New Trends in SLA Research: Theories, Methods, Ethics Lourdes Ortega University of Hawai‘i at Mānoa National Tsing Hua University Taiwan, June 8, 2011 Please cite as: Ortega, L. (2011). New trends in SLA research: Theories, methods, ethics. Invited lecture at National Tsing Hua University, Taiwan, June 8. Copyright © Lourdes Ortega, 2011 Background: Since the mid 1990s, intense disciplinary crisis & reflection in SLA Wagner & Firth (1997) and Lafford’s (2007) Special 10-year anniversary of Firth & Wagner (1997) in Modern Language Journal. Writing reflective overviews: Ortega (2007) and (2011) in VanPatten & Williams (Eds.) (2007) & in Atkinson (Ed.) (2011) Writing an SLA textbook and anthologizing the field: Ortega (2009) and (2010b) Guiding Question: What will it take in the future so we can improve our explanations about second language learning? Values: interdisciplinarity: Cognitive Science Social Theories Study of Bilingualism epistemological diversity Challenge 1: addressing the explicitimplicit knowledge interface Theoretical explosion in SLA: VanPatten & Williams (Eds.) (2007) Atkinson (Ed.) (2011) Main theories in SLA (VanPatten & Williams, Eds., 2007): Connectionist-emergentist (e.g., N. Ellis, Ch. 5) Formal-linguistic SLA (e.g., White, Ch. 3; Carroll, Ch. 9) Skills acquisition (e.g., DeKeyser, Ch. 6) Functionalist-linguistic SLA (e.g., Barvodi-Harlig, Ch. 4; Pienemann, Ch. 8) Cognitive-interactionist (e.g., Gass & Mackey, Ch. 10; VanPatten, Ch. 7; TBLT: Robinson, Skehan, etc...) Vygostkian SLA (e.g., Lantolf & Thorne, Ch. 11) Alternative theories in SLA (Atkinson, Ed., 2011): Complexity theory (e.g., Larsen-Freeman, Ch. 2) Language Socialization Theory (e.g., Duff & Talmy, Ch. 4) Identity Theory Sociocognitive Approach (e.g., Atkinson, Ch. 6) (e.g., Norton & McKinney, Ch. 3) CA-for-SLA (e.g., Kasper & Wagner, Ch. 5) Vygostkian SLA (e.g., Lantolf, Ch. 1) Current positions/foci on “L2 knowledge” Linguistic Generativist theory; Functionalist approaches Implicit only General cognitive Skills acquisition theory Cognitiveinteractionist theories Implicit mostly Vygotskian; Identity Explicit mostly Explicit>Implicit Emergentist, Complexity, and Dynamic Systems theories Social Explicit+Implicit Language Socialization Explicit+Implicit CA-for-SLA; Sociocognitive Implicit Importance is recognized in L2 instruction research: Most explicit Most implicit + Grammar explanation; + Exposure to input e.g., NTHU’s Hung-Tzu made salient by Huang: meta-analysis engineering: + Metalanguage; of input enhancement in SSLA (Lee &• Huang, Phonological or 2008) typographical salience + Instructions to attend to specific form; etc • Frequency • Order of presentation etc Relative recent interest at the psycholinguistic level: DeKeyser (2003) HSLA “explicit > implicit” R. Ellis (2004) LL “Definition & measurement…” N. Ellis (2005) SSLA “At the interface…” Sharwood Smith & Truscott (2005) AL the processing origins of L2 knowledge But still disengagement or underdetermination are the norm: Illustrative case in point: RECAST debate (Lyster, 2004; Long, 2006) Where do we look for benefits brought about by recasts? Evidence of “learning”? 1. immediate responses Incorporation (=successful uptake) S: Some people have racism. T: Some people ARE racist. S: are racist. Loewen & Philp (2006, p. 541) Structural priming A: Where where where you break it? B: Where did you break it? Mae Sot+ A: Mae Sot in Tak? B: Yeah+ A: Why why why did you go there? McDonough (2006, p. 186) Evidence of “learning”? 2.retrospection I said ‘freedom of thinking’. I was not certain if it should be ‘thinking’ or ‘thought’. I didn’t come up with ‘thought’ then, so I said ‘freedom of thinking’, then I felt it might be wrong. Then the teacher said ‘freedom of thought’. So I thought, ‘Oh, oh. I was wrong – just as I thought.’ Nabei & Swain (2002, p. 55) Evidence of “learning”? 3. pre-post test changes Mackey & Goo (2007): Meta-analysis of interaction & feedback, 10 studies of recasts yielded d=0.96 Benefits for learning: explicit or implicit knowledge? Negative evidence & cognitive comparison (L1: Farrar, Nelson....) Noticing Incorporation & (Schmidt) Introspection Testing hypothesis, pushed output (Lyster) explicit?? Positive evidence & enhanced input (L2: Leeman, McDonough, Doughty) Repeated processing Structural priming & Recasts held gains in memory Pre-post-test implicit?? Memory trace, frequency tallying Undeniable: implicit, bottom up, and subconscious processing explicit, top down, and conscious processing What to do? At this stage of SLA’s disciplinary knowledge… …it is problematic for any theory to discard a priori one or the other type of knowledge as irrelevant for explaining L2 acquisition; …and also problematic is to neglect to clarify whether claims about learning are made with regard to explicit or implicit cognition Challenge 1: Methodological solutions required Tall order: SLA researchers will need to draw from cognitive science in this area in order to make explorations neurobiologically plausible (e.g., the work by Georgetown alumni who trained in Michael Ullman’s lab: Kara Morgan-Short, Harriet Wood Bowden) Tall order: Ideally, SLA researchers will also combine training and methodologies to produce yoked behavioral and neurological evidence (e.g., NTHU faculty trained in the psycholinguistics of processing and in neurolinguistics: Chun-Chieh Gloria Yang) Natalie Hsu, Fan-Pei Challenge 2: theorizing experience Differential experience is connected to one of the most salient facts to be explained by any SLA theory: variability and heterogeneity in L2 learning processes and outcomes Yet, traditional SLA theories are illequipped to deal with variability and, as a consequence, they trivialize learner experience as anecdotal, divesting it from any theoretical status The importance of diverse social contexts for L2 learning resides less in externally documented experience or fixed environmental encounters and more in experience that is lived, made sense of, negotiated, contested, and claimed by learners in their physical, interpersonal, social, cultural, and historical context. What to do? 1. Look for theories that offer social respecifications of phenomena: L2 grammar: Systemic-Functional Linguistics L2 communication: Conversation Analysis L2 cognition: Vygotskian theory L2 learning: Language socialization L2 sense of self: Identity theory Good example: NTHU’s YuJung Chang’s study of the non-deficit oriented, agentive identities of 4 doctoral graduate students in the US who were so-called non-native speakers (Chang & Kanno, 2010) 2. Investigate diverse contexts & populations: Second, foreign, heritage language contexts Varying ages L1 semiliterate/L1 oral populations of L2 learners Disparate social milieus with varying L2 use needs Challenge 2: Theoretical solutions already underway But… better done 1 than 2 so far: SLA researchers have begun to look for theories that offer social respecifications of phenomena (“the social turn in SLA”) But SLA as a field continues to investigate very limited contexts & population Yet, crucial: We do not know what new theoretical models will need to be advanced, or how the present ones will need to be modified, once SLA researchers begin to investigate populations that are currently seriously understudied (e.g., Bigelow, 2010; Valdés, 2005;Verhoeven, 1994) Challenge 3: re-evaluating SLA theories through the prism of bilingualism Native speaker as golden benchmark and reference: To explain what we can expect in L2 development data and what we see To judge what ultimate attainment should look like and whether it is possible (in L2) To evaluate educational goals & outcomes, i.e., communicative competence = “like a NS” (linguistic knowledge, pragmatics, collocations, rhetoric, gestures…) Extensive critique against “nativespeakerism” from social and critical perspectives: Canagarajah (2004) Firth & Wagner (1997) García (2009) Hall et al. (2006) Holliday (1994); Holliday & Aboshiha (2009) Jenkins (2006) Leung (2005) Norton & Toohey (2001) Rampton (1990) Seidlhofer (2001) Phillipson & Skutnabb-Kangas (1986) Shohamy (2006) Sridhar (1994) ………..and so many more ... ... ... ...! But in SLA, to present day: L2 acquisition = developing native competence But… what do we mean by “native”? “native”: a language user… + by birth + born to one language only + no detectable traces of other languages “native” = “monolingual native” “native speaker” = One language By birth “non-native speaker” = Not by birth Multiple languages L2 competence in SLA: two monolingual speakers housed in a single head? monolingual-like competence the goal? monolingual and monocultural acts of a special (secondary) nature? language competencies static and fixed in the L1, and dynamic and in flux only in the L2? Monolingual Bias in SLA L2 acquisition = “efforts by monolingual adults to add on a monolingual-like command of an additional language” (Ortega, 2009, p. 5) + in an imagined world where what’s given/owned by birth can never be matched or altered by experience/history (Ortega, 2010a) Accepted tenets in the study of bilingualism: Bilinguals cannot be reduced to the sum of two monolinguals in one. (Grosjean, 1989) Accepted tenets in the study of bilingualism: Context-free, fixed, and dichotomous NS/NNS categories have questionable validity. (Li Wei, 2000) Accepted tenets in the study of bilingualism: The development of multiple language competencies is a process mediated by amount of use/degree of activation across languages. (e.g., Sebastián-Gallés et al., 2005) Accepted tenets in the study of bilingualism: The development of additional language competence interacts with, destabilizes, and most likely transforms the nature of linguistic competence across the languages of the individual (languages interact). (e.g., Cenoz et al., 2001) Good example: NTHU’s I-Ru Su’s study of bi-directional transfer in Taiwanese learners of EFL doing requests and being conventionally indirect (Su, 2010) What to do? 1. Pursue new constructs: “multi-competence is not just the imperfect cloning of mono-competence, but a different state” (Vivian Cook, 2002, pp. 7-8) people who speak more than one language posses varying expertise, inheritance, and affilitation across their languages (Rampton, 1990) 2. Pursue new empirical baselines: Compare incipient and emerging bilinguals to fully developed bilinguals; bi/multilinguals cannot be directly compared to monolinguals; the sole benchmark for comparison cannot be monolinguals. (Birdsong, 2005; Harley & Wang, 1997; Singleton, 2003) 3. Pursue new designs: Investigate a learner’s multiple languages simultaneously within the same study. (Ortega & Carson, 2010) 4. Craft new discourse of bilingualism as potentiality, not deficit: …proponents of this view offer an explanation for adults’ relative failure to reach nativelikeness that is based on neurological changes that occur at a certain age (e.g., puberty) and that lead to a sudden or gradual deterioration or distortion of the implicit language learning mechanism… …proponents of this view offer an explanation for adults’ relative failure to reach nativelikeness that is based on neurological changes that occur at a certain age (e.g., puberty) and that lead to a sudden or gradual deterioration or distortion of the implicit language learning mechanism… … a number of studies show that L2 learners’ employment of formulaic sequences is often problematic. Although learners can produce a considerable number of native-like sequences…, there is evidence that learners’ restricted formulaic repertoires lead them to overuse those sequences they know well … Still, overall, nonnative use of formulaic sequences is less pervasive and less diverse than native norms … It is not surprising, therefore, that L2 learners’ failure to use native-like formulaic sequences is one factor in making their writing feel nonnative… … a number of studies show that L2 learners’ employment of formulaic sequences is often problematic. Although learners can produce a considerable number of native-like sequences…, there is evidence that learners’ restricted formulaic repertoires lead them to overuse those sequences they know well … Still, overall, nonnative use of formulaic sequences is less pervasive and less diverse than native norms … It is not surprising, therefore, that L2 learners’ failure to use native-like formulaic sequences is one factor in making their writing feel nonnative… discourses of deficit are persuasive!! “many bilinguals … have a tendency to evaluate their language competencies as inadequate. Some criticize their mastery of language skills, others strive their hardest to reach monolingual norms, others still hide their knowledge of their “weaker” language, and most simply do not perceive themselves as being bilingual even though they use two (or more) languages regularly” Grosjean (2008, p. 224) Challenge 3: Theoreticalmethodological-ethical solutions badly needed!!! Badly needed: a bilingual turn in SLA!! Reorientating towards studying what L1+L2 (multicompetent/bilingual) users can do, as opposed to only understanding what they cannot or wish not to do in their L2 SLA, a field in pursuit of knowledge about: “learning to be bilingual” supportive of: “pathways to multicompetence” In conclusion… New Trends in SLA Research… 2.… properly investigating learners’ experience 3.…conceptualizing SLA as bilingualism, fighting the monolingual bias 1.… clarifying explicit-implicit knowledge Methods Theories Theories + Methods + Ethics Looking forward, looking ahead To more empirical work on the nature of explicit and implicit language knowledge and their respective contributions to L2 learning To more theorizing into ways to study how the lived experiences afforded by different social contexts shape L2 learning; more empirical work across diverse experiences (=diverse social contexts) To a bilingual turn in SLA!! Thank You lortega@hawaii.edu References: Atkinson, D. (Ed.). (2011). Alternative approaches in second language acquisition. New York: Routledge. Bigelow, M. H. (2010). Mogadishu on the Mississippi: Language, racialized identity, and education in a new land. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. Birdsong, D. P. (2005). Nativelikeness and non-nativelikeness in L2A research. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 43, 319-328. Canagarajah, S. (2004). Subversive identities, pedagogical safe houses, and critical learning. In B. Norton & K. Toohey (Eds.), Critical pedagogies and language learning (pp. 116-137). New York: Cambridge University Press. Cenoz, J., Hufeisen, B., & Jessner, U. (2001). Cross-linguistic influence in third language acquisition: Psycholinguistic perspectives. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. Chang, Y.-J., & Kanno, Y. (2010). NNES doctoral students in English-speaking academe: The nexus between language and discipline. Applied Linguistics, 31, 671–692. Cook, V. (2002). Background of the L2 user. In V. Cook (Ed.), Portraits of the L2 user (pp. 1-28). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. DeKeyser, R. (2003). Implicit and explicit learning. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 313-348). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Ellis, N. C. (2005). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305-352. Ellis, R. (2004). The definition and measurement of L2 explicit knowledge. Language Learning, 54, 227–275. Firth, A., & Wagner, J. (1997). On discourse, communication, and (some) fundamental concepts in SLA research. The Modern Language Journal, 81, 285-300. García, O. (2009). Bilingual education in the 21st century: A global perspective. (with contributions by Hugo Baetens Beardsmore). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. Grosjean, F. (1989). Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language, 36, 3-15. Grosjean, F. (2008). Studying Bilinguals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Hall, J. K., Cheng, A., & Carlson, M. T. (2006). Reconceptualizing multicompetence as a theory of language knowledge. Applied Linguistics, 27, 220–240. Harley, B., & Wang, W. (1997). The critical period hypothesis: Where are we now? In A. M. B. d. Groot & J. F. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 19-51). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Holliday, A. (1994). Appropriate methodology and social context. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Holliday, A., & Aboshiha, P. (2009). The denial of ideology in perceptions of 'nonnative speaker' teachers. TESOL Quarterly, 43, 669-689. Jenkins, J. (2006). Points of view and blind spots: ELF and SLA. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 16, 137-162. Lafford, B. (Ed.). (2007). Second language reconceptualized? The impact of Firth and Wagner (1997). Modern Language Journal, 91, (Issue Supplement s1), 733-942. Lee, S.-K., & Huang, H. T. (2008). Visual input enhancement and grammar learning: A meta-analytic review. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30, 307-331. Leung, C. (2005). Convivial communication: Recontextualizing communicative competence. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 15(2), 119-143. Loewen, S., & Philp, J. (2006). Recasts in the adult L2 classroom: characteristics, explicitness and effectiveness. Modern Language Journal, 90, 536-556. Long, M. H. (2006). Problems in SLA. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Lyster, R. (2004). Differential effects of prompts and recasts in form-focused instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 399-432 . Mackey, A., & Goo, J. M. (2007). Interaction research in SLA: A meta-analysis and research synthesis. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 407452). New York: Oxford University Press. McDonough, K. (2006). Interaction and syntactic priming: English L2 speakers’ production of dative constructions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 179-207. Nabei, T., & Swain, M. (2002). Learner awareness of recasts in classroom interaction: A case study of an adult EFL student's second language learning. Language Awareness, 11, 43-63. Norton, B., & Toohey, K. (2001). Changing perspectives on good language learners. TESOL Quarterly, 35, 307-322. Ortega, L. (2007). Second language learning explained? SLA across nine contemporary theories. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 221-246). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Ortega, L. (2009). Understanding second language acquisition. London: Hodder Arnold. Ortega, L. (2010a). The bilingual turn in SLA. Plenary speech delivered at the American Association for Applied Linguistics Conference, Atlanta, GA, March 6-9. Ortega, L. (Ed.). (2010b). Critical Concepts in Linguistics: Second Language Acquisition. New York: Routledge. Ortega, L. (2011). SLA after the social turn: Where cognitivism and its alternatives stand. In D. Atkinson (Ed.), Alternative approaches in second language acquisition (pp. 167-180). New York: Routledge. Ortega, L., & Carson, J. G. (2010). Multicompetence, social context, and L2 writing research praxis. In T. Silva & P. K. Matsuda (Eds.), Practicing theory in second language writing (pp. 48-71). West Lafayette, IN: Parlor Press. Phillipson, R., & Skutnabb-Kangas, T. (Eds.). (1986). Linguicism rules in education. Roskilde, Denmark Roskilde University Centre Institute VI. Rampton, M. B. H. (1990). Displacing the "native speaker": Expertise, affiliation, and inheritance. English Language Teaching Journal, 44, 97-101. Sebastián-Gallés, N., Echeverría, S., & Bosch, L. (2005). The influence of initial exposure on lexical representation: Comparing early and simultaneous bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language 52, 240255. Seidlhofer, B. (2001). Closing a conceptual gap: The case for a description of English as a lingua franca. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 1, 133-158. Singleton, D. (2003). Critical period or general age factor(s)? In M. P. García Mayo & M. L. García Lecumberri (Eds.), Age and the acquisition of English as a foreign language (pp. 3-22). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. Sharwood Smith, M., & Truscott, J. (2005). Stages or continua in second language acquisition: A Mogul solution. Applied Linguistics, 22, 219-240. Shohamy, E. (2006). Rethinking assessment for advanced language proficiency. In H. Byrnes, H. D. WegerGuntharp & K. Sprang (Eds.), Educating for advanced foreign language capacities: Constructs, curriculum, instruction, assessment (pp. 188-208). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Sridhar, S. N. (1994). A reality check for SLA theories. TESOL Quarterly, 28, 800-805. Su, I.-R. (2010). Transfer of pragmatic competences: A bi-directional perspective. Modern Language Journal 94, 87–102. Valdés, G. (2005). Bilingualism, heritage language learners, and SLA research: Opportunities lost or seized? Modern Language Journal, 89, 410-426. VanPatten, B., & Williams, J. (Eds.). (2007). Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Verhoeven, L. T. (1994). Transfer in bilingual development: The linguistic interdependence hypothesis revisited. Language Learning, 44, 381-415.