Reciprocal aiming: a window into continuous versus discrete movements Laure Fernandez Institute of Human Sciences E.J. Marey (UMR 6233) University of the Mediterranean, Marseille, France Our daily activities incessantly require that we control both the speed and the accuracy our movements via which we interact with the environment. As we all know, moving fast goes at the cost of accuracy and vice versa, which was first documented by Woodworth early as 1899 [1]. The most renowned formalization of this so-called speed-accuracy trade-off is known as Fitts’ law [2], which states that movement time is a linear function of task difficulty, i.e., MT = a + b ID, where MT denotes movement time, a and b are constants, and ID (index of difficulty) equals log2(2A/W), where A and W represent target amplitude and width, respectively. The changes in MT result from changes in the movement patterns, which is most readily observed in the case of reciprocal aiming [3; 4]. In fact, previous studies have shown that the particular kinematic patterns associated with different levels of task difficulty were captured by a unique behavioural dynamics [5]. These patterns span the full range from seemingly concatenated discrete movements, with a clear separation between a deceleration towards the current target and an acceleration towards the next, to fully continuous movements, with a single, fused deceleration/acceleration peak at each movement extremity. Thus, with the appearance of discrete and continuous (rhythmic) movements, the reciprocal aiming task provides a promising window into movement categorization. According to the ecological approach to perception and action, an agent is considered as a constituent part of an environment-agent system; movement is thus understood as emerging from the interaction between the agent’s dynamics and those pertaining to the environment. The agent-environment coupling is thought to be informational (from the environment to the agent) as well as mechanical (from the agent to the environment). These couplings provide the conditions for the emergence of task-specific behavioral dynamics. Identifying how information impacts the particular characteristics of the behavioral dynamics in each specific case is a key challenge for the ecological psychologist. In the present presentation, I will first present our studies on the influence of information on the characteristics of the behavioral dynamics in the case of (reciprocal) aiming. Next, I will focus on the distinction between discrete and reciprocal movements, and discuss the question whether they constitute two distinct movements classes. In fact, the question whether a single control principle underlies the execution of discrete and rhythmic movements has appeared hard to resolve, not in the least so because it has long time been pursued mainly along empirical lines while evading the fundamental question how to demarcate distinct movement classes. Unambiguous classification requires identification of class-defining invariance. Dynamical systems theory offers such a classification principle based on phase flow topologies, which identify all behavioral possibilities within a class [6]. Using a novel technique to reconstruct phase flows from kinematic data, we recently found evidence that movements at low IDs were associated with limit-cycle dynamics (the ‘rhythmic’ mechanism) while those at high IDs were associated with fixed-point dynamics (the ‘discrete’ mechanism) [7]. Moreover, we also showed that ID-MT relation, which has for more than five decades been held to be invariantly linear, in fact contains a discontinuity due to the transition between the discrete and rhythmic mechanism. References: 1. Woodworth, R. S. (1899). The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychological Review, 3, 1-106. 2. Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 47, 381-391. 3. Fernandez, L. & Bootsma, R.J. (2008). Non linear gaining in precision aiming: Making Fitts’ task a bit easier. Acta Psychologica, 129, 217-227. 4. Guiard, Y. (1993). On Fitts’ and Hooke’s laws: Simple harmonic movement in upper-limb cyclical aiming. Acta Psychologica, 82, 139-159. 5. Mottet, D., & Bootsma, R.J. (1999). The dynamics of goal-directed rhythmical aiming. Biological Cybernetics, 80, 235-245. 6. Huys, R., Studenka, B. E., Rheaume, N. L., Zelaznik, H. N. & Jirsa, V. K. (2008). Distinct timing mechanisms produce discrete and continuous movements. PLoS Comput Biol, 4, e1000061. 7. Huys, R., Fernandez, L., Bootsma, R.J., & Jirsa, V.K. (2010). Fitts’ law is not continuous in reciprocal aiming. Proceedings of The Royal Society B.