Eight Reasons to Doubt the Existence of a Geometric Module Nora S. Newcombe Temple University When Sociobiology Met Cognitive Psychology Modular mind Adaptive pressure works to select specific mental abilities Massive modularity Core knowledge Innateness These evolutionarily-selected modules are (naturally) innately specified Swiss Army Knife Analogy “The Swiss Army knife is a flexible tool [because] it is a bundle of tools, each welldesigned for solving a different problem – scissors for cutting paper, corkscrew for opening wine, toothpick for cleaning teeth…..Similarly, the human mind … contains a large number of programs, each well-designed for solving a different adaptive problem: choosing a good mate, caring for children, foraging for food, avoiding predators, navigating a landscape, forming coalitions, trading, defending one’s family against aggression, and so on”— Leda Cosmides Innately-Specified Modules Have Proliferated Language acquisition Face processing Theory of mind Cheater detection Geometric module What Do We Mean By Modularity? Modular cognitive systems are domain specific, innately specified, hard wired, autonomous, and not assembled. Fodor (1983, p. 37) Neural Specialization Does Not Entail Encapsulated Modularity Brain areas generally need to ‘talk to’ one another to support a function Case Study of the Geometric Module A representation of geometric information that guides reorientation following disorientation That does NOT use nongeometric information even when doing so would be advantageous Hermer & Spelke (1996): Search Rates for Toddlers White Room F R C F C .10 .39 .08 .31 .39 .12 .49 .12 N C = Correct N = Near R R = Reversal F = Far N Hermer & Spelke: Search Rates for Adults White Room F R C .02 .57 .41 0 F C 0 .04 N C = Correct N = Near R R = Reversal F = Far .96 0 N Language-as-Bridge Hypothesis Adults may have a further system of representation that is uniquely human and that emerges over the course of development. This system may connect to many other systems of representation, regardless of their domain-specific content. Its operation may be governed by rules and principles allowing the arbitrary combination of information from distinct, domain-specific sources….The language faculty appears to have all the right properties to serve as this uniquely human combinatorial system of representation. --Hermer-Vazquez, Spelke & Katsnelson (1999, p. 34) Support for Role of Language Transition to feature use at 6 years is correlated with productive use of left and right Training left and right leads to feature use Adults who do linguistic shadowing task concurrently do not use features Adaptive Combination Models Various sources of spatial information Ego-referenced: response learning and path integration Allo-referenced: cue learning, place learning Weighting depends on Salience Certainty and variability with which information is encoded Validity probabilities of finding objects given use of the information, derived from interaction with the environment Weighting develops both in real time and in developmental time Point 1: Evidence Against Encapsulation from Non-Human Animals Monkeys use colored walls and large but not small features (a sensible choice given likely cue validity) Other species Chickens Pigeons Fish See Cheng & Newcombe, PBR 2005, for review Without cue With cue 46 % 4.7 % 75 % 3.5 % 3.3 % 46 % 1.5 % 20 % Point 2: Featural Cues Are Only Neglected in Tiny Rooms 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 -10 Toddlers 3-4 Years 5 Years 6 Years Adults Larger Room Small Room Cheng & Newcombe (Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2005) Note: Perfect Performance = 100 Point 3: Use of Features Varies for Several Reasons Activity Active motion focuses spatial attention Active motion leads to remodeling of hippocampal firing Nature of landmarks More distal landmarks provide more useful and ecologically valid information Larger landmarks may be more salient and more likely to be stable Room Within Room Studies Small waist-high enclosure (HermerSpelke size) centered within “large” room (Learmonth et al. size) Large room had one colored wall Children stay within small enclosure Learmonth, Newcombe, Sheridan & Jones (Developmental Science, 2008) How the Data Fit An Adaptive Combination Model Distal Action Target Proximal Feature? Possible? to Feature? Yes Age at Success Hermer-Spelke No No 6 years Learmonth et al. Yes Yes Yes Study 1 Yes No No 6 years Studies 2 & 3 Yes No Yes 4 years 18 months (earliest tested) How the Data Fit An Adaptive Combination Model Distal Action Target Proximal Feature? Possible? to Feature? Yes Age at Success Hermer-Spelke No No 6 years Learmonth et al. Yes Yes Yes Study 1 Yes No No 6 years Studies 2 & 3 Yes No Yes 4 years 18 months (earliest tested) How the Data Fit An Adaptive Combination Model Distal Action Target Proximal Feature? Possible? to Feature? Yes Age at Success Hermer-Spelke No No 6 years Learmonth et al. Yes Yes Yes Study 1 Yes No No 6 years Studies 2 & 3 Yes No Yes 4 years 18 months (earliest tested) Point 4: Featural Cue Use is Easy to Get When Lacking Room within Room Switch-No Pen: 3 Years % of possible instances this box was selected 100 90 80 70 67.71 60 1st 4 Trials 2nd 4 Trials 50 40 30 20.83 20 10 4.17 7.29 0 Correct Opposite Near Far Box Selection Learmonth, Newcombe, Sheridan & Jones (Developmental Science, 2008) Similar finding: Twyman, Spetch & Friedman, (Developmental Psychology, 2007) Point 5: Spatial as Well as Verbal Shadowing Reduces Feature Use in Adults Ratliff & Newcombe, Cognitive Psychology, 2007 Also--Hupbach et al., Spatial Cognition & Computation, 2007 F * Usual results with white room and with colored wall but no concurrent task R C .06 .52 .33 .10 N Point 6: New Evidence from Conflict Paradigms When features are moved, subjects must choose a location based either on features or on geometry These paradigms reveal the fundamental similarity of human adults to children and nonhuman animals Ratliff & Newcombe, Psychological Science, 2008 Conflict Procedure “There are four hiding spots in this room, one at each corner” “I will hide a pair of keys in the same place every time” Conflict Procedure ►4 practice trials (target & landmark stable) ► Leave the room ► Brief delay ~ 2 minutes (drawing task) ► While the participant waits outside, the experimenter goes back into the room to move the landmark clockwise to the next adjacent wall ► Two conflict test trials Where are the keys? A D B C Experiment 1 TRAINING (Between Subjects) N = 32 TESTING Indirect Landmark E G G L E G G L Small room (4x6ft) n=8 Large room (8x12ft) n=8 Direct Landmark Small room (4x6ft) n=8 Large room (8x12ft) n=8 (Landmark = L, Geometrically appropriate = G, and Error = E) Adaptive Combination Predictions When forced to choose one cue over the other (geometry vs. features), conflict test will result in a room size effect Distal landmarks are more valid in the larger room Corners related to feature cues will be more likely to be chosen in the larger room Geometric cues are more salient in the smaller room Geometric cues will be chosen most often in the smaller room Results Significant Room Size effect (p < 0.01) Geometric information guided reorientation in the small room Features guided reorientation in the larger room SMALL ROOM TRAINING & TESTING E LARGE ROOM TRAINING & TESTING E 0 .43 G G (.06) .16 0 (.06) .38 G (.04) .19 (.05) L G .28 .56 (.06) (.06) (Landmark = L, Geometrically appropriate = G, and Error = E) L Experiment 2 How does prior experience impact feature use in a conflict situation? Replicates experiment 1 but switches room sizes between training and testing Predictions: Replicate no effect of landmark location (direct/indirect) Training in the larger room will increase feature use among adults reorienting in the small room Results Cross-experiment comparisons: Significant effect of training Feature use increased in small room testing from Exp. 1 (small room training) to Exp. 2 (large room training) LARGE ROOM TRAINING/ SMALL ROOM TESTING E 0 .03 G SMALL ROOM TRAINING/ LARGE ROOM TESTING G E (.02) G .03 .94 (.02) (.03) .10 0 (.04) L .10 .81 (.04) (.04) G (Landmark = L, Geometrically appropriate = G, and Error = E) L Conclusions Feature use is not associative: No effect of feature location (direct vs. indirect) in Exp 1 or 2 Experience is important As expected, large room experience boosts feature use for the small room test Reorientation depends on process of combining weights associated with features and geometry Not simply relying on the most salient cue Although the conflict procedure does not allow direct assessment of combination Point 7: Geometric Information May Be a Special Case of Relative Information The modularity position predicts failure to reorient in the absence of geometric information Is reorientation a more general discrimination of relative quantity task? Huttenlocher and Lourenco, 2007 Non-geometric, Relative Cue Non-relative Cues Why Might Relative Cues Be Easiest? Multiple cues Area, spatial density, and number Nominal versus ordinal scale “Cues which specify order along a magnitude scale (more, less) may be more easily mapped onto spatial position (left, right) than two distinct unordered properties (red, blue) which are mapped separately onto spatial position” (Huttenlocher & Lourenco, 2007) Is This Pattern True Across Species? Since the initial claims of modularity were made from animal research… Will mice replicate this pattern of results? C57 mice – 4 per group Non-geometric Non-geometric Non-geometric Relative Cue Non-Relative Non-Relative Results: Accuracy Twyman, Newcombe & Gould, J. Comparative Psychology, in press Small - Large Yellow - Blue Dots - Gray 81% 69% 69% Even though there is a trend for the relative cue group to be more accurate, there were no significant differences between groups Results: Trials to Criterion Twyman, Newcombe & Gould, J. Comparative Psychology, in press Small versus Large 12 trials (0.00) Yellow versus Blue 38 trials (5.77) Dot versus Gray 33 trials (6.81) The small-large group learned the task significantly faster than either the yellow-blue or the dot-gray group. Point 8: Use of Features is NOT Merely Associative One might have thought 7 points were enough—but the modularity theorists have a comeback-- The Module Strikes Back Lee, Shusterman & Spelke (2006) argue that— Search behavior following disorientation depends on two distinct processes: a modular reorientation process…and an associative process that directly links landmarks to locations (p. 581) Return of the Jedi Problems with Lee et al. Small moveable landmarks Defining quite small area Alternative way to test the two step hypothesis Use of colored wall in an octagon with alternating short and long sides to discriminate among 3 all white corners The Octagonal Space A D B C Photo of the Octagon Starting with An All-White Octagon Questions Can children use geometry in a more complex figure? Can children use geometry in a figure without an axis of principal symmetry? YES if total GC choices > 50% YES if total GC choices > 50% Methodologically, have children been successfully disoriented? YES if correct choice NOT > average of other GC choices Data from All-White Octagon Newcombe et al., Developmental Science, in press 100 100 50 0 2 3 yr yr Total GC Chance PC 50 0 2 3 yr yr At left, we see that both 2- and 3-year-old children were reliably greater than chance in choosing geometrically correct corners At right, we see a slight and non-reliable ability to choose the correct corner from among the 4 geometrically correct corners Mean GC Data from Octagon with Colored Wall Newcombe et al., Developmental Science, in press 100 50 0 3 5 yr yr Total GC Chance 100 PC 50 0 3 5 yr yr At left, we see that both 3- and 5year-old children were reliably greater than chance in choosing geometrically correct corners At right, we now see a reliable ability to choose the correct corner from among the 4 geometrically correct corners Mean GC What About the Targets in All-White Corners? Correct choices reliably greater than average of other GC corners 35% versus 14% at 3 years 38% versus 10% at 5 years These data show that young children do in fact use features to reorient Can Children Use Features ALONE? Newcombe et al., Developmental Science, in press Three Hiding Boxes Two Hiding Boxes X X X On these 2 conditions, children averaged 50%, reliably greater than chance (33%) In this condition, children averaged 64%, reliably greater than chance (50%) Specific Conclusions Strong evidence for coding of geometric information Little evidence for an encapsulated geometric module or the role of language in puncturing it Good evidence that different types of spatial information are routinely (though not invariably) combined Combination process depends on Cue salience Encoding variability and certainty Acquiring information on cue validity through action and experience General Conclusion We can analyze spatial navigation and orientation as an evolved mental skill without postulating Encapsulated modularity Highly specific innate endowment