How Much Can We Generalize from Impact Evaluations? Eva Vivalt∗ New York University April 30, 2015 Abstract Impact evaluations aim to predict the future, but they are rooted in particular contexts and results may not generalize across settings. I founded an organization to systematically collect and synthesize impact evaluation results on a wide variety of interventions in development. These data allow me to answer this and other questions for the first time using a large data set of studies. I examine whether results predict each other and whether variance in results can be explained by program characteristics, such as who is implementing them, where they are being implemented, the scale of the program, and what methods are used. I find that when regressing an estimate on the hierarchical Bayesian meta-analysis result formed from all other studies on the same intervention-outcome combination, the result is significant with a coefficient of 0.5-0.7, though the R2 is very low. The program implementer is the main source of heterogeneity in results, with government-implemented programs faring worse than and being poorly predicted by the smaller studies typically implemented by academic/NGO research teams, even controlling for sample size. I then turn to examine specification searching and publication bias, issues which could affect generalizability and are also important for research credibility. I demonstrate that these biases are quite small; nevertheless, to address them, I discuss a mathematical correction that could be applied before showing that randomized controlled trials (RCTs) are less prone to this type of bias and exploiting them as a robustness check. ∗ E-mail: eva.vivalt@nyu.edu. I thank Edward Miguel, Bill Easterly, David Card, Ernesto Dal Bó, Hunt Allcott, Elizabeth Tipton, David McKenzie, Vinci Chow, Willa Friedman, Xing Huang, Michaela Pagel, Steven Pennings, Edson Severnini, seminar participants at the University of California, Berkeley, Columbia University, New York University, the World Bank, Cornell University, Princeton University, the University of Toronto, the London School of Economics, among others, and participants at the 2015 ASSA meeting and 2013 Association for Public Policy Analysis and Management Fall Research Conference for helpful comments. I am also grateful for the hard work put in by many at AidGrade over the duration of this project, including but not limited to Jeff Qiu, Bobbie Macdonald, Diana Stanescu, Cesar Augusto Lopez, Jennifer Ambrose, Naomi Crowther, Timothy Catlett, Joohee Kim, Gautam Bastian, Christine Shen, Taha Jalil, Risa Santoso and Catherine Razeto. 1 1 Introduction In the last few years, impact evaluations have become extensively used in development eco- nomics research. Policymakers and donors typically fund impact evaluations precisely to figure out how effective a similar program would be in the future to guide their decisions on what course of action they should take. However, it is not yet clear how much we can extrapolate from past results or under which conditions. Further, there is some evidence that even a similar program, in a similar environment, can yield different results. For example, Bold et al. (2013) carry out an impact evaluation of a program to provide contract teachers in Kenya; this was a scaled-up version of an earlier program studied by Duflo, Dupas and Kremer (2012). The earlier intervention studied by Duflo, Dupas and Kremer was implemented by an NGO, while Bold et al. compared implementation by an NGO and the government. While Duflo, Dupas and Kremer found positive effects, Bold et al. showed significant results only for the NGO-implemented group. The different findings in the same country for purportedly similar programs point to the substantial context-dependence of impact evaluation results. Knowing this context-dependence is crucial in order to understand what we can learn from any impact evaluation. While the main reason to examine generalizability is to aid interpretation and improve predictions, it would also help to direct research attention to where it is most needed. If generalizability were higher in some areas, fewer papers would be needed to understand how people would behave in a similar situation; conversely, if there were topics or regions where generalizability was low, it would call for further study. With more information, researchers can better calibrate where to direct their attentions to generate new insights. It is well-known that impact evaluations only happen in certain contexts. For example, Figure 1 shows a heat map of the geocoded impact evaluations in the data used in this paper overlaid by the distribution of World Bank projects (black dots). Both sets of data are geographically clustered, and whether or not we can reasonably extrapolate from one to another depends on how much related heterogeneity there is in treatment effects. Allcott (forthcoming) recently showed that site selection bias was an issue for randomized controlled trials (RCTs) on a firm’s energy conservation programs. Microfinance institutions that run RCTs and hospitals that conduct clinical trials are also selected (Allcott, forthcoming), and World Bank projects that receive an impact evaluation 2 Figure 1: Growth of Impact Evaluations and Location Relative to Programs The figure on the left shows a heat map of the impact evaluations in AidGrade’s database overlaid by black dots indicating where the World Bank has done projects. While there are many other development programs not done by the World Bank, this figure illustrates the great numbers and geographical dispersion of development programs. The figure on the right plots the number of studies that came out in each year that are contained in each of three databases described in the text: 3ie’s title/abstract/keyword database of impact evaluations; J-PAL’s database of affiliated randomized controlled trials; and AidGrade’s database of impact evaluation results data. are different from those that do not (Vivalt, 2015). Others have sought to explain heterogeneous treatment effects in meta-analyses of specific topics (e.g. Saavedra and Garcia, 2013, among many others for conditional cash transfers), or to argue they are so heterogeneous they cannot be adequately modelled (e.g. Deaton, 2011; Pritchett and Sandefur, 2013). Impact evaluations are still exponentially increasing in number and in terms of the resources devoted to them. The World Bank recently received a major grant from the UK aid agency DFID to expand its already large impact evaluation works; the Millennium Challenge Corporation has committed to conduct rigorous impact evaluations for 50% of its activities, with “some form of credible evaluation of impact” for every activity (Millennium Challenge Corporation, 2009); and the U.S. Agency for International Development is also increasingly invested in impact evaluations, coming out with a new policy in 2011 that directs 3% of program funds to evaluation.1 Yet while impact evaluations are still growing in development, a few thousand are already complete. Figure 1 plots the explosion of RCTs that researchers affiliated with J-PAL, a center for development economics research, have completed each year; alongside are the number of development-related impact evaluations released that year according to 3ie, which keeps a direc1 While most of these are less rigorous “performance evaluations”, country mission leaders are supposed to identify at least one opportunity for impact evaluation for each development objective in their 3-5 year plans (USAID, 2011). 3 tory of titles, abstracts, and other basic information on impact evaluations more broadly, including quasi-experimental designs; finally, the dashed line shows the number of papers that came out in each year that are included in AidGrade’s database of impact evaluation results, which will be described shortly. To summarize, while we do impact evaluation to figure out what will happen in the future, many issues have been raised about how well we can extrapolate from past impact evaluations, and despite the importance of the topic, previously we were unable to do little more than guess or examine the question in narrow settings as we did not have the data. Now we have the opportunity to address speculation, drawing on a large, unique dataset of impact evaluation results. I founded a non-profit organization dedicated to gathering this data. That organization, AidGrade, seeks to systematically understand which programs work best where, a task that requires also knowing the limits of our knowledge. To date, AidGrade has conducted 20 meta-analyses and systematic reviews of different development programs.2 Data gathered through meta-analyses are the ideal data to answer the question of how much we can extrapolate from past results, and since data on these 20 topics were collected in the same way, coding the same outcomes and other variables, we can look across different types of programs to see if there are any more general trends. Currently, the data set contains 637 papers on 210 narrowly-defined intervention-outcome combinations, with the greater database containing 14,993 estimates. The data further allow me to examine a second set of questions revolving around specification searching and publication bias. Specification searching refers to the practice whereby researchers artificially select results that meet the criterion for being considered statistically significant, biasing results. It has been found to be a systematic problem by Gerber and Malhotra in the political science and sociology literature (2008a; 2008b); Simmons and Simonsohn (2011) and Bastardi, Uhlmann and Ross (2011) in psychology; and Brodeur et al. (2012) in economics. I look for evidence of specification searching and publication bias in this large data set, both for its own importance as well as because it is possible that results may appear to be more “generalizable” merely because they suffer from a common bias. I pay particular attention to randomized controlled trials (RCTs), which are considered the 2 Throughout, I will refer to all 20 as meta-analyses, but some did not have enough comparable outcomes for meta-analysis and became systematic reviews. 4 “gold standard” in the sciences and on which development economics has increasingly relied. It is possible that the method may reduce specification searching due to its emphasis on rigor or the increased odds of publication independent of results. It is also possible that RCTs may be done in more selected settings, leading to results not generalizing as well. I will shed light on both of these potential issues. The outline of this paper is as follows. First, I define generalizability, present some basic statistics about it, and use leave-one-out cross-validation to check what kinds of study characteristics can help predict another study’s results better than placebo data. I also conduct leave-one-out hierarchical Bayesian meta-analyses of all but one result within an intervention-outcome combination and check to what extent these meta-analysis results, which theoretically might provide the best estimates of a given program’s effects, predict the result left out. Since some of the analyses will draw upon statistical methods not commonly used in economics, I will use the concrete example of conditional cash transfers (CCTs), which are relatively well-understood and on which many papers have been written, to elucidate the issues. Regarding specification searching, I first conduct caliper tests on the distribution of z-statistics, seeing whether there is a disproportionate number of papers just above the threshold for statistical significance compared to those just below the threshold. The data contain both published papers and unpublished working papers, and I examine how much publication bias there appears to be for results that are significant. After examining how much these biases are present, I discuss how one might correct for them when considering generalizability. There is a simple mathematical adjustment that could be made if one were willing to accept the constraints of a fixed effects meta-analysis model.3 However, I show this would not be an appropriate model for these data. Instead, I turn to using RCTs, which I show do not suffer as much from these biases, as a robustness check. While this paper focuses on results for impact evaluations of development programs, this is only one of the first areas within economics to which these kinds of methods can be applied. In many of the sciences, knowledge is built through a combination of researchers conducting individual studies and other researchers synthesizing the evidence through meta-analysis. This paper begins that natural next step. 3 In particular, Simonsohn et al. (2014) make the assumption of fixed effects. 5 2 Theory 2.1 Heterogeneous Treatment Effects I model treatment effects as potentially depending on the context of the intervention. Each impact evaluation is on a particular intervention and covers a number of outcomes. The relationship between an outcome, the inputs that were part of the intervention, and the context of the study is complex. In the simplest model, we can imagine that context can be represented a “contextual variable”, C, such that: Zj “ α ` βTj ` δCj ` γTj Cj ` εj (1) where j indexes the individual, Z represents the value of an aggregate outcome such as “enrollment rates”, T indicates being treated, and C represents a contextual variable, such as the type of agency that implemented the program.4 In this framework, a particular impact evaluation might explicitly estimate: Zj “ α ` β 1 Tj ` εj (2) but, as Equation 1 can be re-written as Zj “ α ` pβ ` γCj qTj ` δCj ` εj , what β 1 is really capturing is the effect β 1 “ β ` γC. When C varies, unobserved, in different contexts, the variance of β 1 increases. This is the simplest case. One can imagine that the true state of the world has “interaction effects all the way down”. Interaction terms are often considered a second-order problem. However, that intuition could stem from the fact that we usually look for interaction terms within an already fairly homogeneous dataset - e.g. data from a single country, at a single point in time, on a particularly selected sample. Not all aspects of context need matter to an intervention’s outcomes. The set of contextual variables can be divided into a critical set on which outcomes depend and an set on which they do not; I will ignore the latter. Further, the relationship between Z and C can vary by intervention or outcome. For example, school meals programs might have more of an effect on younger children, 4 Z can equally well be thought of as the average individual outcome for an intervention. Throughout, I take high values for an outcome to represent a beneficial change unless otherwise noted; if an outcome represents a negative characteristic, like incidence of a disease, its sign will be flipped before analysis. 6 but scholarship programs could plausibly affect older children more. If one were to regress effect size on the contextual variable “age”, we would get different results depending on which intervention and outcome we were considering. Therefore, it will be important in this paper to look only at a restricted set of contextual variables which could plausibly work in a similar way across different interventions. Additional analysis could profitably be done within some interventions, but this is outside the scope of this paper. Generalizability will ultimately depend on the heterogeneity of treatment effects. The next section formally defines generalizability for use in this paper. 2.2 Generalizability: Definitions and Measurement Definition 1 Generalizability is the ability to predict results accurately out of sample. Definition 2 Local generalizability is the ability to predict results accurately in a particular outof-sample group. Any empirical work, including this paper, will only be able to address local generalizability. However, I will argue we should not be concerned about this. First, the meta-analysis data were explicitly gathered using very broad inclusion criteria, aiming to capture the universe of studies. Second, by using a large set of studies in various contexts and repeatedly leaving out some of the studies when generating predictions, we can estimate the sensitivity of results to the inclusion of particular papers. In particular, as part of this paper I will systematically leave out one of the studies, predict it based on the other studies, and do this for each study, cycling through the studies. As a robustness check I do this for different subsets of the data. There are several ways to measure predictive power. Most measures of predictive power rely on building a model on a “training” data set and estimating the fit of that model on an out-of-sample “test” data set, or estimating on the same data set but with a correction. Gelman et al. (2013) provides a good summary. I will focus on one of these methods - cross-validation. In particular, I will use the predicted residual sum of squares (PRESS) statistic, which is closely related to the mean squared error and can also be used to generate an R2 -like statistic. Specifically, to calculate it one follows this procedure: 7 1. Start at study i “ 1 within each intervention-outcome combination. 2. Generate the predicted value of effect size Yi , Ypi , by building a model based on Y´i , the effect sizes for all observations in that intervention-outcome except i. For example, regress Y´i “ α`βC´i `ε, where C represents a predictor of interest, and then use the estimated coefficients to predict Ypi . Alternatively, generate Ypi as the meta-analysis result from synthesizing Y´i . 3. Calculate the squared error, pYi ´ Ypi q2 . 4. Repeat for each i ‰ 1. 5. Calculate PRESS “ ř n pYi ´ Ypi q2 To aid interpretation, I also calculate the PRESS statistic for placebo data in simulations. The models for predicting Yi are intentionally simple, as both economists and policymakers often do not build complicated models of the effect sizes when drawing inferences from past studies. First, I simply see if interventions, outcomes, intervention-outcomes, region or implementer have any explanatory power in predicting the resultant effect size. I also try using the meta-analysis result M´i , obtained from synthesizing the effect sizes Y´i , as the predictor of Yi . This makes intuitive sense; we are often in the situation of wanting to predict the effect size in a new context from a meta-analysis result. When calculating the PRESS statistic, I correct the results for attenuation bias using a firstorder approximation described in Gelman et al. (2013) and Tibshirani and Tibshirani (2009). y “ CV ´ CV. Ě Representing the cross-validation squared error pYi ´ Ypi q2 as CV, bias While predictive power is perhaps the most natural measure of generalizability, I will also show results on how impact evaluation results correlate with each other. The difference between correlation and predictive power is clear: it is similar to the difference between an estimated coefficient and an R2 . Impact evaluation results could be correlated so that regressing Yi on explanatory variables like M´i could result in a large, significant coefficient on the M´i term while still having a low R2 . Indeed, this is what we will see in the data. To create the meta-analysis result M´i , I use a hierarchical Bayesian random effects model with an uninformative prior, as described in the next section on meta-analysis. 8 2.3 Models Used in Meta-Analyses This paper uses meta-analysis as a tool to synthesize evidence. As a quick review, there are many steps in a meta-analysis, most of which have to do with the selection of the constituent papers. The search and screening of papers will be described in the data section; here, I merely discuss the theory behind how meta-analyses combine results. One of two main statistical models underlie almost all meta-analyses: the fixed-effect model or the random-effects model. Fixed-effect models assume there is one true effect of a particular program and all differences between studies can be attributed simply to sampling error. In other words: Yi “ θ ` ε i (3) where θ is the true effect and εi is the error term. Random-effects models do not make this assumption; the true effect could potentially vary from context to context. Here, Yi “ θi ` εi “ θ̄ ` ηi ` εi (4) (5) where θi is the effect size for a particular study i, θ̄ is the mean true effect size, ηi is a particular study’s divergence from that mean true effect size, and εi is the error. When estimating either a fixed effect or random effects model through meta-analysis, a choice must be made: how to weight the studies that serve as inputs to the meta-analysis. Several weighting schemes can be used, but by far the most common to use are inverse-variance weights. As the variance is a measure of how certain we are of the effect, this ensures that those results about which we are more confident get weighted more heavily. The variance will contain a between-studies term in the case of random effects. Writing the weights as W , the summary effect is simply: řk M “ ři“1 k W i Yi i“1 Wi 9 (6) with standard error b řk 1 i“1 Wi . To build a hierarchical Bayesian model, I first assume the data are normally distributed: Yij |θi „ N pθi , σ 2 q (7) where j indexes the individuals in the study. I do not have individual-level data, but instead can use sufficient statistics: Yi |θi „ N pθi , σi2 q (8) where Yi is the sample mean and σi2 the sample variance. This provides the likelihood for θi . I also need a prior for θi . I assume between-study normality: θi „ N pµ, τ 2 q (9) where µ and τ are unknown hyperparameters. Conditioning on the distribution of the data, given by Equation 8, I get a posterior: θi |µ, τ, Y „ N pθˆi , Vi q where θˆi “ Yi σi2 1 σi2 ` ` µ τ2 1 τ2 , Vi “ 1 σi2 1 ` (10) 1 τ2 (11) I then need to pin down µ|τ and τ by constructing their posterior distributions given noninformative priors and updating based on the data. I assume a uniform prior for µ|τ , and as the Yi are estimates of µ with variance pσi2 ` τ 2 q, obtain: µ|τ, Y „ N pµ̂, Vµ q where Yi i σi2 `τ 2 ř 1 i σi2 `τ 2 (12) ř µ̂ “ , Vµ “ 10 ÿ 1 i 1 σi2 `τ 2 (13) For τ , note that ppτ |Y q “ ppµ,τ |Y q ppµ|τ,Y q . The denominator follows from Equation 12; for the numer- ator, we can observe that ppµ, τ |Y q is proportional to ppµ, τ qppY |µ, τ q, and we know the marginal distribution of Yi |µ, τ : Yi |µ, τ „ N pµ, σi2 ` τ 2 q (14) I use a uniform prior for τ , following Gelman et al. (2005). This yields the posterior for the numerator: ppµ, τ |Y q9ppµ, τ q ź N pYi |µ, σi2 ` τ 2 q (15) i Putting together all the pieces in reverse order, I first simulate τ , then generate ppτ |Y q using τ , followed by µ and finally θi . Unless otherwise noted, I rely on this hierarchical Bayesian random effects model to generate meta-analysis results. 3 Data This paper uses a database of impact evaluation results collected by AidGrade, a U.S. non-profit research institute that I founded in 2012. AidGrade focuses on gathering the results of impact evaluations and analyzing the data, including through meta-analysis. Its data on impact evaluation results were collected in the course of its meta-analyses from 2012-2014 (AidGrade, 2015). AidGrade’s meta-analyses follow the standard stages: (1) topic selection; (2) a search for relevant papers; (3) screening of papers; (4) data extraction; and (5) data analysis. In addition, it pays attention to (6) dissemination and (7) updating of results. Here, I will discuss the selection of papers (stages 1-3) and the data extraction protocol (stage 4); more detail is provided in Appendix B. 3.1 Selection of Papers The interventions that were selected for meta-analysis were selected largely on the basis of there being a sufficient number of studies on that topic. Five AidGrade staff members each independently made a preliminary list of interventions for examination; the lists were then combined and searches done for each topic to determine if there were likely to be enough impact evaluations for a meta- 11 analysis. The remaining list was voted on by the general public online and partially randomized. Appendix B provides further detail. A comprehensive literature search was done using a mix of the search aggregators SciVerse, Google Scholar, and EBSCO/PubMed. The online databases of J-PAL, IPA, CEGA and 3ie were also searched for completeness. Finally, the references of any existing systematic reviews or metaanalyses were collected. Any impact evaluation which appeared to be on the intervention in question was included, barring those in developed countries.5 Any paper that tried to consider the counterfactual was considered an impact evaluation. Both published papers and working papers were included. The search and screening criteria were deliberately broad. There is not enough room to include the full text of the search terms and inclusion criteria for all 20 topics in this paper, but these are available in an online appendix as detailed in Appendix A. 3.2 Data Extraction The subset of the data on which I am focusing is based on those papers that passed all screening stages in the meta-analyses. Again, the search and screening criteria were very broad and, after passing the full text screening, the vast majority of papers that were later excluded were excluded merely because they had no outcome variables in common or did not provide adequate data (for example, not providing data that could be used to calculate the standard error of an estimate, or for a variety of other quirky reasons, such as displaying results only graphically). The small overlap of outcome variables is a surprising and notable feature of the data. Ultimately, the data I draw upon for this paper consist of 14,993 results (double-coded and then reconciled by a third researcher) across 637 papers covering the 20 types of development program listed in Table 1.6 For sake of comparison, though the two organizations clearly do different things, at present time of writing this is more impact evaluations than J-PAL has published, concentrated in these 20 topics. Unfortunately, only 318 of these papers both overlapped in outcomes with another paper and were able to 5 High-income countries, according to the World Bank’s classification system. Three titles here may be misleading. “Mobile phone-based reminders” refers specifically to SMS or voice reminders for health-related outcomes. “Women’s empowerment programs” required an educational component to be included in the intervention and it could not be an unrelated intervention that merely disaggregated outcomes by gender. Finally, micronutrients were initially too loosely defined; this was narrowed down to focus on those providing zinc to children, but the other micronutrient papers are still included in the data, with a tag, as they may still be useful. 6 12 be standardized and thus included in the main results which rely on intervention-outcome groups. Outcomes were defined under several rules of varying specificity, as will be discussed shortly. Table 1: List of Development Programs Covered 2012 Conditional cash transfers Deworming Improved stoves Insecticide-treated bed nets Microfinance Safe water storage Scholarships School meals Unconditional cash transfers Water treatment 2013 Contract teachers Financial literacy training HIV education Irrigation Micro health insurance Micronutrient supplementation Mobile phone-based reminders Performance pay Rural electrification Women’s empowerment programs 73 variables were coded for each paper. Additional topic-specific variables were coded for some sets of papers, such as the median and mean loan size for microfinance programs. This paper focuses on the variables held in common across the different topics. These include which method was used; if randomized, whether it was randomized by cluster; whether it was blinded; where it was (village, province, country - these were later geocoded in a separate process); what kind of institution carried out the implementation; characteristics of the population; and the duration of the intervention from the baseline to the midline or endline results, among others. A full set of variables and the coding manual is available online, as detailed in Appendix A. As this paper pays particular attention to the program implementer, it is worth discussing how this variable was coded in more detail. There were several types of implementers that could be coded: governments, NGOs, private sector firms, and academics. There was also a code for “other” (primarily collaborations) or “unclear”. The vast majority of studies were implemented by academic research teams and NGOs. This paper considers NGOs and academic research teams together because it turned out to be practically difficult to distinguish between them in the studies, especially as the passive voice was frequently used (e.g. “X was done” without noting who did it). There were only a few private sector firms involved, so they are considered with the “other” category in this paper. 13 Studies tend to report results for multiple specifications. AidGrade focused on those results least likely to have been influenced by author choices: those with the fewest controls, apart from fixed effects. Where a study reported results using different methodologies, coders were instructed to collect the findings obtained under the authors’ preferred methodology; where the preferred methodology was unclear, coders were advised to follow the internal preference ordering of prioritizing randomized controlled trials, followed by regression discontinuity designs and differences-indifferences, followed by matching, and to collect multiple sets of results when they were unclear on which to include. Where results were presented separately for multiple subgroups, coders were similarly advised to err on the side of caution and to collect both the aggregate results and results by subgroup except where the author appeared to be only including a subgroup because results were significant within that subgroup. For example, if an author reported results for children aged 8-15 and then also presented results for children aged 12-13, only the aggregate results would be recorded, but if the author presented results for children aged 8-9, 10-11, 12-13, and 14-15, all subgroups would be coded as well as the aggregate result when presented. Authors only rarely reported isolated subgroups, so this was not a major issue in practice. When considering the variation of effect sizes within a group of papers, the definition of the group is clearly critical. Two different rules were initially used to define outcomes: a strict rule, under which only identical outcome variables are considered alike, and a loose rule, under which similar but distinct outcomes are grouped into clusters. The precise coding rules were as follows: 1. We consider outcome A to be the same as outcome B under the “strict rule” if outcomes A and B measure the exact same quality. Different units may be used, pending conversion. The outcomes may cover different timespans (e.g. encompassing both outcomes over “the last month” and “the last week”). They may also cover different populations (e.g. children or adults). Examples: height; attendance rates. 2. We consider outcome A to be the same as outcome B under the “loose rule” if they do not meet the strict rule but are clearly related. Example: parasitemia greater than 4000/µl with fever and parasitemia greater than 2500/µl. 14 Clearly, even under the strict rule, differences between the studies may exist, however, using two different rules allows us to isolate the potential sources of variation, and other variables were coded to capture some of this variation, such as the age of those in the sample. If one were to divide the studies by these characteristics, however, the data would usually be too sparse for analysis. Interventions were also defined separately and coders were also asked to write a short description of the details of each program. Program names were recorded so as to identify those papers on the same program, such as the various evaluations of PROGRESA. After coding, the data were then standardized to make results easier to interpret and so as not to overly weight those outcomes with larger scales. The typical way to compare results across different outcomes is by using the standardized mean difference, defined as: SM D “ µ 1 ´ µ2 σp where µ1 is the mean outcome in the treatment group, µ2 is the mean outcome in the control group, and σp is the pooled standard deviation. When data are not available to calculate the pooled standard deviation, it can be approximated by the standard deviation of the dependent variable for the entire distribution of observations or as the standard deviation in the control group (Glass, 1976). If that is not available either, due to standard deviations not having been reported in the original papers, one can use the typical standard deviation for the intervention-outcome. I follow this approach to calculate the standardized mean difference, which is then used as the effect size measure for the rest of the paper unless otherwise noted. This paper uses the “strict” outcomes where available, but the “loose” outcomes where that would keep more data. For papers which were follow-ups of the same study, the most recent results were used for each outcome. Finally, one paper appeared to misreport results, suggesting implausibly low values and standard deviations for hemoglobin. These results were excluded and the paper’s corresponding author contacted. Excluding this paper’s results, effect sizes range between -1.5 and 1.8 SD, with an interquartile range of 0 to 0.2 SD. So as to mitigate sensitivity to individual results, especially with the small number of papers in some intervention-outcome groups, I restrict attention to those standardized effect sizes less than 2 SD away from 0, dropping 1 additional observation. I report 15 main results including this observation in the Appendix. 3.3 Data Description Figure 2 summarizes the distribution of studies covering the interventions and outcomes considered in this paper. Attention will typically be limited to those intervention-outcome combinations on which we have data for at least three papers, with an alternative minimum of four papers in the Appendix. Table 12 in Appendix C lists the interventions and outcomes and describes their results in a bit more detail, providing the distribution of significant and insignificant results. It should be emphasized that the number of negative and significant, insignificant, and positive and significant results per intervention-outcome combination only provide ambiguous evidence of the typical efficacy of a particular type of intervention. Simply tallying the numbers in each category is known as “vote counting” and can yield misleading results if, for example, some studies are underpowered. Table 2 further summarizes the distribution of papers across interventions and highlights the fact that papers exhibit very little overlap in terms of outcomes studied. This is consistent with the story of researchers each wanting to publish one of the first papers on a topic. We will indeed see that later papers on the same intervention-outcome combination more often remain as working papers. A note must be made about combining data. When conducting a meta-analysis, the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions recommends collapsing the data to one observation per intervention-outcome-paper, and I do this for generating the within intervention-outcome meta-analyses (Higgins and Green, 2011). Where results had been reported for multiple subgroups (e.g. women and men), I aggregated them as in the Cochrane Handbook’s Table 7.7.a. Where results were reported for multiple time periods (e.g. 6 months after the intervention and 12 months after the intervention), I used the most comparable time periods across papers. When combining across multiple outcomes, which has limited use but will come up later in the paper, I used the formulae from Borenstein et al. (2009), Chapter 24. 16 Figure 2: Within-Intervention-Outcome Number of Papers 17 Table 2: Descriptive Statistics: Distribution of Narrow Outcomes Intervention Number of outcomes Mean papers per outcome Max papers per outcome Conditional cash transfers Contract teachers Deworming Financial literacy HIV/AIDS Education Improved stoves Insecticide-treated bed nets Irrigation Micro health insurance Microfinance Micronutrient supplementation Mobile phone-based reminders Performance pay Rural electrification Safe water storage Scholarships School meals Unconditional cash transfers Water treatment Women’s empowerment programs 10 1 12 1 3 4 1 2 1 5 23 2 1 3 1 3 3 3 2 2 16 3 13 5 8 2 9 2 2 4 27 4 3 3 2 2 3 2 5 2 28 3 18 5 10 2 9 2 2 5 47 5 3 3 2 2 3 2 6 2 Average 4.2 5.9 8.0 Table 3: Differences between vote counting and meta-analysis results Vote counting result Negative Insignificant Positive Total Meta-analysis result Negative Insignificant Positive and significant and significant 0 0 0 1 30 14 0 1 21 1 31 35 18 Total 0 45 22 67 4 Generalizability of Impact Evaluation Results 4.1 Method The first thing I do is to report basic summary statistics. I ask: given a positive, significant result - the kind perhaps most relevant for motivating policy - what proportion of papers on the same intervention-outcome combination find a positive, significant effect, an insignificant effect, or a negative, significant effect? How much do the results of vote counting and meta-analysis diverge? Another key summary statistic is the coefficient of variation. This statistic is frequently used as a measure of dispersion of results and is defined as σ µ, where µ is the mean of a set of results and σ the standard deviation.7 The set of results under consideration here is defined by the intervention-outcome combination; I also separately look at variation within papers. While the coefficient of variation is a basic statistic, this is the first time it is reported across a wide variety of impact evaluation results. Finally, I look at how much results overlap within intervention-outcome combinations, using the raw, unstandardized data. I then regress the effect size on several explanatory variables, including the leave-one-out metaanalysis result for all but study i within each intervention-outcome combination, M´i . Whenever I use M´i to predict Yi , I adjust the estimates for sampling variance to avoid attenuation bias. I also cluster standard errors at the intervention-outcome level to guard against the case in which an outlier introduces systematic error within an intervention-outcome. While the main results are based on any intervention-outcome combination covered by at least three papers, as mentioned I try increasing this minimum number of papers in robustness checks that are included in the Appendix. Finally, I construct the PRESS statistic to measure generalizability, as discussed in Section 2.2. In order to be able to say whether the result is large or small, I also calculate an R2 -like statistic for prediction and conduct simulations using placebo data for comparison. The R2 -like statistic is the RP2 r from prediction: RP2 r “ 1 ´ 7 Absolute values are always taken. 19 PRESS SST ot (16) where SST ot is the total sum of squares. In the simulations, I randomly assign the real effect size data to alternative interventions, outcomes, or other variables and then generate the PRESS statistic and the predicted RP2 r for these placebo groups. 4.2 4.2.1 Results Summary statistics Summary statistics provide a first look at how results vary across different papers. The average intervention-outcome combination is comprised 37% of positive, significant results; 58% of insignificant results; and 5% of negative, significant results. To gauge how stable results are, suppose we know that one study in a particular intervention-outcome combination found a positive, significant result (the kind of result one might think could influence policy); drawing another study at random from the set, there is a 60% chance the new result will be insignificant and a 8% chance it will be significant and negative, leaving only about a 32% chance it will again be positive and significant. The differences between meta-analysis results and vote counting results are shown in Table 3. Only those intervention-outcomes which had a vote counting “winner” are included in this table; in other words, it does not include ties. That the meta-analysis result was often positive and significant or negative and significant when the vote counting result was insignificant is likely a function of many impact evaluations being underpowered. In the methods section, I discussed the coefficient of variation, a measure of the dispersion of the impact evaluation findings. Values for the coefficient of variation in the medical literature tend to range from approximately 0.1 to 0.5. Figure 3 shows its distribution in the economics data, across papers within intervention-outcomes as well as within papers. Each of the across-paper coefficients of variation was calculated within an intervention-outcome combination; for example, the effects of conditional cash transfer programs on enrollment rates. When a paper reports multiple results, the previously described conventions work to either select one of them or aggregate them so that there is one result per intervention-outcome-paper that is used to calculate the within-intervention-outcome, across-paper coefficient of variation. The across- 20 Figure 3: Distribution of the Coefficient of Variation paper coefficient of variation is thus likely lower than it might otherwise be due to the aggregation process reducing noise. The within-paper coefficients of variation are calculated where the data include multiple results from the same paper on the same intervention-outcome. There are several reasons a paper may have reported multiple results: multiple time periods were examined; the author used multiple methods; or results for different subgroups were collected. In each of these scenarios, the context is more similar than it typically is across different papers. Variation within a single paper due to different specifications and subgroups has often been neglected in the literature but constituted on average approximately 67% of the variation across papers within a single intervention-outcome combination. As Figure 3 makes clear, the coefficient of variation within the same paper within an interventionoutcome combination is much lower than that across papers. The mean coefficient of variation across papers in the same intervention-outcome combination is 1.9; the mean coefficient of variation for results within the same paper in the same intervention-outcome combination is lower, at 1.2, a difference that is significantly different in a t-test at pă0.05.8 The coefficient of variation clearly depends on the set of results being considered. Outcomes were extremely narrowly defined, as discussed; interventions varied more. For example, a school meals program might disburse different kinds of meals in one study than in another. The contexts also varied, such as in terms of the implementing agency, the age group, the underlying rates 8 All these results are based on truncating the coefficient of variation at 10 as in Figure 3; if one does so at 20, the across-paper coefficient of variation rises to 2.1 and the within-paper to 1.5. 21 of malnutrition, and so on. The data are mostly too sparse to use this information, however, a few papers considered the same programs; the average coefficient of variation within the same intervention-outcome-program combination, across papers, was 1.5. To aid in interpreting the results, I return to the unstandardized values within interventionoutcome combinations. I ask: what is the typical gap between a study’s point estimate and the average point estimate within that intervention-outcome combination? How often do the confidence intervals around an estimated effect size overlap within intervention-outcomes? Table 4 presents some results, excluding risk ratios and rate ratios, which are on different scales. The mean absolute difference between a study’s point estimate and the average point estimate within that intervention-outcome combination is about 90%. Regarding the confidence intervals, a given result in an intervention-outcome combination will, on average, have a confidence interval that overlaps with about 85% of the confidence intervals of the other results in that intervention-outcome. The point estimate will be contained in the confidence interval of the other studies approximately half of the time. 4.2.2 Regression results Do results exhibit any systematic variation? This section examines whether generalizability is associated with study characteristics such as the type of program implementer. I first present some OLS results. As Table 5 indicates, there is some evidence that studies with a smaller number of observations have greater effect sizes than studies based on a larger number of observations. This is what we would expect if specification searching were easier for small datasets; this pattern of results would also be what we would expect if power calculations drove researchers to only proceed with studies with small sample sizes if they believed the program would result in a large effect size or if larger studies are less well-targeted. Interestingly, government-implemented programs fare worse even controlling for sample size (the dummy variable category left out is “Other-implemented”, which mainly consists of collaborations and private sector-implemented interventions). Studies in the Middle East / North Africa region may appear to do slightly better than those in Sub-Saharan Africa (the excluded region category), but not much weight should be put on this as very few studies were conducted in the former region. I then turn to the PRESS statistic in Table 6. In this table, all C represent dummy variables 22 on the RHS of the regression that is fit; for example, the first row uses the fitted Ypi from regressř ing Yi “ α ` n βn Interventionin ` εi where Intervention comprises dummy variables indicating different interventions. The PRESS statistic and RP2 r from each regression of Y on assorted C is listed, along with the average PRESS statistic and RP2 r from the corresponding placebo simulations. It should be noted that, unlike R2 , RP2 r need not have a lower bound of zero. This is because the predicted residual sum of squares, which is by definition greater than the residual sum of squares, can also be greater than the total sum of squares. The p-value gives how likely it is the PRESS statistic is from the distribution of simulation PRESS statistics, using the standard deviation from the simulations.9 As Table 6 shows, one can distinguish the interventions, outcomes, interventionoutcomes, and regions in the data better than chance. The implementer dummy does not have significant predictive power here. One might believe the relatively poor predictive power is due to too many diverse interventions and outcomes being grouped together. I therefore separate out the two interventions with the largest number of studies, CCTs and deworming, to see if patterns are any different within each of these interventions. Results are weaker here (Table 7). While this could partially due to reduced sample sizes, it also suggests that there are many different sources of heterogeneity in the data. Table 8 presents more PRESS statistics, this time using the leave-one-out meta-analysis result from within intervention-outcome combinations to predict the result left out. The significance of the meta-analysis results is striking compared to that of the earlier results, but the low RP2 r should be noted. The regressions in Table 9 show some of the key takeaways of this paper in an easily digested format. The relationship between the PRESS statistics and a regression model can also be seen by comparing Tables 8 and 9. In both cases, the meta-analysis result is a significant predictor of the effect size, but the R2 or RP2 r is also low in both tables. Table 9 also suggests that governmentimplemented programs do not fare as well on average. 9 Simulations were run 100 times for each regression. 23 24 Intervention Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Conditional Cash Transfers Contract Teachers Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Financial Literacy Financial Literacy Financial Literacy HIV/AIDS Education HIV/AIDS Education HIV/AIDS Education HIV/AIDS Education HIV/AIDS Education Irrigation Irrigation Micro Health Insurance Micro Health Insurance Micro Health Insurance Table 4: Differences in Point Estimates Outcome Mean estimate Attendance rate 0.07 Birth in a medical facility 0.07 Enrollment rate 0.08 Height 0.99 Height-for-age -0.01 Labor force participation -0.03 Labor hours -1.99 Pregnancy rate -0.03 Probability skilled attendant at delivery 0.12 Probability unpaid work -0.06 Retention rate -0.01 Test scores 0.06 Unpaid labor hours -1.51 Weight-for-age 0.19 Weight-for-height 0.12 Test scores 0.18 Attendance rate 0.03 Birthweight 0.00 Height 0.09 Height-for-age 0.16 Hemoglobin 0.06 Malformations 0.05 Mid-upper arm circumference 0.08 Test scores 0.08 Weight 0.14 Weight-for-age 0.14 Weight-for-height 0.09 Has savings 0.02 Probability has taken loan 0.03 Savings -17.40 Pregnancy rate -0.01 Probability has multiple sex partners 0.23 Probability sexually active -0.02 STD prevalence 0.09 Used contraceptives 0.04 Consumption 838.55 Total income -6.20 Household health expenditures 0.17 Probability of inpatient visit 0.00 Probability of outpatient visit 0.00 Mean difference 0.03 0.06 0.06 0.03 0.17 0.03 1.64 0.04 0.06 0.07 0.01 0.09 0.97 0.25 0.09 0.06 0.02 0.01 0.28 0.21 0.11 0.05 0.22 0.03 0.25 0.21 0.18 0.01 0.05 67.18 0.01 0.22 0.02 0.10 0.07 887.56 89.13 0.18 0.01 0.03 Units percentage points percentage points percentage points cm z-score percentage points hours/week percentage points percentage points percentage points percentage points standard deviations hours/week z-score z-score standard deviations percentage points kg cm z-score g/dL percentage points cm standard deviations kg z-score z-score percentage points percentage points current US$ percentage points percentage points percentage points percentage points percentage points current US$ current US$ current US$ percentage points percentage points 25 Microfinance Microfinance Microfinance Microfinance Microfinance Microfinance Micronutrients Micronutrients Micronutrients Micronutrients Micronutrients Micronutrients Micronutrients Micronutrients Performance Pay Rural Electrification Rural Electrification Rural Electrification Scholarships Scholarships Scholarships School Meals School Meals School Meals Unconditional Cash Transfers Unconditional Cash Transfers Unconditional Cash Transfers Women’s Empowerment Women’s Empowerment Assets Consumption Probability of owning business Profits Savings Total income Height Height-for-age Hemoglobin Mid-upper arm circumference Test scores Weight Weight-for-age Weight-for-height Test scores Enrollment rate Study time Total income Attendance rate Enrollment rate Test scores Enrollment rate Height-for-age Test scores Enrollment rate Test scores Weight-for-height Savings Total income 21.81 -3.52 0.05 -14.41 41.06 23.54 0.07 0.06 0.08 0.04 0.01 0.09 0.05 0.03 0.13 0.08 1.10 42.46 0.01 0.01 0.11 0.11 0.15 0.12 0.12 0.06 0.02 24.16 6.83 79.83 4.27 0.03 55.06 56.02 18.20 0.36 0.11 0.30 0.11 0.11 0.13 0.09 0.08 0.06 0.01 0.88 42.23 0.00 0.00 0.08 0.10 0.06 0.12 0.12 0.02 0.04 21.69 1.04 current US$ current US$ percentage points current US$ current US$ current US$ cm z-score g/dL cm standard deviations kg z-score z-score standard deviations percentage points hours/week current US$ percentage points percentage points standard deviations percentage points z-score standard deviations percentage points standard deviations z-score current US$ current US$ Table 5: Regression of Effect Size on Study Characteristics (1) Effect size b/se Number of observations (100,000s) Government-implemented (2) Effect size b/se (3) Effect size b/se -0.018*** (0.01) (4) Effect size b/se (5) Effect size b/se -0.019*** (0.00) -0.015** (0.01) -0.150** (0.06) -0.075 (0.05) -0.163*** (0.06) -0.070 (0.04) Academic/NGO-implemented RCT 0.049 (0.04) East Asia 0.120*** (0.00) 0.199*** (0.04) 0.080*** (0.03) -0.015 (0.03) -0.006 (0.04) 0.284** (0.11) 0.009 (0.04) 0.114*** (0.02) 534 0.19 634 0.23 634 0.22 534 0.22 Latin America Middle East/North Africa South Asia Constant Observations R2 0.201*** (0.04) 534 0.20 Table 6: PRESS statistics and RP2 r : All interventions C dummies Intervention Outcome Intervention & Outcome Region Implementer P RESS 46.1 45.6 46 45.5 46.3 P RESSSim 47.2 49 49.9 46 46.4 p-value 0.063 <0.001 <0.001 0.021 0.764 RP2 r -0.01 0.01 0.00 0.00 -0.01 RP2 rSim -0.03 -0.07 -0.09 -0.01 -0.01 RP2 r -RP2 rSim 0.02 0.07 0.09 0.01 0.00 One may be concerned that low-quality papers are either inflating or depressing the degree of generalizability that is observed. There are infinitely many ways to measure paper “quality”; I consider two. First, I consider only those papers that were randomized controlled trials. Second, I use the most widely-used quality assessment measure, the Jadad scale (Jadad et al., 1996). The Jadad scale asks whether the study was randomized, double-blind, and whether there was a description of withdrawals and dropouts. A paper gets one point for having each of these charac- 26 Table 7: Within-intervention PRESS statistics and RP2 r CCTs C dummies Outcome Region Implementer Deworming C dummies Outcome Region Implementer P RESS 6.9 6.5 6.6 P RESSSim 7.1 6.8 6.6 p-value 0.676 0.215 0.762 RP2 r -0.09 -0.02 -0.04 RP2 rSim -0.12 -0.07 -0.04 RP2 r -RP2 rSim 0.03 0.05 0.00 P RESS 10.7 10.4 10.4 P RESSSim 10.8 10.5 10.5 p-value 0.924 0.309 0.543 RP2 r -0.05 -0.02 -0.02 RP2 rSim -0.06 -0.03 -0.03 RP2 r -RP2 rSim 0.01 0.01 0.01 Table 8: PRESS statistics and RP2 r using Within-Intervention-Outcome Meta-Analysis Results to Predict Estimates from Different Implementers Meta-Analysis Result M (on full sample) M (on govt only) M (on acad/NGO only) P RESS 41.9 9.3 32.4 P RESSSim 46 9.9 35.7 p-value <0.001 0.028 <0.001 RP2 r 0.09 0.02 0.09 RP2 rSim 0.00 -0.04 -0.01 RP2 r -RP2 rSim 0.09 0.06 0.09 teristics; in addition, a point is added if the method of randomization was appropriate, subtracted if the method is inappropriate, and similarly added if the blinding method was appropriate and subtracted if inappropriate. This results in a 0-5 point scale. Given that the kinds of interventions being tested are not typically readily suited to blinding, I consider all those papers scoring at least a 3 to be “high quality”. Tables 13 and 14 in the Appendix provide robustness checks using these two quality measures. Table 15 also considers those intervention-outcome combinations with at least four papers; Table 16 includes the one observation previously dropped for having an effect size more than 2 SD away from 0; in Table 17, I use a fixed effect meta-analysis to create the leave-one-out meta-analysis results. To illustrate the mechanics more clearly, Figures 4 and 5 show the density of results according to a hierarchical Bayesian model with an uninformative prior.10 Figure 4 shows the results within one intervention-outcome combination: conditional cash transfers and enrollment rates. The dark dots correspond to the aggregated estimates of the government versions of the interventions; the light, the academic/NGO versions. The still lighter dots found in figures in the Appendix repre10 Code adapted from Hsiang, Burke and Miguel (2013), based on Gelman et al., 2013). 27 Table 9: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results Meta-analysis result Observations R2 (1) Effect size b/se (2) Effect size b/se (3) Effect size b/se 0.595*** (0.09) 0.530*** (0.05) 0.709*** (0.14) 566 0.08 103 0.15 439 0.08 The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows the results on the full data set; column (2) shows the results for those effect sizes pertaining to programs implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for academic/NGO-implemented programs. sent those papers with “other” implementers (collaborations or private sector implementers). The dashed black line shows the overall weighted mean. The two panels on the right side of Figure 4 give the weighted distribution of effects according to a hierarchical Bayesian model. In the first panel, all intervention-implementer pairs are pooled; in the second, they are disaggregated. This figure graphically depicts what a meta-analysis does. We can make a similar figure for each of the 48 intervention-outcome combinations on which we have sufficient papers (available online; for details, see Appendix A). To summarize results further, we may want to aggregate up to the intervention-implementer level. Figure 5 shows the effects if the meta-analysis were to aggregate across outcomes within an intervention-implementer as if they were independent. This figure is based on only those interventions that have been attempted by both a government agency and an academic team or NGO. The standard errors here are exceptionally small for a reason: each dot has aggregated all the results of all the different papers and outcomes under that intervention-implementer. Every time one aggregates in this fashion, even if one assumes that the multiple observations are correlated and corrects for this, the standard errors will decrease. The standard errors are thus more indicative of how much data I have on those particular programs - not much, for example, in the case of academic/NGO-implemented unconditional cash transfers. Figure 5 is simply for illustrative purposes, as collapsing across outcomes may not make sense if 28 Effect size (in SD) Figure 4: Example of Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: Conditional Cash Transfers and Enrollment Rates 2.0 2.0 1.5 1.5 − − 1.0 1.0 − − − − − − − − 0.5 0.0 − − − − − − − − 0.0 Academic (light) vs. Government (dark) All studies Mo et al. (2013) Baird et al. (2011) Edmonds and Schady (2012) Ferreira, Filmer and Schady (2009) Behrman, Parker and Todd (2004) Akresh, de Walque and Kazianga (2013) Angelucci et al. (2010) Maluccio, Murphy and Regalia (2009) Galasso (2006) Galiani and McEwan (2013) Arraiz and Rozo (2011) de Brauw and Gilligan (2011) Davis et al. (2002) de Janvry, Finan and Sadoulet (2012) Chaudhury, Friedman and Onishi (2013) Ham (2010) Perova (2010) Garcia and Hill (2009) Dubois, de Janvry and Sadoulet (2012) Ward et al. (2010) Olinto and Souza (2005) Attanasio et al. (2010) Fuwa (2001) Ferro, Kassouf and Levison (2007) Rubio−Codina (2003) Gitter and Barham (2008) Barrera−Osorio et al. (2008) −0.5 Glewwe and Kassouf (2008) −0.5 0.5 This figure provides the point estimate and confidence interval for each paper’s estimated effect of a conditional cash transfer program (intervention) on enrollment rates (outcome). The first grey box on the right hand side of the figure shows the aggregate distribution of results using the hierarchical Bayesian procedure, and the second grey box farther to the right shows the distributions for the government-implemented and academic/NGO-implemented studies separately. Government-implemented programs are denoted by dark grey, while academic/NGO-implemented studies are in lighter grey. The even lighter dots in other figures found in the Appendix represent those papers with “other” implementers (collaborations or private sector implementers). The data have been converted to standard deviations for later comparison with other outcomes. Conventions were followed to isolate one result per paper. These are detailed in the Appendix and in the online coding manual, but the main criteria were to use the result with the fewest controls; if results for multiple time periods were presented, the time period closest to those examined in other papers in the same intervention-outcome was selected; if results for multiple subgroups were presented, such as different age ranges, results were aggregated as these data were typically too sparse to do subgroup analyses. Thus, the data have already been slightly aggregated in this figure. the outcomes are not comparable. Still, unless one expects a systematic bias affecting governmentimplemented programs differently vis-a-vis academic/NGO-implemented programs, it is clear that the distribution of the programs’ effects looks quite different, and the academic/NGO-implemented programs routinely exhibit higher effect sizes than their government-implemented counterparts. Figure 5 also illustrates the limitations of meta-analyses: while the academic/NGO-implemented 29 1.5 1.5 1.0 1.0 0.5 − − − − − − 0.0 − − − − − − 0.5 0.0 Academic (light) vs. Government (dark) All studies HIV/AIDS Education Deworming School meals −1.5 Financial literacy −1.5 Performance pay −1.0 Micronutrient supplementation −1.0 Contract teachers −0.5 Microfinance −0.5 Conditional cash transfers Effect size (in SD) Figure 5: Government- and Academic/NGO- Implemented Projects Differ Within the Same Interventions This figure focuses only on those interventions for which there were both government-implemented and academic/NGO-implemented studies. All outcomes and papers are aggregated within each intervention-implementer combination. While it appears that the academic/NGO-implemented studies do better overall, the higher weighting of some of the better government-implemented interventions (in particular, conditional cash transfer programs, which tend to have very large sample sizes) disguises this. This figure both illustrates that academic/NGO-implemented programs seem to do better than government-implemented programs and shows why caution must be taken in interpreting results. studies do better overall, the higher weighting of some of the better government-implemented interventions hides this in the results in the side panel. This again points to the fact that what one is aggregating over and the weighting scheme used is important. 5 Specification searching and publication bias Results on generalizability could be biased in the presence of specification searching and publi- cation bias. In particular, if studies are systematically biased, they could speciously appear to be more generalizable. In this section, I examine these issues. First, I test for specification searching and publication bias, finding that these biases are quite limited in my data, especially among randomized controlled trials. I then suggest a mathematical correction that could be applied, much in 30 the spirit of Simonsohn et al.’s p-curve (2014), which looks at the distribution of p-values one would expect given a true effect size. I run some simulations that show that my mathematical correction would recover the correct distribution of effect sizes. However, since that approach depends on a key assumption that there is one true effect size and all deviations are noise, and one might want to weaken that assumption, I show how one might do that. Finally, I restrict attention to just the subset of studies that had been RCTs, as I showed they did not appear subject to the same biases, and repeat the earlier analyses. 5.1 5.1.1 Specification searching: how bad is it? Method To examine the issue of specification searching, I start by conducting a series of caliper tests, following Gerber and Malhotra (2008a). As they describe (2008b), even if results are naturally concentrated in a given range, one should expect to see roughly comparable numbers of results just on either side of any threshold when restricting attention to a narrow enough band. I consider the ranges 2.5%, 5%, 10%, 15% and 20% above and below z=1.96, in turn, and examine whether results follow a binomial distribution around 1.96 as one would expect in the absence of bias. I do these tests on the full data set - here there is no need to consider only those results for intervention-outcome combinations covered by a certain number of papers, for example - but then also break it down in several ways, such as by RCT or non-RCT and government-implemented or non-government-implemented. When doing this kind of analysis, one should also carefully consider the issues arising from having multiple coefficients coming from the same papers. Gerber and Malhotra (2008a; 2008b) address the issue by breaking down their results by the number of coefficients contributed by each paper, so as to separately show the results for those papers that contribute one coefficient, two coefficients, and so on. I also do this, but in addition use the common statistical method of aggregating the results by paper, so that, for example, a paper with four coefficients below the threshold and three above it would be counted as “below”. The approach followed in Gerber and Malhotra (2008a; 2008b) retains slightly different information. While it preserves the number of coefficients on either side of the threshold, it does not reduce the bias that may be present if one 31 or two of the papers are responsible for much of the effect. By presenting a set of results collapsed by paper, I can test if results are sensitive to this. 5.1.2 Results I begin by simply plotting the distribution of z-statistics in the data for different groups. The distributions, shown in Figure 6, are consistent with specification searching, particularly for government-implemented programs and non-RCTs: while noise remains, mostly from using multiple results from the same study, which tend to be clustered, there appears to be a bit of a deviation from the downward trend around 1.96, the threshold for statistical significance at the 5% level for a two-sided test. These are “de-rounded” figures, accounting for the fact that papers may have presented results which were imprecise; for example, specifying a point estimate of 0.03 and a standard error of 0.01. Since these results would artificially cause spikes in the distribution, I re-drew their z-statistics from the uniform range of possible results ( 0.025 0.015 ´ 0.035 0.05 ), as in Brodeur et al. (2012). Figure 6: Distribution of z-statistics This figure shows histograms of the z-statistics, by implementer and whether the result was from an RCT. A jump around 1.96, the threshold for significance at the 5% level, would suggest that authors were wittingly or unwittingly selecting significant results for inclusion. 32 Table 10: Caliper Tests: By Result All studies 2.5% Caliper 5% Caliper 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper RCTs 2.5% Caliper 5% Caliper 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper Non-RCTs 2.5% Caliper 5% Caliper 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper Over Caliper Under Caliper p-value 119 190 353 519 675 92 176 357 526 706 <0.10 79 123 253 376 498 74 135 274 404 543 40 67 100 143 177 18 41 83 122 163 <0.01 <0.05 Table 11: Caliper Tests: By Paper All studies 2.5% Caliper 5% Caliper 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper RCTs 2.5% Caliper 5% Caliper 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper Non-RCTs 2.5% Caliper 5% Caliper 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper Over Caliper Under Caliper 54 71 81 100 104 43 71 101 119 129 35 51 59 71 72 36 57 80 91 96 19 20 22 29 32 7 14 21 28 33 33 p-value <0.10 <0.10 <0.05 Overall, these figures look much better than the typical ones in the literature. I designed AidGrade’s coding conventions partially to minimize bias,11 which could help explain the difference. The government z-statistics are perhaps the most interesting. While they may reflect noise rather than bias, it would be intuitive for governments to exert pressure over their evaluators to find significant, positive effects. Suppose there are two ways of obtaining a significant effect size: putting effort into the intervention (such as increasing inputs) or putting effort into leaning on the evaluators. For large-scale projects, it would seem much more efficient to target the evaluator. While this story would be consistent with the observed evidence, many other explanations remain possible. Turning to the caliper tests, I still find little evidence of bias (Tables 10 and 11). For the caliper tests, I use the raw rather than de-rounded data, as de-rounding could mask subtle jumps at z=1.96. Whether considering results independently or collapsed by paper, non-RCTs appear to suffer from bias, but RCTs perform much better. It should be recalled that as the distribution of the z-statistics is skewed, we should expect to see fewer results just over as opposed to just under the threshold for significance for a wide enough band, which is indeed what we see for RCTs. The results, especially for RCTs, mark a great difference from Gerber and Malhotra’s results on the political science (2008a) or sociology (2008b) literature. I reproduce one of their tables in the Appendix to illustrate (Table 19), as the difference between my results and their row of mostly p ă 0.001 results is striking. 5.2 Publication bias Turning to publication bias, published impact evaluations are more likely to have significant findings than working papers, as we can see in Table 12. RCTs are also greatly selected for publication. However, once controlling for whether a study is an RCT, publication bias is reduced. As discussed, an attempt was made to be very comprehensive in the data gathering process, and both published and unpublished papers were searched for and included. I can therefore re-run the main regressions separately for published and unpublished papers (Table 18 in the Appendix). Results are fairly similar. The coefficient on the meta-analysis term is marginally not significant 11 Such as by focusing on the specifications with fewest controls and only collecting subgroup data where results for all subgroups were reported. 34 Table 12: Publication Bias (1) Published b/se Published RCT (2) Published b/se (3) Published b/se 1.855*** (0.27) 2.796*** (1.11) 1.227 (0.37) 1.690 (0.59) 9381 9381 4.429*** (0.85) Significant RCT*Significant Observations 637 Exponentiated coefficients Each column presents the results of a logistic regression of whether a study or result was published on different characteristics. Column (1) considers papers; Columns (2) and (3) consider results for simplicity, though the decision of whether or not a paper will be published could be a more complicated function of the individual results’ significance. for unpublished papers (p=0.12), but this appears likely influenced by the small sample size; the coefficient is of a similar magnitude, and if I run the meta-analyses on the full data set and then restrict attention to the subset of unpublished papers (reducing noise), rather than first restricting attention to the unpublished papers and then generating the meta-analysis results, the coefficient is again highly significant (p¡0.001).12 In short, published and unpublished papers appear roughly comparable in terms of generalizability; it therefore can be hoped that if any papers were not only not published but also “file drawered”, i.e. not even available as a working paper, this may not be too much of a concern. Since I cannot say with certainty that “file drawered” studies are similar to unpublished studies, or that “file drawered” studies are necessarily to unpublished studies what unpublished studies are to published studies, this paper ultimately can only speak to the results of papers that do come out in some form. Finally, we might believe that earlier or later papers on the same intervention-outcome combination might show systematically different results. On the one hand, later authors might face pressure to find large or different results from earlier studies in order to publish better. In other disciplines, one can also sometimes observe a “decline effect”, whereby early, published results were 12 Results are available upon request. 35 Figure 7: Variance of Results Over Time, Within Intervention-Outcome much more promising than later replications, possibly due to publication bias. To investigate this, I generate the absolute value of the percent difference between a particular result and the mean result in that intervention-outcome combination. I then compare this with the chronological order of the paper relative to others on the same intervention-outcome, scaled to run from 0 to 1. For example, if there were 5 papers on a particular intervention-outcome combination, the first would take the value 0.2, the last, 1. Figure 7 provides a scatter plot of the relationship between the absolute percent difference and the chronological order variables, restricting attention to those percent differences less than 1000%. There is a weak positive relationship between them, indicating that earlier results tend to be closer to the mean result than the later results, which are more variable, but this is not significant. Further, the relationship varies according to the cutoff used. Table 20 in Appendix C illustrates. One could also look at how results vary by journal rankings, but there are relatively few papers published in economics journals that have a ranking assigned to them as most rankings focus on e.g. the top 500, as in RePEc, so the data would not yet be sufficient; future work can address this issue. 36 5.3 Identifying bias vs. heterogeneous effects Generalizability and specification searching and publication bias are intimately related. This can be seen by considering how one might correct the bias among the papers in a single interventionoutcome combination. If one were to consider these papers to be estimating the same true effect, as in the fixed effects model,13 one would immediately know the variance of the effect sizes. In a fixed effects model, it is always the same. Yi is normally distributed, with variance equal to 2{n. This can be seen by considering that the only variation in Yi is assumed to be due to sampling variance, σ 2 {n, and since Yi is standardized, σ 2 =1, resulting in 2{n for two samples. This implies that if one believed the fixed effects model were true, one would not even need to correct the effect size estimates Yi in order to know their variance. For some of the measures of generalizability previously discussed, such as a simple PRESS statistic which tried to see how well the group mean effect size, excepting i, predicted Yi , we can pin down the correct statistic using only the sample size.14 This serves to underscore the inappropriateness of the fixed effect model when considering generalizability. Bias and generalizability are not separately identified by themselves. If one brought data from a random effects model and tried to fit it to a fixed effects model, the real differences in effect size would appear as “bias”. There are a few possible solutions. A random effects model would seem to be better as it allows the possibility of heterogeneous treatment effects. However, one would still have to be confident that one could distinguish bias from heterogeneous effects; one needs another lever. One possibility would be to exploit different levels of the data. For example, perhaps if one looked within very narrow groups of results that could plausibly share a true effect size, one could reasonably consider each of those groups of results as fitting a separate fixed effects model and correct them before aggregating up in a hierarchical model. Concretely, if the goal were to have an unbiased random effects model within a particular intervention-outcome combination, one could correct the data at the paper level, supposing there were enough results within a paper on the 13 Used in e.g. Simonsohn et al. (2014). Other measures, such as the coefficient of variation, would also require adjusting θ, but this could also be easily obtained as in Simonsohn et al. (2014). 14 37 same outcome. I do not take this approach, because apart from being data-intensive, this approach requires having homogenous subgroups and, as previously shown, effect size estimates vary quite a bit even within the same intervention-outcome-paper in the data. Still, if one wanted to take this approach, I include simulations in Appendix D that show that one can indeed recover the original distribution of effect sizes to a close approximation. Instead, I turn to a different robustness check. Since I previously found that not only are the biases quite small overall, but RCTs exhibit even fewer signs of specification searching and publication bias, I thus re-run the main regression relating to generalizability on the subset of the data that were RCTs (Table 13, Appendix C). 6 Conclusion How much impact evaluation results generalize to other settings is an important topic, and data from meta-analyses are the ideal data with which to answer this question. With data on 20 different types of interventions, all collected in the same way, we can begin to speak a bit more generally about how results tend to vary across contexts and what that implies for impact evaluation design and policy recommendations. I started by defining generalizability and relating it to heterogeneous treatment effects models. After examining key summary statistics, I conducted leave-one-out hierarchical Bayesian metaanalyses within different narrowly-defined intervention-outcome combinations, separating the metaanalyses into different specifications by type of implementer. The results of these meta-analyses were significantly associated with the effect size left out at each iteration, with the typical coefficient on the meta-analysis result being approximately 0.5-0.7; it would be 1 if the estimate were identical to the meta-analysis result. However, the meta-analysis results were not significantly associated with the results of studies implemented by governments. Further, the effect sizes of governmentimplemented programs appeared to be lower even after controlling for sample size. This points to a potential problem when results from an academic/NGO-implemented study are expected to scale through government implementation. Specification searching and publication bias could affect both impact evaluation results and their generalizability, and I next turned to examine these topics. While each issue is present, neither 38 turns out to be particularly important in the data. RCTs fare better than non-RCTs. Overall, impact evaluation results were very heterogeneous. Even within the same paper, where one might expect contexts to be very similar, there was a high degree of variation within the same intervention-outcome combination. When considering the coefficient of variation, a unitless figure that can be compared across outcomes, within-paper variation was 67% of across-paper variation. Both, however, were quite high, at 1.2 and 1.9, respectively; in comparison, results in the medical literature might have coefficients of variation of about 0.1-0.5. The average across-paper, withinintervention-outcome coefficient of variation for the set of papers covering the same program was 1.5. There are some steps that researchers can take that may improve the generalizability of their own studies. First, just as with heterogeneous selection into treatment (Chassang, Padró i Miquel and Snowberg, 2012), one solution would be to ensure one’s impact evaluation varied some of the contextual variables that we might think underlie the heterogeneous treatment effects. Given that many studies are underpowered as it is, that may not be likely; however, large organizations and governments have been supporting more impact evaluations, providing more opportunities to explicitly integrate these analyses. Efforts to coordinate across different studies, asking the same questions or looking at some of the same outcome variables, would also help. The framing of heterogeneous treatment effects could also provide positive motivation for replication projects in different contexts: different findings would not necessarily negate the earlier ones but add another level of information. In summary, generalizability is not binary but something that we can measure. This paper showed that past results have significant but limited ability to predict other results on the same topic and this was not seemingly due to bias. Knowing how much results tend to extrapolate and when is critical if we are to know how to interpret an impact evaluation’s results or apply its findings. 39 References AidGrade (2013). “AidGrade Process Description”, http://www.aidgrade.org/methodology/ processmap-and-methodology, March 9, 2013. AidGrade (2015). “AidGrade Impact Evaluation Data, Version 1.1”. Alesina, Alberto and David Dollar (2000). “Who Gives Foreign Aid to Whom and Why?”, Journal of Economic Growth, vol. 5 (1). Allcott, Hunt (forthcoming). “Site Selection Bias in Program Evaluation”, Quarterly Journal of Economics. Bastardi, Anthony, Eric Luis Uhlmann and Lee Ross (2011). “Wishful Thinking: Belief, Desire, and the Motivated Evaluation of Scientific Evidence”, Psychological Science. Becker, Betsy Jane and Meng-Jia Wu (2007). “The Synthesis of Regression Slopes in MetaAnalysis”, Statistical Science, vol. 22 (3). Bold, Tessa et al. (2013). “Scaling-up What Works: Experimental Evidence on External Validity in Kenyan Education”, working paper. Borenstein, Michael et al. (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Wiley Publishers. Boriah, Shyam et al. (2008). “Similarity Measures for Categorical Data: A Comparative Evaluation”, in Proceedings of the Eighth SIAM International Conference on Data Mining. Brodeur, Abel et al. (2012). “Star Wars: The Empirics Strike Back”, working paper. Cartwright, Nancy (2007). Hunting Causes and Using Them: Approaches in Philosophy and Economics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cartwright, Nancy (2010). “What Are Randomized Controlled Trials Good For?”, Philosophical Studies, vol. 147 (1): 59-70. Casey, Katherine, Rachel Glennerster, and Edward Miguel (2012). “Reshaping Institutions: Evidence on Aid Impacts Using a Preanalysis Plan.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, vol. 127 (4): 1755-1812. Chassang, Sylvain, Gerard Padr I Miquel, and Erik Snowberg (2012). “Selective Trials: A PrincipalAgent Approach to Randomized Controlled Experiments.” American Economic Review, vol. 102 (4): 1279-1309. Deaton, Angus (2010). “Instruments, Randomization, and Learning about Development.” Journal 40 of Economic Literature, vol. 48 (2): 424-55. Duflo, Esther, Pascaline Dupas and Michael Kremer (2012). “School Governance, Teacher Incentives and Pupil-Teacher Ratios: Experimental Evidence from Kenyan Primary Schools”, NBER Working Paper. Evans, David and Anna Popova (2014). “Cost-effectiveness Measurement in Development: Accounting for Local Costs and Noisy Impacts”, World Bank Policy Research Working Paper, No. 7027. Ferguson, Christopher and Michael Brannick (2012). “Publication bias in psychological science: Prevalence, methods for identifying and controlling, and implications for the use of meta-analyses.” Psychological Methods, vol. 17 (1), Mar 2012, 120-128. Franco, Annie, Neil Malhotra and Gabor Simonovits (2014). “Publication Bias in the Social Sciences: Unlocking the File Drawer”, Working Paper. Gerber, Alan and Neil Malhotra (2008a). “Do Statistical Reporting Standards Affect What Is Published? Publication Bias in Two Leading Political Science Journals”, Quarterly Journal of Political Science, vol 3. Gerber, Alan and Neil Malhotra (2008b). “Publication Bias in Empirical Sociological Research: Do Arbitrary Significance Levels Distort Published Results”, Sociological Methods &Research, vol. 37 (3). Gelman, Andrew et al. (2013). Bayesian Data Analysis, Third Edition, Chapman and Hall/CRC. Hedges, Larry and Therese Pigott (2004). “The Power of Statistical Tests for Moderators in MetaAnalysis”, Psychological Methods, vol. 9 (4). Higgins JPT and S Green, (eds.) (2011). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions, Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration. Available from www.cochrane-handbook.org. Hsiang, Solomon, Marshall Burke and Edward Miguel (2013). “Quantifying the Influence of Climate on Human Conflict”, Science, vol. 341. Independent Evaluation Group (2012). “World Bank Group Impact Evaluations: Relevance and Effectiveness”, World Bank Group. Jadad, A.R. et al. (1996). “Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: Is blinding necessary?” Controlled Clinical Trials, 17 (1): 112. 41 Millennium Challenge Corporation (2009). “Key Elements of Evaluation at MCC”, presentation June 9, 2009. Page, Matthew, McKenzie, Joanne and Andrew Forbes (2013). “Many Scenarios Exist for Selective Inclusion and Reporting of Results in Randomized Trials and Systematic Reviews”, Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, vol. 66 (5). Pritchett, Lant and Justin Sandefur (2013). “Context Matters for Size: Why External Validity Claims and Development Practice Don’t Mix”, Center for Global Development Working Paper 336. Rodrik, Dani (2009). “The New Development Economics: We Shall Experiment, but How Shall We Learn?”, in What Works in Development? Thinking Big, and Thinking Small, ed. Jessica Cohen and William Easterly, 24-47. Washington, D.C.: Brookings Institution Press. Saavedra, Juan and Sandra Garcia (2013). “Educational Impacts and Cost-Effectiveness of Conditional Cash Transfer Programs in Developing Countries: A Meta-Analysis”, CESR Working Paper. Simmons, Joseph and Uri Simonsohn (2011). “False-Positive Psychology: Undisclosed Flexibility in Data Collection and Analysis Allows Presenting Anything as Significant”, Psychological Science, vol. 22. Simonsohn, Uri et al. (2014). “P-Curve: A Key to the File Drawer”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Tibshirani, Ryan and Robert Tibshirani (2009). “A Bias Correction for the Minimum Error Rate in Cross-Validation”, Annals of Applied Statistics, vol. 3 (2). Tierney, Michael J. et al. (2011). “More Dollars than Sense: Refining Our Knowledge of Development Finance Using AidData”, World Development, vol. 39. Tipton, Elizabeth (2013). “Improving generalizations from experiments using propensity score subclassification: Assumptions, properties, and contexts”, Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 38: 239-266. RePEc (2013). “RePEc h-index for journals”, http://ideas.repec.org/top/ top.journals.hindex.html. Vivalt, Eva (2015). “Selection Bias in Impact Evaluations: Evidence from the World Bank”, Working Paper. Walsh, Michael et. al. (2013). “The Statistical Significance of Randomized Controlled Trial Results 42 is Frequently Fragile: A Case for a Fragility Index”, Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. USAID (2011). “Evaluation: Learning from Experience”, USAID Evaluation Policy, Washington, DC. 43 Appendices A Guide to Appendices A.1 Appendices in this Paper B) Excerpt from AidGrade’s Process Description (2013). C) Additional results. D) Simulations showing the recoverability of the distribution of unbiased data in fixed effect models. A.2 Further Online Appendices Having to describe data from twenty different meta-analyses and systematic reviews, I must rely in part on online appendices. The following are available at http://www.evavivalt.com/research: E) The search terms and inclusion criteria for each topic. F) The references for each topic. G) The coding manual. H) Figures showing hierarchical Bayesian meta-analysis results for each intervention-outcome combination. 44 B Data Collection B.1 Description of AidGrade’s Methodology The following details of AidGrade’s data collection process draw heavily from AidGrade’s Process Description (AidGrade, 2013). Figure 8: Process Description Stage 1: Topic Identification AidGrade staff members were asked to each independently make a list of at least thirty international development programs that they considered to be the most interesting. The independent lists were appended into one document and duplicates were tagged and removed. Each of the 45 remaining topics was discussed and refined to bring them all to a clear and narrow level of focus. Pilot searches were conducted to get a sense of how many impact evaluations there might be on each topic, and all the interventions for which the very basic pilot searches identified at least two impact evaluations were shortlisted. A random subset of the topics was selected, also acceding to a public vote for the most popular topic. Stage 2: Search Each search engine has its own peculiarities. In order to ensure all relevant papers and few irrelevant papers were included, a set of simple searches was conducted on different potential search engines. First, initial searches were run on AgEcon; British Library for Development Studies (BLDS); EBSCO; Econlit; Econpapers; Google Scholar; IDEAS; JOLISPlus; JSTOR; Oxford Scholarship Online; Proquest; PubMed; ScienceDirect; SciVerse; SpringerLink; Social Science Research Network (SSRN); Wiley Online Library; and the World Bank eLibrary. The list of potential search engines was compiled broadly from those listed in other systematic reviews. The purpose of these initial searches was to obtain information about the scope and usability of the search engines to determine which ones would be effective tools in identifying impact evaluations on different topics. External reviews of different search engines were also consulted, such as a Falagas et al. (2008) study which covered the advantages and differences between the Google Scholar, Scopus, Web of Science and PubMed search engines. Second, searches were conducted for impact evaluations of two test topics: deworming and toilets. EBSCO, IDEAS, Google Scholar, JOLISPlus, JSTOR, Proquest, PubMed, ScienceDirect, SciVerse, SpringerLink, Wiley Online Library and the World Bank eLibrary were used for these searches. 9 search strings were tried for deworming and up to 33 strings for toilets, with modifications as needed for each search engine. For each search the number of results and the number of results out of the first 10-50 results which appeared to be impact evaluations of the topic in question were recorded. This gave a better sense of which search engines and which kinds of search strings would return both comprehensive and relevant results. A qualitative assessment of the search results was also provided for the Google Scholar and SciVerse searches. Finally, the online databases of J-PAL, IPA, CEGA and 3ie were searched. Since these databases are already narrowly focused on impact evaluations, attention was restricted to simple keyword 46 searches, checking whether the search engines that were integrated with each database seemed to pull up relevant results for each topic. Ultimately, Google Scholar and the online databases of J-PAL, IPA, CEGA and 3ie, along with EBSCO/PubMed for health-related interventions, were selected for use in the full searches. After the interventions of interest were identified, search strings were developed and tested using each search source. Each search string included methodology-specific stock keywords that narrowed the search to impact evaluation studies, except for the search strings for the J-PAL, IPA, CEGA and 3ie searches, as these databases already exclusively focus on impact evaluations. Experimentation with keyword combinations in stages 1.4 and 2.1 was helpful in the development of the search strings. The search strings could take slightly different forms for different search engines. Search terms were tailored to the search source, and a full list is included in an appendix. C# was used to write a script to scrape the results from search engines. The script was programmed to ensure that the Boolean logic of the search string was properly applied within the constraints of each search engines capabilities. Some sources were specialized and could have useful papers that do not turn up in simple searches. The papers listed on J-PAL, IPA, CEGA and 3ies websites are a good example of this. For these sites, it made more sense for the papers to be manually searched and added to the relevant spreadsheets. After the automated and manual searches were complete, duplicates were removed by matching on author and title names. During the title screening stage, the consolidated list of citations yielded by the scraped searches was checked for any existing meta-analyses or systematic reviews. Any papers that these papers included were added to the list. With these references added, duplicates were again flagged and removed. Stage 3: Screening Generic and topic-specific screening criteria were developed. The generic screening criteria are detailed below, as is an example of a set of topic-specific screening criteria. The screening criteria were very inclusive overall. This is because AidGrade purposely follows a different approach to most meta-analyses in the hopes that the data collected can be re-used by researchers who want to focus on a different subset of papers. Their motivation is that vast 47 resources are typically devoted to a meta-analysis, but if another team of researchers thinks a different set of papers should be used, they will have scour the literature and recreate the data from scratch. If the two groups disagree, all the public sees are their two sets of findings and their reasoning for selecting different papers. AidGrade instead strives to cover the superset of all impact evaluations one might wish to include along with a list of their characteristics (e.g. where they were conducted, whether they were randomized by individual or by cluster, etc.) and let people set their own filters on the papers or select individual papers and view the entire space of possible results. 48 Figure 9: Generic Screening Criteria Category Methodologies Inclusion Criteria Impact evaluations that have counterfactuals Publication status Time period of study LocationGeography Quality Peer-reviewed or working paper Any Any Any Exclusion Criteria Observational studies, strictly qualitative studies N/A N/A N/A N/A Figure 10: Topic-Specific Criteria Example: Formal Banking Category Intervention Outcomes Inclusion Criteria Formal banking services specifically including: - Expansion of credit and/or savings - Provision of technological innovations - Introduction or expansion of financial education, or other program to increase financial literacy or awareness - Individual and household income - Small and micro-business income - Household and business assets - Household consumption - Small and micro-business investment - Small, micro-business or agricultural output - Measures of poverty - Measures of well-being or stress - Business ownership - Any other outcome covered by multiple papers Exclusion Criteria Other formal banking services Microfinance N/A Figure 11 illustrates the difference. For this reason, minimal screening was done during the screening stage. Instead, data was collected broadly and re-screening was allowed at the point of doing the analysis. This is highly beneficial for the purpose of this paper, as it allows us to look at the largest possible set of papers and all subsets. After screening criteria were developed, two volunteers independently screened the titles to determine which papers in the spreadsheet were likely to meet the screening criteria developed in Stage 3.1. Any differences in coding were arbitrated by a third volunteer. All volunteers received training before beginning, based on the AidGrade Training Manual and a test set of entries. Volunteers’ training inputs were screened to ensure that only proficient volunteers would be allowed to continue. 49 Figure 11: AidGrade’s Strategy 50 Of those papers that passed the title screening, two volunteers independently determined whether the papers in the spreadsheet met the screening criteria developed in Stage 3.1 judging by the paper abstracts. Any differences in coding were again arbitrated by a third volunteer. The full text was then found for those papers which passed both the title and abstract checks. Any paper that proved not to be a relevant impact evaluation using the aforementioned criteria was discarded at this stage. Stage 4: Coding Two AidGrade members each independently used the data extraction form developed in Stage 4.1 to extract data from the papers that passed the screening in Stage 3. Any disputes were arbitrated by a third AidGrade member. These AidGrade members received much more training than those who screened the papers, reflecting the increased difficulty of their work, and also did a test set of entries before being allowed to proceed. The data extraction form was organized into three sections: (1) general identifying information; (2) paper and study characteristics; and (3) results. Each section contained qualitative and quantitative variables that captured the characteristics and results of the study. Stage 5: Analysis A researcher was assigned to each meta-analysis topic who could specialize in determining which of the interventions and results were similar enough to be combined. If in doubt, researchers could consult the original papers. In general, researchers were encouraged to focus on all the outcome variables for which multiple papers had results. When a study had multiple treatment arms sharing the same control, researchers would check whether enough data was provided in the original paper to allow estimates to be combined before the meta-analysis was run. This is a best practice to avoid double-counting the control group; for details, see the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (2011). If a paper did not provide sufficient data for this, the researcher would make the decision as to which treatment arm to focus on. Data were then standardized within each topic to be more comparable before analysis (for example, units were converted). The subsequent steps of the meta-analysis process are irrelevant for the purposes of this paper. 51 It should be noted that the first set of ten topics followed a slightly different procedure for stages (1) and (2). Only one list of potential topics was created in Stage 1.1, so Stage 1.2 (Consolidation of Lists) was only vacuously followed. There was also no randomization after public voting (Stage 1.7) and no scripted scraping searches (Stage 2.3), as all searches were manually conducted using specific strings. A different search engine was also used: SciVerse Hub, an aggreator that includes SciVerse Scopus, MEDLINE, PubMed Central, ArXiv.org, and many other databases of articles, books and presentations. The search strings for both rounds of meta-analysis, manual and scripted, are detailed in another appendix. 52 C Additional Results 53 54 Intervention Conditional cash transfers Conditional cash transfers Conditional cash transfers Conditional cash transfers Conditional cash transfers Conditional cash transfers Conditional cash transfers Conditional cash transfers Conditional cash transfers Conditional cash transfers HIV/AIDS Education HIV/AIDS Education HIV/AIDS Education Unconditional cash transfers Unconditional cash transfers Unconditional cash transfers Insecticide-treated bed nets Contract teachers Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Deworming Financial literacy Improved stoves Improved stoves Improved stoves Improved stoves Irrigation Irrigation Microfinance Microfinance Microfinance Table 12: Descriptive Statistics: Standardized Narrowly Defined Outcomes Outcome # Neg sig papers # Insig papers Attendance rate 0 6 Enrollment rate 1 6 Height 0 1 Height-for-age 0 6 Labor force participation 1 12 Probability unpaid work 1 0 Test scores 1 2 Unpaid labor 0 2 Weight-for-age 0 2 Weight-for-height 0 1 Pregnancy rate 0 2 Probability has multiple sex partners 0 2 Used contraceptives 1 5 Enrollment rate 0 2 Test scores 0 1 Weight-for-height 0 2 Malaria 0 3 Test scores 0 1 Attendance rate 0 1 Birthweight 0 2 Diarrhea incidence 0 1 Height 3 10 Height-for-age 1 9 Hemoglobin 0 13 Malformations 0 2 Mid-upper arm circumference 2 0 Test scores 0 0 Weight 3 8 Weight-for-age 1 6 Weight-for-height 2 7 Savings 0 2 Chest pain 0 0 Cough 0 0 Difficulty breathing 0 0 Excessive nasal secretion 0 1 Consumption 0 1 Total income 0 1 Assets 0 3 Consumption 0 2 Profits 1 3 # Pos sig papers 9 21 1 1 5 4 2 3 0 1 0 0 4 0 1 0 6 2 1 0 1 4 4 2 0 5 2 7 5 2 3 2 2 2 1 1 1 1 0 1 # Papers 15 28 2 7 18 5 5 5 2 2 2 2 10 2 2 2 9 3 2 2 2 17 14 15 2 7 2 18 12 11 5 2 2 2 2 2 2 4 2 5 55 Microfinance Microfinance Micro health insurance Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Micronutrient supplementation Mobile phone-based reminders Mobile phone-based reminders Performance pay Rural electrification Rural electrification Rural electrification Safe water storage Scholarships Scholarships Scholarships School meals School meals School meals Water treatment Water treatment Women’s empowerment programs Savings Total income Enrollment rate Birthweight Body mass index Cough prevalence Diarrhea incidence Diarrhea prevalence Fever incidence Fever prevalence Height Height-for-age Hemoglobin Malaria Mid-upper arm circumference Mortality rate Perinatal deaths Prevalence of anemia Stillbirths Stunted Test scores Triceps skinfold measurement Wasted Weight Weight-for-age Weight-for-height Appointment attendance rate Treatment adherence Test scores Enrollment rate Study time Total income Diarrhea incidence Attendance rate Enrollment rate Test scores Enrollment rate Height-for-age Test scores Diarrhea incidence Diarrhea prevalence Savings 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 1 3 5 7 0 2 0 1 0 0 0 1 1 0 4 1 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 3 3 1 4 1 3 5 5 2 2 22 23 11 2 9 12 5 6 4 5 2 0 2 19 23 18 0 3 2 1 1 2 1 1 2 2 3 2 2 1 1 1 0 2 1 3 4 0 5 1 0 2 7 8 29 0 7 0 0 9 0 0 7 1 0 13 10 8 2 1 1 2 2 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 1 1 5 1 3 5 2 7 5 3 11 6 2 5 32 36 47 2 18 12 6 15 4 5 10 2 2 36 34 26 3 5 3 3 3 2 2 2 2 2 3 2 3 2 6 2 Women’s empowerment programs Average Total income 0 0.6 0 4.2 2 2.9 2 7.7 56 Table 13: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: RCTs Only Meta-analysis result Observations R2 (1) Effect size b/se (2) Effect size b/se (3) Effect size b/se 0.570*** (0.14) 0.290 (0.33) 0.654*** (0.15) 477 0.05 45 0.04 415 0.06 The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows the results on the full set of RCTs; column (2) shows the results for those effect sizes pertaining to programs implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for academic/NGO-implemented programs. 57 Table 14: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: Minimum Quality Score Meta-analysis result Observations R2 (1) Effect size b/se (2) Effect size b/se (3) Effect size b/se 0.534*** (0.13) 0.442 (0.38) 0.555*** (0.16) 417 0.05 22 0.10 381 0.04 The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows the results on the full set of “high-quality” papers according to the Jadad scale; column (2) shows the results for those effect sizes pertaining to programs implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for academic/NGO-implemented programs. 58 Table 15: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: Minimum Four Papers per Intervention-Outcome Meta-analysis result Observations R2 (1) Effect size b/se (2) Effect size b/se (3) Effect size b/se 0.682*** (0.12) 0.544 (0.45) 0.703*** (0.14) 539 0.07 92 0.04 427 0.07 The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows the results for all the intervention-outcome combinations covered by at least four papers; column (2) shows the results for those effect sizes pertaining to programs implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for academic/NGO-implemented programs. 59 Table 16: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: Including All Observations Meta-analysis result Observations R2 (1) Effect size b/se (2) Effect size b/se (3) Effect size b/se 0.580*** (0.09) 0.466*** (0.08) 0.708*** (0.14) 567 0.06 104 0.05 439 0.08 The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows the results on the full data set; column (2) shows the results for those effect sizes pertaining to programs implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for academic/NGO-implemented programs. 60 Table 17: Regression of Effect Size on Fixed Effect Meta-Analysis Results Meta-analysis result Observations R2 (1) Effect size b/se (2) Effect size b/se (3) Effect size b/se 0.657*** (0.08) 0.543*** (0.05) 0.814*** (0.07) 566 0.11 103 0.18 439 0.10 The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an intervention-outcome combination in a fixed effect meta-analysis; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows the results on the full data set; column (2) shows the results for those effect sizes pertaining to programs implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for academic/NGO-implemented programs. 61 Table 18: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: Published vs. Unpublished Papers Meta-analysis result (1) Published b/se (2) Unpublished b/se 0.600*** (0.14) 0.461 (0.30) 480 0.06 73 0.16 Observations R2 The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows the results using the set of papers that were published; column (2) shows the results using only unpublished or working papers. 62 Table 19: Caliper Tests for Political Science, Reproduced from Gerber and Malhotra (2008a) for Comparison A. APSR Vol. 89-101 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper Vol. 96-101 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper Vol. 89-95 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper B. AJPS Vol. 39-51 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper Vol. 46-51 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper Vol. 39-45 10% Caliper 15% Caliper 20% Caliper Over Caliper Under Caliper p-value 49 67 83 15 23 33 ă0.001 ă0.001 ă0.001 36 46 55 11 17 21 ă0.001 ă0.001 ă0.001 13 28 21 4 12 6 0.02 0.008 0.003 90 128 165 38 66 95 ă0.001 ă0.001 ă0.001 56 80 105 25 45 66 ă0.001 0.001 0.002 34 48 60 13 21 29 0.002 ă0.001 ă0.001 63 Table 20: Regression of Studies’ Absolute Percent Difference from the Within-InterventionOutcome Mean on Chronological Order Chronological order Observations R2 (1) ă500% b/se (2) ă1000% b/se (3) ă1500% b/se (4) ă2000% b/se 0.053 (0.21) 0.699 (0.47) 0.926 (0.61) 0.971 (0.67) 480 0.13 520 0.13 527 0.12 532 0.12 Each column restricts attention to a set of results a given maximum percentage away from the mean result in an intervention-outcome combination. 64 D Simulations I show that the distribution of standardized effect sizes is recoverable from a range of biases, including selection bias (dropping insignificant results), data-peeking (adding data until getting the results one wants), cherry-picking dependent variables (choosing dependent variables so as to get significance), and selectively excluding outliers. I use the same specifications (fixed effects vs. random effects, etc.) as Simonsohn et al. (2014). They take a vector of estimated effect sizes and return a scalar unbiased estimate of the effect size; I focus on the distribution of effect sizes. Each measures the PRESS statistic, defined as: PRESS “ n ÿ pYi ´ Ypi q2 (17) i“1 where Ypi is the predicted value for the effect size Yi based on all observations except i. Here, Ypi represents the leave-one-out mean effect size. Code is available at http://www.evavivalt.com/research. 65 Figure 12: Three selection bias simulations. Specifications, left to right: 1. Selectively reporting significant studies: N=40, 20 observations per cell, fixed effects. 2. Selectively reporting significant studies: Predetermined sample size between N=10 and N=70, 5-35 observations per cell, fixed effects. 3. Selectively reporting significant studies: Predetermined sample size between N=10 and N=70, 5-35 observations per cell, random effects: Yi „ N pθ̄, 0.2q. In each case, only significant values are kept. Figures contain 500 simulated studies. 66 Figure 13: Three p-hacking simulations. Specifications, left to right: 1. Data-peeking: If pą0.05 with N=20, add 10 observations. 2. Cherry-picking: If pą0.05 with one dependent variable, try another two. 3. Selectively excluding outliers: If pą0.05, drop observations more than 2 SD from the mean, first from one group, then the other, or both if necessary. In each case, only significant values are kept. Present figures contain 500 simulated studies. 67