ATTENTION: SELECTIVITY AND RESOURCE ALLOCATION • SELECTIVITY – what events “capture” attention? – how complete is selectivity? • AROUSAL AND ALERTNESS – does capacity change over time? – how is it affected by arousal,task demands or intention? • DIVIDED ATTENTION – how well can we do two things at once? – can we improve our skill in dividing attention? MEASURING WHERE ATTENTION IS FOCUSSED tracking eye fixations this is the first day of the rest of your life in the. tracking ear fixations (“shadowing”) “when in the course of …..” “and another score for the Gators ……” “.. another score, uh …” EYE TRACKING TECHNOLOGY Seven records of eye movements by the same subject. Each record lasted 3 minutes. 1) Free examination. Before subsequent recordings, the subject was asked to: 2) estimate the material circumstances of the family; 3) give the ages of the people; 4) surmise what the family had been doing before the arrival of the "unexpected visitor;" 5) remember the clothes worn by the people; 6) remember the position of the people and objects in the room; 7) estimate how long the "unexpected visitor" had been away from the family (from Yarbus (1967). DYNAMICS OF VISUAL ATTENTION Fig. 4-18, p. 122 “COVERT” SHIFTS OF SPATIAL ATTENTION (Posner & Cohen, 1984) fixate center throughout trial: cue for likely side of test: (p. = .8) @ 100 to 1000 msec D e c is io n T im e respond to target figure: X @ 700 900 600 550 500 450 400 valid 350 300 invalid 100 300 500 Cue-Target Delay ATTENTION TO OBJECTS vs. LOCATIONS (Egly 1994) Fig. 4-22, p. 126 UNILATERAL NEGLECT: Impaired shifting of spatial attention Damage to Right Hemisphere: Left hemisphere: focus on right side of space Right Hemisphere: focus on left and right side SELECTIVE ATTENTION AS A SENSORY FILTER (Broadbent, 1958) input “channels” sensory analysis pattern recogn “early” filter • Evidence for “early” selection: – poor detection and memory for unattended input channel(s) – tendency to report concurrent inputs “by channel” left ear: 2 . .4 . .9 right ear: 6 . .1 . .8 report: 2,4 ,9 . .6, 1 •Evidence against “early” selection: –shadowing disrupted by S’s name –context can force switch to ignored ear –meaning of “ignored” words can affect behavior EFFECTS OF AN “UNATTENDED” WORD (MacKay, 1972) “ . . the boy threw a rock at the bank and..” “ . .scissor . . ladder . . money . . finger . .” “Ignored” words not remembered, but still bias interpretation of sentence • Evidence for “activation without awareness” • Such “automatic” effects are small, and depend on special conditions ATTENTION AS ALLOCATION OF LIMITED CAPACITY (Kahneman, 1972) AROUSAL, ATTENTION AND PERFORMANCE Performance For many tasks, performance suffers if arousal is too low or too high (Yerkes & Dodson, 1903) + - low capacity too low Arousal high focus too narrow Easterbrook, ‘59: Cue Utilization Theory R T to T O N E ( m s e c ) MEASURING ATTENTIONAL ALLOCATION (Posner & Boies, 1971) task: decide if two successive letters are the same or different 600 500 400 300 200 -1 -0.5 0 + 0.5 R 1 1.5 2 J 2.5 ? press key • decision and response selection as attentional “bottlenecks” THE ATTENTIONAL DEMANDS OF SHADOWING (Johnston & Heinz, 1978) Shadowed List: ignored List: desk couch chair sofa etc... NONE or voice class DIFF DIFF SAME DIFF DIFF SAME RT to tone alone: 320 msec RT to TONE PRACTICE AND EXPERTISE Staszewski, 1988 task: mental multiplication S o lu tio n tim e ( s e c ) 300 hours (!) of practice on simple (1 by 1) and complex (2 by 5) problems using leftto-right procedures e.g. 267 x 97: “. . nine times two is eighteen hundred; nine time six is 540, that’s 2340 . .” 120 100 80 first block 60 last bock 40 20 0 1x1 1x3 1x5 2x3 Problem Size 2x5 PRACTICE AND DIVIDED ATTENTION • Practice in shadowing (Underwood, 1976) – Task: shadow prose in left ear – and detect occasional digits in right ear • Oxford Undergrads: 13% hits • Neville Moray: 71% hits • Practice in dictation (Spelke, Hirst & Neisser, 1976) – Task: read text for meaning, and write down spoken words – after months of practice, no “cost” of dictation on reading speed or comprehension Is attention skill domain-specific? ATTENTION AND CELL PHONES Strayer, Drew & Johnston, 2003 • About 150 million cell phones • 85% use them while driving • Inattention a leading cause of crashes • So: simulated driving task (track pace car), with/out hands-free chat Drive Accidents 0 Brake onset 933 ms Following distance 25.8 ft Billboard recognition 6.9 Billboard fixation 0.66 Drive & Chat 3 1112 ms 29.3 ft 3.9 0.62 Date: 2003-03-27 New Study Shows Drivers Using Cell Phones Twice As Likely To Cause Rear-end Collisions CHAPEL HILL -- Drivers talking on cell phones are nearly twice as likely as other drivers involved in crashes to have rear-end collisions, according to a new University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill study. Crashes involving cell phone use, however, are less likely to result in fatalities or serious injuries than crashes not involving the devices. LAPSES OF ATTENTION: THE “COGNITIVE FAILURES QUESTIONNAIRE (CFQ) • How often do you . . . – read something and realize you haven’t been thinking about it? – forget why you went from one room to another? – bump into people? – forget if you’ve locked the door? – forget to keep appointments? – drop things? – fail to hear people speaking when you’re doing something else? • Ratings correlate with performance in tasks of selective and divided attention (e.g., stroop interference; Tipper & Baylis, 1987)