Thomas, J.H., Bowling, J. R., Fox, R., & Dula, C. S. (2010, November).

advertisement
DWI (Driving While Impulsive): Investigating Relationships Between
Impulsivity and Dangerous Driving in a Simulated Environment
Jesse Thomas, Justin Bowling, Russell Fox, & Chris S. Dula
APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY LABORATORY
East Tennessee State University
http://www.etsu.edu/apl/
Johnson City, Tennessee
INTRODUCTION
METHOD
 Although innovations and improvements to automobile safety have
decreased some driving risks, motor vehicle crashes remain a serious
public health problem and are one of the leading causes of death in the
United States (Hilton & Shankar, 2003; NHTSA, 2003).
Participants:
 Participants were 26 undergraduate students from a small university in the
Southeastern United States.
 Impulsivity is a personality trait that has been defined as ‘‘a
predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internal or external
stimuli without regard to the negative consequences of these reactions to
oneself or others” (Moeller, Barratt, Dougherty, Schmitz, & Swann,
2001).
 The inability to control impulses can undermine the intentions of an
individual, leading to poor decision making (Nordgren, van der Pligt, &
Harreveld, 2007; Crocker, Major, & Steele, 1998).
 Previous research has reported that time perception is influenced by
impulsivity, which could contribute to impulsive individuals exhibiting an
faster cognitive processing speed with increased activity levels (Barratt,
2002)
 Dangerous driving behaviors include intentional acts of aggression,
negative emotions, and risk taking behaviors (Dula & Geller, 2003).
 Dangerous behaviors may occur without the presence of negative emotion
or intent to harm (Willemsen, Dula, Declercq, & Verhaeghe, 2008).
 The literature suggests that impulsiveness may increase the probability of
dangerous driving behaviors (Dahlen, Martin, Ragan, & Kuhlman, 2005;
Deffenbacher, Filetti, Richards, Lynch, & Oetting, 2003).
 This study investigated the relationship between levels of impulsivity,
reaction time, and dangerous driving behaviors.
 There were at total of 15 females (57.7%) and 11 males (42.3%), with a
range from 19 years of age to 39 years of age (Mean = 23.88, SD=5.63).
 Participants scheduled a time to come into the lab to participate in a the
driving simulator study, via an online participant managing system.
Measures:
 Vigil Continuous Performance Task (VCPT). Participants’ level of
impulsivity, as well as average reaction time in responding to stimuli, was
assessed by their total number of errors of commission and their average
delay time on the VCPT, respectively.
 STISIM Drive™ Computer Simulated Driving Program. Dependent
variables collected by the computer program included: total number of
speeding events, percent of driving time spent speeding, the total amount
of time taken to complete the course, total number of collisions, and the
total number of center line crossings.
Procedure:
 After obtaining informed consent, participants completed the Vigil
Continuous Performance Task (VCPT).
 Participants were then led to a computer driving simulator apparatus, and
instructed to drive the course as safely as possible and obey all traffic laws
while maneuvering through the course.
 Participants drove a course that was 6.44 miles in length. The course
included driving obstacles such as traffic light decisions, passing on a twolane road, and pedestrian crossings.
 Participant’s driving behavior was then assessed by calculating the total
number of dangerous driving behaviors listed above.
HYPOTHESES
RESULTS
 H1: It was hypothesized that there would be a significant relationship
between number of errors of commission on the Vigil Continuous
Performance Task and propensity to speed in a simulated driving
environment (i.e. number of speed exceedences, percent of time speeding,
and amount of time taken to complete the course).
 H1: There was a significant relationship between errors of commission on
the VCPT and the number of speed exceedences at a p<.05 level (r=.407).
There was a significant relationship between errors of commission on the
VCPT and amount of time taken to complete the course at the P<.01 level
(r=-.526). There was no significant relationship between errors of
commission on the VCPT and percentage of time spent speeding (r=.318).
 H2: It was also hypothesized that there would be a significant relationship
between number of errors of commission (impulsiveness) and average
delay time (reaction time) on the Vigil Continuous Performance Task.
 H3: Additionally it was hypothesized that there would be a significant
relationship between number of errors of commission on the Vigil
Continuous Performance Task and the other dangerous driving variables
listed in the Method section of this poster.
RESULTS (CONT.)
 H2 : It was also hypothesized that there would be a significant relationship
between number of errors of commission (impulsiveness) and average delay
time (reaction time) on the VCPT. There was a significant relationship
between these two measures on the P<.01 level (r = -.413)
 H3 : There was a significant relationship between the number of errors of
commission on the VCPT and the numbers of collisions at the P<.05
significance level (r=-.444). There was no significance between the errors
of commission on the VCPT and the number of centerline crossings (r = .285)
DISCUSSION
 Results showed a significant positive relationship between errors of commission on the
VCPT and the number of speed exceedences. This means that as errors of commission
increased, so did the number of speed exceedences. This suggests that individuals with
higher of levels of impulsivity may be inclined to speed a great number of times while
driving.
 Additionally a significant negative relationship between the number of errors of
commission on the VCPT and the amount of time taken to complete the simulated driving
course was observed. In other words, as errors of commission increased, course
completion time decreased. This would make sense if those with higher impulsivity did
indeed exceed the posted speed limit more often.
 There was no significant relationship between the errors of commission on the VCPT and
the total percent of time spent speeding. Since there was a positive relationship with the
number of speed occurrences, this could mean that individuals with higher levels of
impulsivity repeatedly corrected their speeds to the posted limit only to raise it again.
 There was a significant positive relationship between numbers of errors of commission
and average delay time on the VCPT. This means that as the number of errors of
commission increased, the average delay time also increased. This could suggest that the
individuals in this study with higher levels of impulsivity took longer to react to stimuli in
the VCPT.
 There was a significant positive relationship between the number of errors of commission
on the VCPT and the number of collisions in the simulated driving course. This means
that as the number of VCPT commission errors went up, so did collisions. There was no
significant difference between the number of VCPT commission errors and center line
crossings.
Limitation:
 Could have used a more diverse sample to see if findings generalize to populations other
than those consisting of college students.
Future Research:
 Future research should measure the time it takes drivers to react to obstacles in a simulated
driving environment.
REFERENCES
 References available upon request
CONTACT: Jesse H. Thomas thomasjh@goldmail.etsu.edu, Justin Bowling bowlingjr@goldmail.etsu.edu, Russell Fox foxr@goldmail.etsu..edu, or Chris S. Dula dulac@etsu.edu
Download