means of the population from which they are drawn are the same.
Throughout our discussion of the findings of this study, we present eta squared (η2)—the
proportion of the total sums of squares that is accounted for by the between sums of squares—as
the effect size to measure the magnitude of the differences. We relied on Cohen’s (1988) criteria
for interpreting the magnitude of the effects as small (η2 = 0.01), medium (η2 = 0.059), and large
(η2 = 0.138).
When comparing those criminal justice decision makers that had information about the
photo array and its outcome to those who did not, the overall evidentiary strength ratings are not
significantly different as shown in Table 9. While it appears that having knowledge of a suspect
pick results in a slightly higher rating than those without, that difference was not statistically
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
59
Police Foundation
Table 9. Mean Ratings of Overall Evidentiary Strength by Knowledge of Photo Array by Pick
Type
Photo array decision by Knowledge of
victim/witness
photo array used
and outcome
across rater
types
No knowledge of
photo array across
all rater types
Finding and
significance
No pick made
2.81
2.85
n.s
Filler pick made
2.75
2.87
n.s
Suspect pick made
3.93
3.64
n.s
significant and therefore not meaningful43. Indeed, it appears that the suspect-pick cases were
stronger than the other pick types at the outset. Therefore, it is appropriate to conclude that other
information is responsible for differences in outcomes, not specifically the fact that the suspects
were picked. Similarly, there are no significant differences across raters from both conditions for
filler picks or no picks, thereby not significantly hindering the case. These findings suggest that
having a photo array in a case (despite the pick type made) does not add anything significant to
the interpretation of the overall evidentiary strength than had no lineup been run at all.
Furthermore, as also shown in the Evidentiary Strength Study (described earlier in this
report), cases with suspect picks have higher evidentiary strength ratings than those in which
fillers were picked or no picks were made. While one may have drawn conclusions that those
higher scores resulted from the inclusion of the suspect pick, the results from this experimental
analysis disprove that hypothesis; indeed, we found that the higher evidentiary strength ratings
were present despite the suspect picks. As such, the inclusion of positive photo array results
43
This finding means that while the mean difference in the rating is one that is so small and could likely be
attributed to chance.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
60
Police Foundation
indicates that suspect picks do not add anything substantive to the case in the eyes of key
criminal justice decision makers.
Influence of Photo Arrays on Evidentiary Strength Ratings for Specific Types of Evidence
The following series of analyses explore the extent to which ratings of any one category
of evidence are influenced by knowledge of a suspect pick (as well as a filler pick or no pick) in
the photo array, by comparing those evaluators who had the array information to those who did
not.
Physical evidence. As shown in Table 10, the physical evidence ratings are not
substantially improved by having a photo array. Additionally, our results indicate that cases in
which suspects were picked were associated with stronger physical evidence at the outset, as is
demonstrated by the fact that those evaluators not knowing about the suspect picks, rated those
cases higher than the cases with no picks or filler picks and the fact that the ratings between the
two groups did not significantly differ for suspect picks. We can also conclude that when
suspects are picked in photo arrays, that finding does not result in a bias in the interpretation of
the physical evidence.
Table 10. Mean Ratings for Strength of Physical Evidence by Knowledge of Photo Array
within Pick Types
Photo array decision by
victim/witness
Knowledge of
photo array used
and outcome
across all rater
types
No pick made
2.43
2.51
n.s.
Filler pick made
2.30
2.51
n.s.
Suspect pick made
3.27
3.09
n.s
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
No knowledge of
photo array across
all rater types
61
Finding and
significance
Police Foundation
Identification Information. The across group means of strength of identification
information evidence ratings for raters with and without photo array data are presented in Table
11. Again, there were no significant differences across the experimental conditions, indicating
Table 11. Mean Evidentiary Strength Ratings for Identification Information by Knowledge of
Photo Array
Photo array decision by
victim/witness
Knowledge of
photo array used
and outcome
across all rater
types (1 - 5)
No knowledge of
photo array across
all rater types (1 - 5)
Finding and
significance
No pick made
2.75
2.91
n.s.
Filler pick made
2.75
2.94
n.s.
Suspect pick made
3.87
3.62
n.s
that even though photo arrays, especially those with suspect picks, are a subset of the category of
identification information, they do not seem to add anything meaningful to the interpretation of
that category of evidence. Importantly, while people tend to think that the pick of a suspect from
a lineup is the identification information in the case, the other identifying information in these
cases with suspect picks accounts for the higher scores; i.e., strong identification information was
present despite inclusion of a photo array (3.62 for cases with suspect picks versus 2.91 and 2.94
respectively for no picks and filler picks). These findings are consistent with the physical
evidence findings, indicating no incremental benefit of including photo arrays, even when the
suspect is picked in the case. Again there are many forms of identification information linking
suspects to crimes, without a need for a photo array.
Suspect Statement Information. Knowledge of the photo array, even when suspects
were picked, did not result in higher ratings of the suspect statement (see Table 12), indicating no
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
62
Police Foundation
Table 12. Mean Ratings of Strength of Suspect Statement by Knowledge of Pick Type
Photo array decision by
victim/witness
Knowledge of
No knowledge of
photo array used photo array across
and outcome
all rater types (1 - 5)
across all rater
types ( 1- 5)
Finding and
significance
No pick made
2.92
3.00
n.s
Filler pick made
2.56
2.97
n.s.
Suspect pick made
3.18
3.23
n.s.
biasing effect of photo arrays on interpretation of suspect statements. This finding also indicates
that key criminal justice decision makers evaluate suspect statements independently; indeed,
there is no meaningful difference in case ratings based on the pick types or knowledge of the
photo array.
Suspect history. As shown in Table 13, there were also no significant differences across
the two experimental groups when considering the evidentiary strength ratings for suspect
history. Again, it appears that there is no biasing effect of suspect picks on the interpretation of
suspect statements. This finding suggests that key criminal justice decision makers evaluate
Table 13. Mean Ratings of Strength of Suspect History by Knowledge of Pick Type
Photo array decision by
victim/witness
Knowledge of
photo array used
and outcome
across all rater
types ( 1- 5)
No knowledge of
photo array across
all rater types (1 - 5)
No pick made
2.68
2.78
n.s.
Filler pick made
2.76
2.75
n.s.
Suspect pick made
3.21
3.12
n.s.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
63
Finding and
significance
Police Foundation
suspect histories independently and that there is virtually no difference in them based on the pick
types made or the knowledge of the array.
Victim characteristics. As shown in Table 14, there are no differences across the
experimental groups with regard to evidentiary strength ratings for victim characteristics (e.g.
credibility, etc.), even when a suspect is picked, indicating no biasing effect on the interpretation
of victim credibility, etc. This finding also indicates that key criminal justice decision makers
evaluate victim characteristics independent of pick types. There also do not appear to be
substantively higher scores for victim characteristics in the cases in which suspects were picked
versus those in which no picks were made or fillers were picked.
Table 14. Mean Evidentiary Strength Ratings for Victim Characteristics by Knowledge of
Photo Array within Pick Type
Pick Type
Knowledge of
photo array used
and outcome
across all rater
types ( 1- 5)
No knowledge of
photo array across
all rater types (1 - 5)
Finding and
significance
No pick made
3.25
3.39
n.s.
Filler pick made
3.42
3.51
n.s.
Suspect pick made
3.64
3.55
n.s.
Witness characteristics. Finally, as shown in Table 15, there were no observed
differences across ratings among those with or without photo information with regard to the
ratings of witness characteristics; therefore, knowledge of photo array outcomes do not seem to
impact interpretations of witness characteristics suggesting that evaluators were able to
independently evaluate witnesses. It also suggests that knowledge of suspect picks does not bias
the interpretation of witness characteristics. As with victim characteristics, there also do not
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
64
Police Foundation
appear to be substantively higher scores for witness characteristics for the cases in which
suspects were picked versus when no picks were made or fillers were picked.
Table 15. Mean Ratings of Witness Characteristics by Knowledge of Photo Array by Pick
Type
Photo array decision by
victim/witness
Knowledge of
No knowledge of
photo array used photo array across
and outcome
all rater types (1 - 5)
across all rater
types (1- 5)
Finding and
significance
No pick made
3.29
3.36
n.s.
Filler pick made
3.41
3.41
n.s.
Suspect pick made
3.61
3.49
n.s.
Part B:
Impact of Photo Array Knowledge on Evidentiary Strength by Case Dispositions
Among the not prosecuted cases, the evidentiary strength ratings did not vary when
evaluators were provided with photo arrays and pick types made (see Table 16). This suggests
suspect picks did not improve particularly weak cases, nor did the no-pick or filler-pick cases
further diminish an already weak case.
Table 16.
Overall Evidentiary Strength Ratings by Knowledge of Lineup Decision for Cases
Not Prosecuted
Photo array decision by
victim/witness
Knowledge of
photo array
across all rater
types (1- 5)
No knowledge of
photo array across
all rater types (1 - 5)
Finding and
significance
No pick made
2.03
2.04
n.s
Filler pick made
2.34
2.56
n.s.
Suspect pick made
3.14
2.90
n.s.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
65
Police Foundation
Within the adjudicated guilty cases, however, the evidentiary strength ratings did vary
between treatment conditions (with photo array or not) but only for cases in which suspects were
picked. However, while raters with photo array data gave significantly higher overall
evidentiary strength ratings when suspects were picked as compared to the group that did not
know the suspects were picked (mean = 4.33 vs. 3.99, p ≤ .05), this was only true among those
cases for which the evidence was already particularly strong (see Table 17).
Table 17. Overall Evidentiary Strength Ratings by Knowledge of Lineup Decision for Cases
Adjudicated Guilty
Photo array decision by
victim/witness
Knowledge of
photo array used
and outcome
across all rater
types (1- 5)
No knowledge of
photo array across
all rater types (1 - 5)
Finding and
significance
No pick made
4.38
4.51
n.s
Filler pick made
4.32
4.35
n.s.
Suspect pick made
4.33
3.99
t(64) = 2.275, p ≤ .05
However, when comparing mean ratings of evidentiary strength for the cases which had been
adjudicated guilty or not, there were strong differences as shown in Table 18 below. Across all
cases, regardless of pick type or knowledge of the photo array, the non-prosecuted cases
significantly differed from those in which suspects were adjudicated guilty, suggesting that
police and prosecutors in Austin made the proper decisions with regard to inclusion of the
correct suspects and in terms of prosecuting those in which the cases had a significantly stronger
evidentiary basis, and not prosecuting those for which the overall evidence, even when suspects
were picked, was weak. Ultimately, in this study neither photo array presentation methods nor
pick types differentiated between adjudications or impacted evidentiary strength ratings.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
66
Police Foundation
Table 18. Mean Overall Evidentiary Strength Ratings By Judicial Outcomes within Pick Type
Pick type
With or without
photo array
Adjudicated
Guilty
Not
Adjudicated
t-test and p value
No pick
With photo array
4.38
2.03
t (60) = 11.255
p ≤ .001
No photo array
4.51
2.04
t (60) = 12.623
p ≤ .001
With photo array
4.32
2.34
t (25) = 6.331
p ≤ .001
No photo array
4.35
2.56
t (25) = 5.607
p ≤ .001
With photo array
4.33
3.14
t (45) = 5.407
p ≤ .001
No photo array
3.99
2.90
t (45) = 4.093
p ≤ .001
Filler pick
Suspect pick
Part C:
Qualitative Case Analysis
It was important to this study that we also conduct a qualitative case analysis because the
experimental findings were based on group averages, and therefore cannot tell us about what
may have happened in any individual case. In addition, because errors such as wrongful
convictions may only be present in a small number of cases (in fact we do not and cannot know
the actual rate), we conducted an assessment between the groups with the identifications and
those without to identify any cases in which scores were anomalous with the pick types (e.g. a
case with a suspect pick received a lower score by those who knew the suspect was picked than
for the group who had no information about a photo array).
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
67
Police Foundation
In order to gain a better understanding of the specific case ratings across both treatment
groups, we examined three possible patterns of differences: a) proportions of cases in which the
scores from the group who had the photo array were higher than for those who did not,
regardless of pick type; b) cases in which the scores from the group who had the photo array
were the same as those who did not, regardless of pick type; and c) cases in which the scores
from the group who had the photo array were lower than for those who did not, also regardless of
pick type. This allowed us to identify anomalous data, e.g. a case with a suspect pick in which
those with the photo array actually had lower scores than those who evaluated the case without
any id information. We were then able to conduct a qualitative, case-by-case assessment of the
reasons for score anomalies.
Results
The initial set of results show that there were a number of expected findings, but also a
number of anomalous findings (see Table 19). If knowledge that a suspect was picked
Table 19. Impact of Photo Array on Ratings of Overall Evidentiary Strength across Pick Types
Scores went up
(score with id > score without id)
Scores did not change
(score with id = score without id)
Scores went down
(score with id < score without id)
Total
Suspect
Pick
35 (67%)
Filler
Pick
8 (23%)
No Pick
Total
19 (30%)
62 (41%)
8 (15%)
11 (31%)
14 (22%)
33 (22%)
9 (17%)
16 (46%)
31 (48%)
56 (37%)
35
(23.2%)
64
(42.4%)
151
(100%)
52
(34.4%)
influences the evaluators in terms of the strength of the case evidence, then we would expect the
scores to go up, as they did in two-thirds of the cases with suspect picks. Similarly, we would
expect scores to go down when a filler was picked or no one was picked, which they did in
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
68
Police Foundation
almost half the cases. However, if there was no influence of the picks on the cases, or there was
ambiguity associated with the picks, we would expect the ratings to stay the same, which was the
case in about 22% of cases, especially for filler picks.
Cases with No Score Changes When Photo Array Was Included
In twenty-two percent (22%) of cases in which the photo array and associated
information were provided to the evaluators, that information had no impact on the overall
evidentiary strength rating. Among these cases, 42% were for no pick cases, 33.3% were for
filler picks, and 24% were for suspect picks, indicating that in almost a fourth of cases where
suspects are picked, it does not substantively add to the interpretation of evidentiary strength.
Surprisingly, raters did not reduce the case scores when fillers were picked in one third of the
cases, perhaps indicating that the case evidence was not degraded by the knowledge that a nonsuspect was picked by the victim or witness.
Cases in which Scores Decreased When the Photo Array was Included
In 37% of the cases in which evaluators had knowledge of the id, the scores went down,
primarily when no picks were made or filler picks were made (55% and 29% respectively), as
might be expected. This finding suggests that when lineups are run without success (the suspect
is not picked), key criminal justice decision makers tend to give lower scores than those for
which there was no photo array. This suggests that the inclusion of an unsuccessful photo array
may cause key actors in the criminal justice system to discount the other evidence pointing to the
suspect. Surprisingly, ratings went down in 16% of cases in which a suspect was picked. A
detailed analysis of these nine cases revealed that for most of the cases (n = 7), the reasons for
the score reduction were clear (see below). However, we could not establish a reason for one,
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
69
Police Foundation
and for the other, although the score for the suspect pick went down, both the groups gave this
case a high score (over 4.0 out of 5.0). These explanations are provided below:
1. This was a sequential lineup where the witness/victim picked the suspect, but then
asked to see the photos a second time, and in the second lap, the suspect was not
picked.
2. In two cases, the witness/victim made two picks from the lineup, a suspect and a filler,
but the data were recorded as a suspect picks.
3. A co-conspirator at the scene was identified by the victim, but the victim was not sure
which one of the two had done the shooting.
4. In this case, the victim was described as “slow,” and there was another suspect in the
case.
5. There was a second lineup run for this suspect in which the suspect was not picked.
6. The victim was untruthful and therefore considered unreliable (the report and
statement were inconsistent).
Cases in which Scores Increased When the Photo Array was Included
In just over 40% of cases in which photo arrays were provided to the case evaluators, the
case scores went up, and among those, two-thirds resulted from suspects being picked,
suggesting a strong influence of suspect picks on evaluations of overall case strength.
Surprisingly, cases with no suspect picks accounted for about 30% of the increases in scores; yet,
in about 79% of those cases, researchers could not find an explanation, suggesting that maybe
just having a photo array, regardless of anyone being picked, may lead evaluators to think the
case is stronger than it actually is. However, this assertion should be interpreted with caution
since: a) these are relatively small numbers (n = 15), and b) we cannot determine the reasons for
the increases from the case files. For the remaining 21% (n = 4) where there were explanations of
why the no-picks resulted in higher scores that can be explained as follows:
1) The police report about the lineup procedure included information about the suspect’s
history, thereby accounting for the higher score.
2) While a filler was picked by the victim, it was determined that the victim was being
untruthful, and two co-conspirators were identified by other witnesses in the case.
3) In two cases, there was a second witness who did identify the suspect.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
70
Police Foundation
In thirteen percent (n = 8) of cases in which fillers were picked by witnesses/victims but the
scores went up, we could establish reasonable explanations for 75% (n = 6) of them as follows:
1) In two cases there were secondary lineups in which a suspect was picked, even
though in one the witness/victim was only 50% sure.
2) The witness recognized the suspect as familiar, even though a filler was picked.
3) Another witness picked the other suspect who was also at the scene.
4) The officer noted in the case file about the lineup procedure, that the witness first
picked the suspect but then was not sure and instead selected a filler with uncertainty.
5) Surprisingly, the officer noted that the filler picked by the witness/victim closely
resembled the suspect in the lineup and thus believed that the suspect had been
identified (in the simultaneous condition). Importantly, the prosecution did not move
forward.
Part D:
Differences Among Key Criminal Justice Decision Makers in
Evaluating Evidentiary Strength
One of the purposes of the experimental study was to assess whether ratings of
evidentiary strength would vary across different types of criminal justice practitioners, and
whether that would vary by the independent variable (with photo array and outcome information
or without). We conducted t-tests for the mean evidentiary strength ratings across groups.
Throughout the discussion of findings in this section, we present Cohen’s d as the effect size
representing the magnitude of the differences.44 The criteria for interpreting the magnitude of
the effects are as follow: small effect (d = .20); medium effect (d = .50); and large effect (d
= .80) see Cohen (1988).
For ratings of overall evidentiary strength, there was just one comparison that was
significant as shown in Table 20. When interpreting the overall case strength it appears the
judges had slightly higher evidentiary strength ratings than did those of defense attorneys,
however the effect is considered small. The “no photo array” condition demonstrated no
44 Cohen’s d is the difference between sample means (X1 – X2) divided by the pooled standard deviation.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
71
Police Foundation
differences, so the judges appear slightly more likely to be influenced in their overall ratings
when an id is included in the case.
Table 20. Group Differences in Ratings of Overall Evidentiary Strength
Treatment
Group
With ID
Group 1
Group 2
p value
Effect size
Judge = 3.19
Defense = 3.02
.05
.21 (small)
In examining group differences for physical evidence, when the photo array result is
provided, defense attorneys rate cases weaker than do police investigators or judges as
demonstrated in Table 21. However, when considering cases where no photo array information
is provided, judges rate the physical evidence stronger than do prosecutors.
Table 21: Group Differences in Ratings of Physical Evidence
Treatment
Group
With ID
Without ID
Group 1
Group 2
p value
Effect size
Defense = 2.61
Police = 2.76
≤ .05
.18 (small)
Defense = 2.54
Judges = 2.72
≤ .05
.19 (small)
Judges = 2.85
Prosecutors = 2.63
≤ .01
.28 (small)
In considering differences across rater groups with regard to the strength of the suspect
statement in implicating him/her, police rated that evidence surprisingly weaker when the pick
types were included in the case as compared to both defense attorneys and prosecutors (see Table
22). However when the evaluators examined cases without photo arrays, they did not differ
significantly. Thus when a photo array is provided, defense and prosecutors may be slightly
more likely to evaluate the suspects’ statements as more incriminating.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
72
Police Foundation
Table 22. Group Differences in Ratings of Suspect Statement Evidence
Treatment
Group
With ID
Group 1
Group 2
p value
Effect size
Police = 2.74
Defense = 3.00
≤ .05
.25 (small)
Police = 2.74
Prosecutors = 3.05
≤ .01
.35 (small)
When exploring the strength of the suspects’ histories in being somewhat more
incriminating, judges (with or without photo array information) tend to put more weight on the
suspects’ histories than do prosecutors or defense attorneys, as shown in Table 23.
Table 23. Group Differences in Ratings of Suspect History
Treatment
Group
With ID
Without ID
Group 1
Group 2
p value
Effect size
Judges = 3.01
Defense = 2.66
≤ .001
.36 (small)
Judges = 3.02
Prosecutors = 2.82
≤ .05
.22 (small)
Judges = 3.08
Defense = 2.85
≤ .05
.22 (small)
Judges = 3.11
Prosecutors = 2.82
≤ .001
.31 (small)
With regard to victim characteristics (veracity), only one difference was apparent;
defense attorneys surprisingly gave more credence to victim characteristics as shown in Table 24
below and this was only true in the “no” condition (without a photo array).
Table 24. Group Differences in Ratings of Victim Characteristics
Treatment
Group
Without ID
Group 1
Group 2
p value
Effect size
Defense = 3.53
Police = 3.37
≤ .05
.19 (small)
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
73
Police Foundation
When considering witness characteristics, however, an interesting pattern emerged. As
shown in Table 25, when the photo array decision was provided to prosecutors, they rated the
veracity of the witness as stronger than the police. In contrast, when considering the same case
where no photo array information is provided, the police tended to rate witness characteristics as
Table 25. Group Differences in Ratings of Witness Characteristics
Treatment
Group
With ID
Group 1
Group 2
p value
Effect size
Prosecutors = 3.55
Police = 3.31
≤ .001
.28 (small)
Without ID
Police = 3.36
Judges = 3.52
≤ .05
.25 (small)
Police = 3.33
Defense = 3.48
≤ .05
.17 (small
weaker than the judges or defense attorneys, suggesting possibly that police are less likely to be
convinced by witnesses.
Finally, and perhaps most importantly, when considering the identification information as
a whole, judges appear to consider this type of information more important (see Table 26).
Table 26. Group Differences in Ratings of Identification Information
Treatment
Group
With ID
Group 1
Group 2
p value
Effect size
Judges = 3.18
Police = 3.04
≤ .05
.18 (small)
Without ID
Judges = 3.42
Police = 3.10
≤ .001
.31 (small)
Judges = 3.42
Defense = 3.12
≤ .001
.34 (small)
Judges = 3.42
Prosecutors = 3.20
≤ .01
.25 (small)
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
74
Police Foundation
Indeed, when there is no photo array included in the case, they are more likely than all
three groups to consider the other identifying information as critical, whereas when they have it,
their rating of its strength is lower and there is just a slight difference between them and the
police evaluators.
In sum, there are clearly some differences in the way different criminal justice
practitioner types evaluate and interpret the strength of evidence in the cases. Most notably,
judges appear to differ from the other types of raters (police, prosecutors, and defense attorneys)
in that they tend to rate evidence as stronger. This is only slightly true for the overall case when
compared to the defense (which is not at all surprising). However, there is an across the board
affect with regard to identification information which demonstrates that judges are likely to have
somewhat higher ratings than all other groups when there is no photo array provided. This is
consistent with the earlier finding that judges rank identification information most important in
their evaluations of cases. Higher evidentiary strength ratings for suspect histories were also
common for judges compared to other groups. Perhaps judges become more convinced over time
that past behavior predicts future behavior (which is indeed one of the main tenets of human
behavior asserted by psychologists, based on its predictive strength). However, that impact may
mean judges are more skeptical about the ability of individuals to change or less willing to
consider each case’s full merits when they are aware of a suspect history. It is important to note
here that suspect histories as defined in this study consist not only of criminal history, but could
include gang affiliation that may, in itself, be biasing. Judges also rate physical evidence higher
when no photo information is included in cases, suggesting that perhaps they revert to physical
evidence in absence of information about the pick type.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
75
Police Foundation
Other interesting findings include that when photo array pick types are present,
prosecutors rate the veracity of witnesses more strongly than do police or defense attorneys.
Perhaps, this is logical due to the general skepticism held by police, and that prosecutors benefit
more than others from believing the witnesses. Another suggestive finding is that police and
judges seem to be more convinced by the physical evidence when a photo array is present than
are defense attorneys, probably because defense attorneys generally benefit their clients more
from knowing that photo arrays are unreliable, and are therefore less likely to allow a suspect
pick (e.g. to increase their beliefs about the physical evidence). Police officers also appear more
skeptical with regard to the evidentiary value of suspect statements and witness characteristics
(when no id is made), although not as much where victims are concerned (they did have a
slightly lower score of victim characteristics than compared to defense), suggesting that perhaps
police are skeptical when it comes to witnesses (likely based on experience), but not so much
where victims are involved (as they are the ones for whom all members of the criminal justice
system are working, despite the fact that the system itself does not always work to benefit them).
Limitations of the Present Study
The present study had a number of limitations as described. First and foremost, while the
number of cases from the AJS Field Studies was substantial, the number of lineups and
associated cases eligible for our study were limited. We chose to use only the ‘pristine’ cases as
characterized by the study authors, so as to control for the effects of non-pristine lineups. Also,
due to the great range of sample sizes across the sites, we selected to only use those from Austin,
so as to minimize variability from potential site differences encountered in AJS’ EWID Field
Studies (Wells, et al., 2011) and furthermore, so as to not over generalize from a sample whose
cases were drawn primarily from one site. Next, due to Texas law, we were not able to examine
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
76
Police Foundation
cases involving juveniles or those in which sexual assaults had occurred. While the resulting
number of lineups (n = 151) was sufficient for our experiment, based on our power analysis, it
was not sufficient for drawing conclusions about the potentially wrongfully convicted. While this
was not a purpose of our study, it did minimize our ability to detect any pattern with regard to
whether suspect picks in Phase I were associated with weak evidence. As a result, we did
conduct a qualitative case analysis to identify anomalies in the cases.
Next, the fact that we restricted our experiment to Austin limits our ability to make any
generalizations to other jurisdictions. This may be particularly important as in Austin, we learned
that it is the policy of the Travis County District Attorney not to proceed with any cases in which
the pick of a suspect from a photo array is the only evidence associated with the case. This may
mean that in other jurisdictions, if this conservative standard is not applied, that the potential for
getting it wrong is likely much greater. Nevertheless, photo arrays did not appear to add to the
evidentiary strength except when suspects were picked in cases that were already particularly
strong.
Similarly, we cannot say with certainty that the evaluators selected for our sample
represent views that may be held by all persons working in the same capacity. While we did have
diversity in our sample, a couple of things stand out as potentially affecting the generalizability
of our findings. First, most of the evaluators we used were either retired or fairly advanced in
their careers. This was necessitated by the fact that we wanted to ensure that the participants had
not worked on these particular cases, and the need for availability on an ongoing basis over a
period of several weeks, something not possible for current full time employed (especially
prosecutors and police). The fact that most of our evaluators were very experienced in their
respective fields may have provided some extra “wisdom” related to potential pitfalls or even
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
77
Police Foundation
foibles of their respective disciplines, and therefore, we cannot assume that same level of
sophistication for lesser experienced police, prosecutors, or defense attorneys. Nevertheless, our
findings may be conservative in terms of “getting it right,” that is making appropriate and
accurate determination of guilt or innocence.
The findings from this study do not allow us to determine whether the pick types from the
photo array influenced the continuation of the case at any particular point in the criminal justice
process; it may be that police are influenced by a suspect pick as a means for investigating the
case, but that it becomes less important as other evidence accumulates. This may imply that if
there is a biasing effect early on, and no other evidence is found, that the police may still feel
strongly about referring it to the prosecutor. Our study used a retrospective approach in
examining case strength after all of the evidence had been accumulated, and therefore we were
not able to address this issue.
Another limitation of the study was that due to the need for multiple evaluators on a
given day, some evaluators who had previously been in a role in the system, different than their
current or most recent one (e.g. a prosecutor who has since become a judge), were asked to “step
into the shoes” of their former role on any given day. While the individuals felt that they could
speak from a different perspective on any given day, there is a possibility that separating out the
totality of their various perspectives may not have been feasible. In particular, those that were
judges were likely to have come from a different role at some point. Nevertheless, we identified
some group differences despite this fact.
Additionally, the police investigators in our study were no longer serving in the capacity
of an investigator (as we were not able to include those still active in the agency in an
investigative role). As such, some of the investigators that had gone on to advanced roles in the
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
78
Police Foundation
agency and retired at those levels, also may not have been fully able to put themselves in the
mindset of “just an investigator” given their new broader perspective. Indeed, the views by
police may have been moderated by their age and experience more than would have been likely
if they were currently in investigative roles. Overall Results and Discussion
The controversy surrounding eyewitness identification procedures has been the subject of
extensive scientific debate going back more than 100 years when Münsterberg’s (1908) treatise,
“On the Witness Stand” suggested the fallibility of eyewitnesses in recalling events, and was met
with stark criticism from John Henry Wigmore,45 as well as his scientific colleagues. In the
1970s, significant eyewitness identification research was initiated and some 40 plus years later,
many controversies can still be found among the research and practices of eyewitness
identification, despite the many contributions of science. Much of that focus has been on system
and estimator variables.
Now in the 21st century, scientific advances have allowed modern society to get to “the
truth” in cases with clear and convincing DNA evidence. This alone has led to hundreds of
formerly convicted persons being exonerated and/or proved innocent despite serving years or
even decades in prisons across the U.S. The focus on exonerating innocent persons should most
certainly continue. Nevertheless, a significant proportion of these original convictions were
based solely or predominantly on eyewitness identifications alone, raising significant concerns
about lineup procedures. There has been extensive research on various aspects of eyewitness
memory, influences on eyewitnesses during the crime, lineup and show-up procedures, and
photo array methods. Indeed, several decades of research have informed our knowledge about
45See
James M. Doyle (2005). True Witness: Cops, Courts, Science and the Battle Against Misidentification. New
York: Palgrave MacMillan.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
79
Police Foundation
the unreliability of eyewitnesses and victims including those variables that cannot be improved
upon by science and have alerted us to the need to improve the reliability of eyewitness
identification and reduce bias in the administration of photo arrays and lineups. Yet surprisingly,
little attention has been paid to the influence of lineups on the interpretation of case strength.
One major focus in the scientific exploration of the accuracy and reliability of eyewitness
identification has been on the presentation methods used in photo arrays; the traditional
simultaneous presentation method, and the sequential method, now adopted in many jurisdictions.
Substantial scientific evidence has mounted on both of these methods; however, a series of
recent issues has resulted in significant controversy over which approach produces more accurate
and reliable results (Mecklenburg, 2006; Mecklenburg, Bailey, & Larson, 2008; Malpass, 2006;
Mickes, Flowe & Wixted, 2012; Steblay, 2011; Steblay, Dysart, Fulero & Lindsay, 2001; Wells,
Steblay, & Dysart, 2011; Wixted & Mickes, 2012).
The recent AJS’ EWID Field Studies demonstrated that the sequential method of
presentation resulted in significantly fewer misidentifications (Wells, et al., 2011), findings
consistent with much of the accumulated evidence from laboratory studies. However, because
the misidentifications were those in which known innocents (fillers or “foils”) were selected, a
key question emerging about this finding is the whether filler picks are representative of the
more consequential error; picking a suspect when the actual perpetrator is not in the lineup.
Practitioners and scientists alike have asserted and acknowledged that fillers picked from lineups
are very unlikely to be prosecuted, as those individual are, with exceedingly few exceptions,
“known innocents.” At the same time, it is important to note that as a result of an increasing
number of cases in which DNA evidence has exonerated individuals erroneously convicted by
eyewitness evidence, or perhaps with other advances and/or increasing public pressure over time,
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
80
Police Foundation
many policy and practice changes have occurred in prosecutors’ offices, as well as police
departments regarding the unreliability of eyewitness id evidence, and its relative importance in
establishing guilt or innocence. This has rendered some agencies less likely to prosecute on the
basis of a victim/witness identification alone or to even put much weight on the identification as
was shown in this study in Austin.
In this study, there were no significant differences in the proportions of cases adjudicated
guilty versus not adjudicated based on the photo array presentation method, indicating that the
presentation method may not matter in terms of case outcomes. Indeed, our study demonstrated
that many other factors influenced the case outcomes.
The observational results revealed a strong association between outcomes of photo array
process (i.e. lineup choices) and case dispositions in that a greater proportion of cases with guilty
findings (68 percent) were associated with suspect picks, as compared to those in which no picks
were made (25 percent) or fillers were picked (29 percent). Nevertheless, the pick types did not
translate to inaccurate outcomes.
In our experimental study, there were no differences in evidentiary strength ratings for
each pick type regardless of whether the photo array was included or not. This finding
demonstrates that the pick types do not influence the interpretations of the overall case evidence
or any of the specific categories of evidence. This means that although the presentation method
leads to the pick type, the pick type does not, in turn, lead to the case disposition; instead, other
factors account for the outcomes. However, there was an exception to this among the
adjudicated cases, where those with the photo array information assigned ratings slightly higher
than did those without the array (4.33 versus 3.99). However, it should be noted that the score
(almost 4 out of 5) still represents a very strong case that would likely also lead to conviction. In
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
81
Police Foundation
essence, there is an impact of suspect picks on outcomes, but only for those cases that were
particularly strong despite the photo array. When evaluators were not provided with information
that a suspect was picked, they rated the case the same as those who were aware that the suspect
was picked. In cases that were weak, the photo array had no bearing (regardless of pick type).
Our study demonstrated a strong distinction among cases with guilty findings versus
those that were not prosecuted with regard to evidentiary strength. The mean evidentiary
strength ratings for cases adjudicated guilty across pick types was 4.34 versus those not
prosecuted of 2.50, suggesting that a case whose evidence rendered it a rating of 3.99 would be
more likely to result in a conviction. Even when suspects were picked, the not-prosecuted cases
averaged 3.14 whereas those adjudicated guilty averaged 4.33.
One of the key findings from this study was that the inclusion of a photo array in a case
did not appear to have a significant influence on the overall ratings of evidentiary strength by key
criminal justice decision makers. Indeed, for the cases in which photo lineup information
(including pick type) was provided to the evaluators (the “yes” experimental condition), the
evidentiary strength ratings were statistically equivalent to those from evaluators to whom no
information about a photo array or its outcomes was provided (the “no” experimental condition).
Surprisingly, this was true even for cases in which a suspect was picked. Furthermore, while
cases in which suspects were picked were associated with higher ratings of overall evidentiary
strength, this was also true in the condition where the knowledge of a suspect pick was hidden
from evaluators. This indicates that the pick of a suspect is not likely to be the source of the
increased ratings of overall case strength; instead, evaluators saw those cases as stronger despite
the inclusion of a photo array. Essentially, this suggests that the police most likely got these
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
82
Police Foundation
suspects right (i.e. had the correct suspects), given the strength of other corroborating evidence in
the cases.
The aforementioned findings suggest that the inclusion of a photo array does not provide
any added benefit to the evidentiary basis for the case (neither strengthening nor weakening it) in
the eyes of police, prosecutors, defense attorneys, or judges than would be provided without a
photo array, both a serendipitous and counter-intuitive finding. When examining the relationship
between pick types and evidentiary strength, there was no demonstrated bias of knowing the pick
type on the ratings of evidentiary strength for no picks or filler picks; means for both groups—
with and without id— were not significantly different, nor were they for suspect picks when the
cases were not adjudicated. However, among adjudicated guilty suspects, the photo arrays
resulting in suspect picks were rated significantly higher (mean of 4.33) than were those without
the photo arrays (3.99), demonstrating a biasing effect of photo arrays when suspects were
picked, but only when the cases were already particularly strong (mean of almost 4 or above on a
5-point scale). This means that for all intent and purposes, suspect picks enhance already strong
cases, but have no meaningful impact on weaker cases (in this case less than 3 on a 5-point
scale).46
This key finding does not imply, however, that photo arrays are not diagnostic among
police as photo arrays may have some investigative importance; indeed, police may use them as
an investigatory tool to help guide their investigations. More research is needed, however, to
examine whether policies or procedures in investigative units specify the need for a documented
46
The mean evidentiary strength ratings for cases adjudicated guilty across pick types were 4.34 vs. 2.50 for those
not prosecuted, a finding that was statistically significant; t(45) = 5.41, p < 001. This suggests that a case with
ratings of 3.99 would be more likely to result in a conviction based on these data. The means across guilty and not
guilty adjudications of 4.34 and 2.50 suggest a 3.99 is more like a 4.34 (.35 mean difference) than a 2.50 (1.49 mean
difference).
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
83
Police Foundation
justification for including a potential suspect/confirmed suspect in a lineup or photo array. In this
study, the researchers observed some cases in which the rationale for the inclusion of a particular
suspect in a photo array was not documented in the case file, and was not readily apparent. This
does not necessarily mean the investigator did not have a reason, simply, that it was not well
justified in the case file. Without a documented justification, it may be that the administration of
an array or lineup is premature; if a suspect is picked, it may lead an investigator in one
particular direction (i.e. put too much weight on the suspect pick, despite the known problems
with reliability of victims, witnesses, and the procedures themselves) while at the same time,
“ruling out” another viable suspect.
Furthermore, the fact that the picks do not provide any incremental value in most cases
does not mean that they would not strengthen the police and prosecutor “stories” in court cases
(heard by juries). No doubt that when a victim in a courtroom points to the suspect being tried
and says, “that was him,” it has a profound effect on the jury (or any observer for that matter) in
favor of the prosecution’s case. Indeed, research with jurors/juries on the role of eyewitness id
information has regularly shown to have significant biasing effects on juries (Bodenhausen,
1990; Chapadelaine & Griffin, 1997; Kerr et al., 2008). The key here is that the drama-induced
impact is not necessarily reflective of ground truth. In other words, the “story” of the case may
be improved when a suspect is picked from a photo array (even when other witnesses or victims
do not pick the suspect or pick a filler) or worsened when fillers are picked or no one is picked
(in favor of the defense). Nevertheless, key decision makers in our study were not necessarily
strongly influenced by the photo array outcomes in interpreting evidence and its strength in
connecting the suspect to the crime.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
84
Police Foundation
Indeed, an anecdotal finding by researchers in this study (during the pilot test in Charlotte
and the study in Austin) was that police, prosecutors, and defense attorneys typically refer to
“cases” as the entirety of the case they would present if heard by a jury, i.e. the evidence, as well
as the context and prosecutor proposed theory and story of the crime and the plausible alternative
explanations offered by the defense, but not necessarily the objective evidence alone. This is the
reality of the adversarial system, but when no courtroom story lines are required (as is the case in
the vast majority of cases that result in plea agreements), it is very important that the key
decision makers are able to interpret the evidence in an objective manner in order to ensure
justice.
We did not find any significant biasing effect of suspect picks on interpretation of other
case evidence, again showing that photo arrays do not impact on how other evidence is
interpreted. This finding also suggests that criminal justice decision makers can sufficiently
separate different types of evidence, lending validity to the “Evidentiary Strength Scale”
(Amendola & Slipka, 2009). This instrument shows promise as tool for prosecutors (and
potentially others), to separate the individual case facts from the context or “story” about the case,
which should lend validity to the accuracy of their interpretations of evidentiary strength in
delivering just outcomes. There was evidence of strong consistency of ratings among the
evaluators in this data set, suggesting that indeed, various evidentiary factors can be assigned
values and relative weights (Amendola, forthcoming).
While it may be surprising that photo array outcomes did not even bias ratings of
“identification information,”47 it does underscore the fact that cases often have a range of
identification information that allows them to connect suspects to the crime, rendering the result
47
“Identification Information” was defined by the participants in the instrument development phase of the project as
“Independent corroboration of information linking the suspect to the particular incident, regardless of source.”
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
85
Police Foundation
of a photo array a relatively unimportant factor among them. These other factors considered in
the identification information category include: a) clothing, tattoo, hairstyle and other
descriptions of perpetrators; b) details of crime obtained through the investigation (e.g. finding a
stolen item on a suspect, etc.); c) witness id information (e.g., detailed account of incident is
given by witness consistent with other evidence, etc.); d) third party/ complainant information
(e.g. pawn shop owner knows suspect and verifies he/she came in with stolen property, third
party statement implicating the suspect, etc.); e) circumstances surrounding arrest (e.g. suspect
hiding near crime scene, etc.); f) co-conspirator flips, thereby implicating suspect; and g)
anonymously provided information.
The aforementioned finding implies that the identification information, other than the
photo array and its result, may be stronger, more reliable, or more important to criminal justice
decision makers, or that the other identification information may simply stand on its own without
the need for a photo array, again, indicating that lineups (at least in Austin) do not add any useful
information to the evidentiary basis for a case. Importantly, all types of decision makers ranked
the identification information among the most important of the six categories of evidence (police
investigators = #2, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges = #1). Similarly, all but judges
ranked the physical evidence among the most important type of evidence48 (police = #1, defense
and prosecutors = #2), although judges seemed to think characteristics of witnesses and victims
were more important than physical evidence.
There were some distinct differences in the way that criminal justice practitioner types
evaluate the evidentiary strength. Judges, for example, rated suspect histories as stronger in
implicating the suspects than did prosecutors or defense attorneys, but not police. It is possible
48
There were six distinct categories of evidence as determined by criminal justice decision makers.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
86
Police Foundation
that judges become more convinced over time that criminal background and/or gang affiliation
of suspects makes them more likely guilty than do other groups and suggests greater skepticism
on their part about the ability of individuals to change. Interestingly, police and judges rate the
physical evidence higher than defense attorneys when a photo array is present. This is probably
because defense attorneys generally benefit their clients more from knowing that photo arrays
are unreliable, and therefore are less likely to, for example, allow a suspect pick to increase their
ratings of physical evidence. And, judges rate the physical evidence more strongly than the
prosecutors when no photo arrays were provided to either group, possibly suggesting the
prosecutors’ are more influenced by photo arrays.
Perhaps, surprisingly, police rated suspect statements lower than did prosecutors or
defense attorneys. When an ID was present, prosecutors rated the witness credibility as higher.
With regard to victims, defense attorneys tended to rate the victim’s credibility as higher than the
police when no photo array information was provided. Perhaps, this may indicate that
prosecutors benefit more than others from believing the witnesses.
Police officers also appear more skeptical with regard to the evidentiary value of suspect
statements than defense or prosecutors. They rated witness characteristics weaker than did judges
or defense attorneys. These findings are may be attributable to their general cultural tendency
toward skepticism.
Conclusion
Given the extensive findings over the past four decades on the unreliability of
eyewitnesses despite the best efforts to minimize errors and improve reliability of administrative
procedures, these findings suggest potentially a different course for the future. The fact that
photo arrays in this study did not add to the cases’ overall interpreted evidentiary strength or the
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
87
Police Foundation
strength of any specific category of evidence, suggests that use of lineup procedures may not
actually increase the ability to detect truth or substantially improve or impair the ratings of
evidentiary strength of the case over what the other case evidence already provides.
Yet, scientists have avoided inquiries that would establish the validity of relying upon
eyewitness identification as an indicator of ground truth, despite the well-established,
aforementioned problems of unreliability and misidentification. Our study provides some reason
for the criminal justice community to question whether or how the use of photo arrays
contributes to meting out justice. In light of the fact that many individuals have been exonerated
by DNA evidence and that the primary cause of the wrongful convictions was the exclusion of
the suspect from the photo array and the ensuing wrong pick, it is important that we consider not
only the relative importance of photo arrays, but also the utility of them in executing justice.
Certainly, many changes have occurred in prosecutors’ offices (and probably many police
departments) regarding the use of eyewitness id evidence over the past few decades, often
requiring significant corroboration of a suspect pick in lineup procedures. In a vast majority of
wrongful conviction cases (where the DNA or other evidence exonerated the suspect), it has
been shown that the identification made by the witness or victim, was the only piece of evidence.
Indeed, the Travis County District Attorney’s office noted in 2012 that ID-only cases do not
provide sufficient justification for prosecution. Assuming this is not likely true in every
jurisdiction, despite today’s knowledge of the fallibility of witness and victim identifications, the
fact that eyewitness misidentifications have led to wrongful convictions should, at a minimum,
raise questions about the use of eyewitness id without other strong corroborating evidence to
accompany it, if not to fully re-examine its use at all as a material factor in cases. The fact that
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
88
Police Foundation
the case dispositions in the Austin cases were largely attributed to other case evidence prevented
the miscarriage of justice in both potential wrongful convictions and failing to convict the guilty.
Future research should attempt to focus on other categories of evidence, particularly
physical and forensic evidence, and the appropriate interpretation of that evidence by key
criminal justice decision makers who are responsible for closing at least 90% of cases (without
juries). One potential implication for police departments is that they continue to explore methods
for improving investigative procedures so as to reduce the emphasis placed on photo arrays,
given their limitations in improving the evidentiary strength of cases. Police agencies should also
train their officers and investigators regarding the limited utility and limitation of lineup
procedures, and encourage the collection of and emphasis on physical evidence, and other forms
of identification information in order to de-emphasize the importance of lineups as critical to a
case. Additionally, police departments may benefit from implementing policies that require clear
documentation (in the case file) of investigators’ justifications for including potential suspects in
lineups and photo arrays so that they are not done prematurely or lead to an overly narrow
investigative focus.
In the two studies we presented here, we conducted a follow-up on the Wells et al. (2011)
study of sequential versus simultaneous presentation methods to determine whether the
presentation methods were associated with differences in quantitative (case dispositions) and
qualitative outcomes (evidentiary strength ratings). However, our results demonstrated that there
is not a relationship between presentation methods and case dispositions or evaluations of
evidentiary strength. This finding is not surprising given the fact that, at least among these cases
in Austin, there were likely many influences on the case dispositions and evidentiary strength not
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
89
Police Foundation
the least of which are appropriately-derived confessions, physical evidence, and many other
forms of identifying information, etc.
Nevertheless, we found that regardless of pick type (suspect, filler, no pick), ratings of
evidentiary strength were significantly higher for those cases adjudicated guilty versus not
prosecuted, suggesting that prosecutors appeared to have made the right decisions.
Observationally, it appeared that suspect picks were associated with higher ratings and
proportions of guilty outcomes; however, our experiment showed for the cases with guilty
findings that included suspect picks, that the cases were particularly strong despite the suspect
picks (those not knowing that a suspect was picked rated the cases very high, despite the fact that
in those cases, the suspect picks may have provided more confidence).
When evaluators did not look at photo arrays and pick types, the ratings for those
adjudicated guilty all averaged above 3.9 on a 5-point scale. However, for those not adjudicated,
average evidentiary strength ratings were below 2.5 for the no pick and filler pick cases, as
compared to 3.14 for suspect picks, suggesting that the overall evidence in those suspect pick
cases was not strong enough to proceed with a prosecution; yet, another indication that the
prosecutors made accurate decisions.
Most importantly, our experiment showed that including a photo array in cases had no
meaningful impact on the evidentiary strength value of the case as a whole, or any particular
category of evidence. As such, at least for those key criminal justice decision makers we relied
upon in Austin, the photo arrays neither biased the interpretation of the other evidence in the
cases, nor made a meaningful difference in how the case was interpreted, suggesting that it did
not add any real value to the case nor to its outcomes. Cases that were particularly strong
appeared to be so, despite the photo array raising the question of the relative importance of photo
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
90
Police Foundation
arrays in getting at truth regarding guilt or innocence. This study did not, however, look at the
biasing effect of lineups on jurors. Because key criminal justice decision makers such as those
included in our study handle about 95% of cases out of court, there is reason to believe that at
least for cases in which plea deals are reached, that photo arrays will have little impact on case
dispositions arrived at without juries. Indeed, the right to have one’s case heard before a “jury of
its peers,” does not mean that the jury will “get it right” either.
Indeed, it appears that police departments, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges are
becoming better versed in the scientific evidence related to eyewitness identification practices,
and will continue to do so. Of course, this is not likely to be the case in all agencies, depending
upon their leadership, political views, or unwillingness to acknowledge the importance of
scientific evidence. It is important to note that while the evaluators in our study were likely
representative of those in the Travis County area, this does not mean that they (or the Travis
County criminal justice system as a whole) is representative of other jurisdictions nationwide.
Therefore it is important that these findings be interpreted within that context.
At the same time, the presence of photo arrays did not appear to have biasing impacts on
case evaluators, suggesting that at least for these evaluators, photo arrays did not take the place
of other strong evidence in prosecuting the cases. These findings together suggest no added
benefit to the evidentiary basis of the case by inclusion of a photo array— indeed a serendipitous
finding. This finding does beg the rarely asked question: Are lineups alone necessary in
identifying “truth” as demanded of our justice system? The fact that today, massive advances in
physical and forensic evidence have occurred, may have contributed to our findings that
identification information in a case tends to stand on its own without the need for corroboration
from a witness or victim picking a suspect out of a lineup.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
91
Police Foundation
In conducting a qualitative case analysis we did reveal some important anomalies.
Specifically, our quantitative case analysis revealed few anomalies with regard to cases being
strengthened by suspect picks. Importantly, in about 30% of cases where no suspect was picked
and over 20% of cases in which a filler was picked, evaluators increased their ratings of
evidentiary strength. While in some cases, there were good explanations for these changes
(especially for the filler picks), in most of the cases it seems that just the inclusion of a photo
array, regardless of its result, led case evaluators to increase their ratings. Nevertheless, as
shown in the experimental findings, these increases were not statistically meaningful in terms of
their magnitude, yet it does speak to some of the idiosyncrasies associated with photo arrays.
Finally, where knowledge about errors in eyewitness procedures and memory are well
established, these findings do not really add to those facts. Instead, in considering actual cases in
the field, it appears that many of these problems were mitigated by sound law enforcement and
prosecutorial practices with regard to the limited importance of photo arrays in moving cases
forward. Our findings do, however, suggest a re-consideration of the benefit(s), if any, lineups
provide over and above the more objective case evidence, as our study showed no added benefit
beyond the other evidence. It is perhaps true that lineups may assist officers in facilitating an
investigation, but in some cases that assistance may lead to tunnel vision with regard to a
particular suspect thereby eliminating the actual perpetrator (although that did not appear to be
true in any of the cases we evaluated in Austin). It is also clear that lineups add a dramatic effect
in courtrooms, and that they may strengthen the theories of both the prosecution or defense (due
to the adversarial process), but perhaps not in terms of ground truth as to actual guilt or
innocence, thereby underscoring the need for corroboration when relying on lineups and photo
arrays in cases to ensure justice.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
92
Police Foundation
Nevertheless, dispositions in Travis County, Texas were supported by evidentiary
strength ratings; indeed, while there may certainly be influences on dispositions other than truth,
the system as a whole seemed to get it right in Austin. Importantly, evaluators in this study were
encouraged to consider all evidence present in the cases, even if it were not to be admissible in a
court proceeding, thereby adding to the validity of the Strength of Evidence Scale as a closer
proxy of ground truth.
The results of this study are particularly meaningful for a number of reasons. Firstly, the
fact that 90 – 95% of cases are settled through plea agreements rather than through jury trials,
suggests that these key decision makers are responsible for interpreting evidence and in
facilitating justice, this study attempted to consider their interpretation of evidentiary strength as
considerably more important than the almost exclusive focus on jury decision making in past
research. Indeed, these criminal justice decision makers account for the majority of criminal
justice outcome decisions in the system. Secondly, the fact that these evaluators were trained in
this experiment to rate evidence in a singular category, in order to avoid biasing the
interpretation of other evidence, and were “checked” in this process by other team members
during consensus discussions regarding individual ratings, suggests that evaluators were able to
separate case facts from context in order to arrive at more scientifically sound decisions.
Thirdly, all types of decision makers (regardless of having the photo id information or
not) ranked the identification information among the most important of the evidentiary categories,
however, our findings suggested that the identification information stood on its own without the
addition of the photo arrays, suggesting limits to their utility. Similarly, all but judges ranked the
physical evidence among the two most important, whereas judges seemed to think that
characteristics of witnesses and victims were more important than physical evidence. Finally,
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
93
Police Foundation
these findings are important because little attention has been paid to how prosecutors and others
consider evidence in their decisions and findings. Whereas research with juries on the role of
eyewitness id information has regularly been shown to have significant biasing effects on juries
as well (Bodenhausen, 1990; Chapadelaine & Griffin, 1997; Kerr et al., 2008) the same was not
true for the police, prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges in our study.
Recommendations for Practice and Future Research
Given the extensive findings over the past four decades on the unreliability of
eyewitnesses, despite the best efforts to minimize errors and improve reliability of administrative
procedures, these findings potentially suggest a different course for the future. Future research
should attempt to focus on other categories of evidence, particularly, physical and forensic
evidence, and the appropriate interpretation of that evidence by key criminal justice decision
makers who are responsible for closing at least 90% of cases (without juries). One potential
implication for police departments is that they continue to explore methods for improving
investigative procedures so as to reduce the emphasis placed on photo arrays, given their
limitations in improving the evidentiary strength of cases. Police agencies should also train their
officers and investigators regarding the limited utility and limitation of lineup procedures, and
encourage the collection of and emphasis on physical evidence and other forms of identification
information in order to de-emphasize the importance of lineups as critical to a case. Additionally,
police departments may benefit from implementing policies that require clear documentation (in
the case file) of investigators’ justifications for including potential suspects in lineups and photo
arrays so that they are not done prematurely or lead to an overly narrow investigative focus.
Finally, law enforcement personnel should [continue to] emphasize corroboration when relying
on photo arrays or lineups in criminal cases.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
94
Police Foundation
References
Adams, K. (1983). The effect of evidentiary factors on charge reduction. Journal of Criminal
Justice, 11, 525-537. doi:10.1016/0047-2352(83)90005-3
Alderden, M.A., & Ullman, S.E. (2012). Creating a more complete and current picture:
Examining police and prosecutor decision-making when processing sexual assault cases.
Violence Against Women, 18(5), 525-551. doi:10.1177/1077801212453867
Amendola, K.L. (2014). The development of an instrument to rate evidentiary strength in
criminal cases. Unpublished manuscript, Police Foundation, Washington D.C.
Amendola, K.L., & Slipka, M.G. (2009). Strength of evidence scale. Unpublished instrument,
Police Foundation, Washington, DC. Contained in Appendix A of this report.
Behrman, B.W., & Davey, S.L. (2001). Eyewitness identification in actual criminal cases: An
archival analysis. Law and Human Behavior, 25(5), 475-491. doi:
10.1023/A:1012840831846
Benton, T.R., Ross, D.F., Bradshaw, E., Thomas, W.N., & Bradshaw, G.S. (2006). Eyewitness
memory is still not common sense: Comparing jurors, judges and law enforcement to
eyewitness experts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(1), 115-129.
doi:10.1002/acp.1171
Bodenhausen, G.V. (1990). Second-guessing the jury: Stereotypic and hindsight biases in
perceptions of court cases. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20(13), 1112-1121.
doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1990.tb00394.x
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
95
Police Foundation
Boyce, M.A., Lindsay, D.S., & Brimacombe, C.A.E. (2008). Investigating investigators:
Examining the impact of eyewitness identification evidence on student-investigators.
Law and Human Behavior, 32(5), 439-453. doi:10.1007/s10979-007-9125-5
Bushway, S.D., & Redlich, A.D. (2012). Is plea bargaining in the “shadow of the trial” a
mirage? Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 28(3) 437-454. doi:10.1007/s10940-0119147-5.
Carlson, C.A. (2008). Distinctiveness in an eyewitness identification paradigm: Comparing
simultaneous and sequential lineups. (Doctoral Dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest
Dissertations and Theses. Retrieved from
http://gradworks.umi.com/33/15/3315570.html
Chae, Y. (2010). Application of laboratory research on eyewitness testimony. Journal of
Forensic Psychology Practice, 10(3), 252-261. doi:10.1080/15228930903550608
Chapdelaine, A., & Griffin, S.F. (1997). Beliefs of guilt and recommended sentence as a function
of juror bias in the O. J. Simpson trial. Journal of Social Issues, 53(3), 477-485.
doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1997.tb02123.x
Clark, S.E. (2012). Costs and benefits of eyewitness identification reform psychological science
and public policy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(3), 238-259.
doi:10.1177/1745691612439584
Clark, S.E., & Davey, S.L. (2005). The target-to-foils shift in simultaneous and sequential
lineups. Law and Human Behavior, 29(2), 151-172. doi:10.1007/s10979-005-2418-7
Clark, S.E., & Tunnicliff, J.L. (2001). Selecting lineup foils in eyewitness identification
experiments: Experimental control and real-world simulation. Law and Human
Behavior, 25(3), 199-216. doi: 10.1023/A:1010753809988
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
96
Police Foundation
Clifford, B.R., & Scott, J. (1978). Individual and situational factors in eyewitness testimony.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 63(3), 352-359. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.63.3.352
Committee on Identifying the Needs of the Forensic Science Community, National Research
Council. (2009). Strengthening forensic science in the United States: A path forward.
(Report No. NCJ 228091). Retrieved from:
https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/228091.pdf
Connors, E., Lundregan, T., Miller, N., & McEwen, T. (1996). Convicted by juries, exonerated
by science: Case studies in the use of DNA evidence to establish innocence after trial.
(Report No. NCJ 161258). Washington DC: National Institute of Justice. Retrieved
from: https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles/dnaevid.pdf
Cowdery, N. (2005). Wrongful conviction and double jeopardy. Judicial Officers' Bulletin, 17(4),
27-30.
Cutler, B.L, Penrod, S.D., & Martens, T.K. (1987). The reliability of eyewitness identification:
The role of system and estimator variables. Law and Human Behavior, 11(3), 233-258.
doi:10.1007/BF01044644
Cutler, B.L., & Bull Kovera, M. (2008). Introduction to commentaries on the Illinois pilot Study
of lineup reforms. Law and Human Behavior, 32(1), 1-2. doi:10.1007/s10979-0079120-x
Deffenbacher, K.A., Bornstein, B.H., Penrod, S.D., & McGorty, E.K., (2004). A meta-analytic
review of the effects of high stress on eyewitness memory. Law and Human Behavior,
28(6), 687-706. doi:10.1007/s10979-004-0565-x
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
97
Police Foundation
Devenport, J.L., Penrod, S.D., & Cutler, B.L. (1997). Eyewitness identification evidence:
Evaluation commonsense evaluations. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 3(2-3),
338-361. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.3.2-3.338
Doyle, J.M. (2005). True witness: Cops, courts, science, and the battle against
misidentification. New York, New York: Palgrave MacMillan.
Durose, M.R., & Langan, P.A. (2003). Felony Sentences in state courts, 2000. (Report No. NCJ
198821). Retrieved from Bureau of Justice Statistics website:
http://bjs.gov/content/pub/pdf/fssc00.pdf
Espinoza, R.K.E., & Willis-Esqueda, C. (2008). Defendant and defense attorney characteristics
and their effects on juror decision-making and prejudice against Mexican Americans.
Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14(4), 364-371.
doi:10.1037/a0012767
Eyewitness misidentification. (n.d.). In The Innocence Project’s understanding the causes (2014).
Retrieved from http://www.innocenceproject.org/understand/EyewitnessMisidentification.php
Finklea, K.M., & Ebbesen, E.B., (2007). Eyewitness accuracy in the real world: DNA evidence
as “ground truth” for eyewitness accuracy rates. Unpublished manuscript.
Frederick, B., & Stemen, D. (2012). The anatomy of discretion: An analysis of decision making technical report (Report for Award No. 2009-IJ-CX-0040). Washington DC: Vera
Institute of Justice. Retrieved from:
https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/240334.pdf
Freedman, M.H. (1966). Professional responsibility of the criminal defense lawyer: The three
hardest questions. The Michigan Law Review Association, 64(8), 1469-1484.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
98
Police Foundation
Gardner, T.J. & Anderson, T.M. (7th Ed.). (2012). Criminal evidence: Principles and cases. Belmont, California: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning.
Garner, J. H. & Maxwell, C.D. (2009). Prosecution and conviction rates for intimate
partner violence. Criminal Justice Review 34(1), 44-79. doi:10.1177/0734016808324231
Garrett, B. (2008). Judging innocence. Columbia Law Review, 108(1), 55-142.
Glater, J.D., (2008, August 8). Study finds settling is better than going to trial. The New York
Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2008/08/08/business/08law.html
Gould, J.B., Carrano, J., Leo, R., & Young, J. (2012). Predicting erroneous convictions: A social
science approach to miscarriages of justice. (Report for Award No. 2009-IJ-CX-4110).
Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Retrieved from:
http://observer.american.edu/spa/djls/prevent/upload/Predicting-ErroneousConvictions.pdf
Greathouse, S.M., & Bull Kovera, M. (2009). Instruction bias and lineup presentation moderate
the effects of administrator knowledge on eyewitness identification. Law and Human
Behavior, 33(1), 70-82. doi:10.1007/s10979-008-9136-x
Gross, S.R., & Shaffer, M. (2012). Exonerations in the United States, 1989-2012. (Research
Report). Retrieved from the National Registry of Exonerations website:
https://www.law.umich.edu/special/exoneration/Documents/exonerations_us_1989_201
2_full_report.pdf
Hedding, N. (2002). The fine line between strategic miscalculation and harmful error:
Consequences and repercussions of legal malpractice to the criminal defense attorney.
Journal of Legal Advocacy and Practice, 4, 222-234.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
99
Police Foundation
Jacoby, J.E., Mellon, L.R., Ratledge, E.C., and Turner, S.T. (1982). Prosecutorial decisionmaking: A national study. (Report for Award No. 79-NI-AX-0006 and 79-NI-AX0034). Washington DC: National Institute of Justice. Retrieved from
http://www.jijs.org/publications/prospubs/Decisionmaking.pdf
Kassin, S.M., Dror, I.E., & Kukucka, J. (2013). The forensic confirmation bias: Problems,
perspectives, and proposed solutions. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
Cognition, 2(1), 42-52. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2013.01.001
Kassin, S.M., Reddy, M.E., & Tulloch, W.F. (1990). Juror interpretations of ambiguous
evidence: The need for cognition, presentation order, and persuasion. Law and Human
Behavior, 14(1), 43-55. doi:10.1007/BF01055788
Kerr, N.L., Boster, F., Callen, C.R., Braz, M.E., O’Brien, B., & Horowitz, I. (2008). Jury
nullification instructions as amplifiers of bias. International Commentary on Evidence,
6(1), 1554-4567. doi: 10.2202/1554-4567.1068
Kilpatrick, D.G., Resnick, H.S., Ruggiero, K.J., Conoscenti, L.M., & McCauley, J. Drugfacilitated, incapacitated, and forcible rape: A national study. (Research Report for
Award No. 2005-WG-BX-0006). Charleston, South Carolina: National Crime Victims
Research & Treatment Center. Retrieved from
http://www.niccsa.org/downloads/elders/DRUGFACILITATEDINCAPACITATEDAN
DFORCIBLERAPE.pdf
Konecni, V.J., & Ebbesen, E.B. (1986). Courtroom testimony by psychologists on eyewitness
identification issues: Critical notes and reflections. Law and Human Behavior, 10(1-2),
117-126. doi:10.1007/BF01044563
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
100
Police Foundation
LaFree, G. (1989). Rape and criminal justice: The social construction of sexual assault, Belmont,
California: Wadsworth.
Lindholm, T. (2008). Who can judge the accuracy of eyewitness statements? A comparison of
professionals and lay-persons. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22(9), 1301-1314.
doi:10.1002/acp.1439
Lindsay, R.C., & Wells, G.L. (1985). Improving eyewitness identifications from lineups:
Simultaneous versus sequential lineup presentation. Journal of Applied Psychology,
70(3), 556-564. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.70.3.556
Loftus, E.F. (1975). Leading questions and the eyewitness report. Cognitive Psychology, 7(4),
560-72. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(75)90023-7
Loftus, E.F., Miller, D.G., & Burns, H.J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into
a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory,
4(1), 19-31. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.4.1.19
Loftus, E.F., & Palmer, J.C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of
the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 13(5), 585-589. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80011-3
Magnussen, S., Melinder, A., Stridbeck, U., & Raja, A.Q. (2010). Beliefs about factors affecting
the reliability of eyewitness testimony: A comparison of judges, jurors and the general
public. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 122-133. doi: 10.1002/acp.1550
Maguire, K., & Pastore, A.L. (2003). Sourcebook of Criminal Justice Statistics: 2002. (Report
No. NCJ 203301). Washington DC: Bureau of Justice Assistance. Retrieved from:
https://www.hsdl.org/?view&did=711164
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
101
Police Foundation
Malpass, R.S. (2006). A policy evaluation of simultaneous and sequential lineups. Psychology,
Public Policy, and Law, 12(4), 394-418. doi: 10.1037/1076-8971.12.4.394
Malpass, R.S., & Devine, P.G. (1981). Eyewitness identification: Lineup instructions and the
absence of the offender. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66(4), 482-489. doi:
10.1037/0021-9010.66.4.482
McCloskey, M., & Egeth, H.E. (1983). Eyewitness identification: What can a psychologist tell a
jury? American Psychologist, 38(5), 550-563. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.38.5.550
McKenna, J.D., Treadway, M., & McCloskey, M.E. (1992). Expert psychological testimony on
eyewitness reliability: Selling psychology before its time. In P. Suedfeld & P.E. Tetlock
(Eds.), Psychology and social policy (pp. 283-293). New York, New York: Hemisphere
Publishing Cooperation.
Mecklenberg, S.H. (2006). The Illinois pilot program on sequential double-blind identification
procedures. Report submitted to the state legislature of the State of Illinois on behalf of
the Illinois State Police, Springfield, Illinois.
Mecklenburg, S.H., Bailey, P.J., & Larson, M.R. (2008). The Illinois field study: A significant
contribution to understanding real world eyewitness identification issues. Law and
Human Behavior, 32(1), 22-27. doi: 10.1007/s10979-007-9108-6
Memon, A., & Gabbert, F. (2003). Unravelling the effects of sequential presentation in culpritpresent lineups. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(6), 703-714. doi: 10.1002/acp.909
Merola, M. (1982). Federal, state and local governments: Partners in the fight against violent
crime. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 73(3), 965-984.
Mickes, L., Flowe, H.D., & Wixted, J.T. (2012). Receiver operating characteristic analysis of
eyewitness memory: Comparing the diagnostic accuracy of simultaneous versus
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
102
Police Foundation
sequential lineups. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18(4), 361-376.
doi:10.1037/a0030609
Münsterberg, H. (1908). On the witness stand. Garden City, New York: Doubleday, Page &
Company.
National Center for Prosecution Management (1974). Report to the Bronx County District
Attorney on the case evaluation system. (Report No. NCJ 029088). Retrieved from:
https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/Digitization/29088NCJRS.pdf
National Institute of Justice (1999). Eyewitness evidence: A guide for law enforcement. (Report
No. NCJ 178240). Retrieved from: https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/178240.pdf
Oppel Jr., R. (2011, September 25). Sentencing shift gives new leverage to prosecutors. The New
York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/09/26/us/tough-sentenceshelp-prosecutors-push-for-plea-bargains.html?pagewanted=all&_r=1&.
Peterson, J.L, Hickman, M.J., Strom, K.J., & Johnson, D.J. (2013). Effect of forensic evidence
on criminal justice case processing. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 58(S1), S78-S90. doi:
10.1111/1556-4029.12020
Peterson, J.L., Ryan, J.P., Houlden, P.J., & Mihajlovic, S. (1987). The uses and effects of
forensic science in the adjudication of felony cases. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 32(6),
1730-1753.
Pozzulo, J.D., Lemieux, J.M.T., Wilson, A., Crescini, C., & Girardi, A. (2009). The influence of
identification decision and DNA evidence on juror decision-making. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 39(9), 2069-2088. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00516.x
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
103
Police Foundation
Pratt, T.C., Gaffney, M.J., Lovrich, N.P., & Johnson, C.L. (2006). This isn't CSI: Estimating the
National backlog of forensic DNA cases and the barriers associated with case processing.
Criminal Justice Policy Review, 17(1), 32-47. doi:10.1177/0887403405278815
Raeder, Myrna (2012). Overturning wrongful convictions and compensating exonerees. In
Springer, Encyclopedia of Criminology and Criminal Justice, (Bruinsma & Weisburd,
eds.).
Samuels, J.E., Davies, E.H., & Pope, D.B. (2013). Collecting DNA at arrest: Policies, practices,
and implications. (Report for Award No. 2009-DN-BX-0004). Washington, DC: The
Urban Institute Justice Policy Center. Retrieved at:
https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/242813.pdf
Schacter, D.L., Dawes, R., Jacoby, L.L., Kahneman, D., Lempert, R., Roediger, H.L., &
Resenthal, R. (2008). Police fourm: Studying eyewitness investigations in the field.
Law and Human Behavior, 32(1), 3-5. doi:10.1007/s10979-007-9093-9
Smith, L.L., & Bull, R. (2012). Identifying and measuring juror pre-trial bias for forensic
evidence: Development and validation of the Forensic Evidence Evaluation Bias Scale.
Psychology, Crime & Law, 18(9), 797-815. doi: 10.1080/1068316X.2011.561800
Smith, L.L., & Bull, R. (2013). Exploring the disclosure of forensic evidence in police interviews
with suspects. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, July, 1-6.
doi: 10.1007/s11896-013-9131-0
Smith, L.L., Bull, R., & Holliday, R. (2011). Understanding juror perceptions of forensic
evidence: Investigating the impact of case context on perceptions of forensic evidence
strength. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 56(2), 409-414. doi: 10.1111/j.15564029.2010.01671.x
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
104
Police Foundation
Sporer, S.L., Penrod, S., Read, D., & Cutler, B. (1995). Choosing, confidence, and accuracy: A
meta-analysis of the confidence-accuracy relation in eyewitness identification studies.
Psychological Bulletin, 118(3), 315-327. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.3.315
Steblay, N.K. (1997). Social Influence in eyewitness recall: A meta-analytic review of lineup
instruction efforts. Law and Human Behavior, 21(3), 283-298. doi:
10.1023/A:1024890732059
Steblay, N.K. (2011). What we know now: The Evanston Illinois field lineups. Law and Human
Behavior, 35(1), 1-12. doi: 10.1007/s10979-009-9207-7
Steblay, N.K., Dysart, J.E., Fulero, S., & Lindsay, R.C.L. (2001). Eyewitness accuracy rates in
sequential and simultaneous lineup presentations: A meta-analytic comparison. Law
and Human Behavior, 25(5), 459-473. doi: 10.1023/A:1012888715007
Steblay, N.K., Dysart, J.E., & Wells, G.L. (2011). Seventy-two tests of the sequential lineup
superiority effect: A meta-analysis and policy discussion. Psychology, Public Policy,
and Law, 17(1), 99-139. doi: 10.1037/a0021650
Tollestrup, P.A., Turtle, J. W., & Yuille, J. C. (1994). Actual victims and witnesses to robbery
and fraud: An archival analysis. In D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, & M. P. Toglia, (Eds.), Adult
eyewitness testimony: Current trends and developments (pp. 144-160). New York City,
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Wagstaff, G.F., MacVeigh, J., Boston, R., Scott, L., Brunas-Wagstaff, J., & Cole, J. (2003). Can
laboratory findings on eyewitness testimony be generalized to the real world? An
archival analysis of the influence of violence, weapon presence, and age on eyewitness
accuracy. Journal of Psychology, 137(1), 17-28.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
105
Police Foundation
Wells, G.L. (1978). Applied eyewitness-testimony research: System variables and estimator
variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(12), 1546-1557. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.36.12.1546
Wells, G.L., Malpass, R., Lindsay, R.C.L., Fisher, R.P., Turtle, J.W., & Fulero, S.M. (2000).
From the lab to the police station: A successful application of eyewitness research.
American Psychologist, 55(6), 581-598. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.6.581
Wells, G.L., Memon, A., & Penrod, S.D. (2006). Eyewitness evidence: Improving its probative
value. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7(2), 45-75. doi: 10.1111/j.15291006.2006.00027.x
Wells, G.L, Small, M., Penrod, S.D., Malpass, R.S., Fulero, S.M., & Brimacombe, C.A.E. (1998).
Eyewitness identification procedures: Recommendations for lineups and photospreads.
Law and Human Behavior, 22(6), 603-607. doi: 10.1023/A:1025750605807
Wells, G.L., Steblay, N.K., & Dysart, J.E. (2011). A test of the simultaneous vs. sequential
lineup methods: An initial report of the AJS national eyewitness identification field
studies. Des Moines, Iowa: American Judicature Society. Retrieved from:
http://www.popcenter.org/library/reading/PDFs/lineupmethods.pdf
Wise, R.A., & Safer, M.A. (2010). A comparison of what U.S. judges and students know and
believe about eyewitness testimony. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40(6),
1400-1422. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00623.x
Wixted, J.T., & Mickes, L. (2012). The field of eyewitness memory should abandon probative
value and embrace receiver operating characteristic analysis. Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 7(3), 275-278. doi: 10.1177/1745691612442906
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
106
Police Foundation
Wright, D.B., & McDaid, A.T. (1996). Comparing system and estimator variables using data
from real lineups. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10(1), 75-84. doi:
10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199602)10:1<75::AID-ACP364>3.0.CO;2-E
Yuille, J.C., & Wells, G.L. (1991). Concerns about the application of research findings: The
issue of ecological validity. In J. Doris (Ed.). The suggestibility of children’s
recollections: Implications for eyewitness testimony (pp. 118-128). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
107
Police Foundation
Appendix A
Police Foundation Strength of Evidence Scale
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
108
Police Foundation
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
109
Police Foundation
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
110
Police Foundation
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
111
Police Foundation
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
112
Police Foundation
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
113
Police Foundation
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
114
Police Foundation
Appendix B
Individual Final Rating Form
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
115
Police Foundation
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
116
Police Foundation
Photo Arrays in Eyewitness Identification
117
Police Foundation
Download