Optical Illusions The word “illusion” comes from the Latin word illudere, which means “to mock.” What is an optical illusion? Optical illusions are images or pictures that we perceive differently than they really are. Put another way, optical illusions occur when our eyes send information to our brains that tricks us into perceiving something that does not match reality. Scientists believe optical illusions are possible because our brains are so good at recognizing patterns and seeing familiar objects. Our brains work quickly to make a whole image from separate pieces. What do optical illusions tell us about visual perception? To perceive is to create a figure or shape that does not necessarily appear as such in the real world but that we can represent mentally so that we can recognize it under various conditions (for instance, when it is partly hidden). By studying the way that the brain fills in missing or ambiguous visual information, we can learn a lot about the way that we perceive the world. Optical illusions provide fertile ground for such study, because they involve ambiguous images that force the brain to make decisions that tell us about how we perceive things. Optical illusions give us a better understanding of how human visual perception works. They force us to recognize that opposite to what we might think, what we see of the world is not a simple physical record like a picture. On the contrary, the signals that your eyes receive from the elements in a scene in front of them are often ambiguous. Your brain is constantly interpreting these signals to construct an image that makes sense to it. In fact, your brain tries so hard to make sense of everything that it often finds meaning even where there is none, thus creating optical illusions. 3 different types of optical illusions: Cognitive, Physiological, and Literal illusions. Literal Illusions: What is a literal illusion? A literal illusion creates images that are different from the objects that make them. For example: Some optical illusions are physiological. Which means that they’re caused by some sort of physical means in the eyes or the brain. Distorting illusions use different techniques to make objects of similar size, length or curvature appear distorted. A famous example of a distorting illusion is the Müller Lyer illusion. Doesn’t the line in the middle look longer than the ones above and below it? But really, all three lines are the same length. Paradox illusions occur as a result of pictures or objects that cannot exist or are physically impossible. Paradox illusions are popular in works of art. Cognitive Illusions The way you look at an object can affect how you see it. Sometimes there are two images in the same picture, but you can only see one at a time so your brain chooses one (when it deals with too much information). Cognitive illusions rely on knowledge about the world and are also under conscious control. Ambiguous illusions are pictures or objects that offer significant changes in appearance. Perception will 'switch' between the alternates as they are considered in turn as available data does not confirm a single view. The Necker cube is a well-known example, the motion parallax due to movement is being misinterpreted, even in the face of other sensory data. Another popular is the Rubin vase. Paradox Illusion: Paradox Illusions are illusions that are three dimensional and impossible in real life. But seem oddly convincing in two dimensional drawings. These illusions are often dependent on a cognitive misunderstanding that adjacent ends must join. The Penrose Stairs is a great example of a Paradox illusion, also known as the “impossible staircase”. After-image Illusion: An afterimage is a non-specific term that refers to an image continuing to appear in one's vision after staring at the original image was shown for a certain period of time. An afterimage may be a normal phenomenon (physiological afterimage) or may be pathological (palinopsia). Illusory palinopsia may be a pathological exaggeration of physiological afterimages. Ewald Hering explained how the brain sees afterimages, in terms of three pairs of primary colors. This opponent process theory states that the human visual system interprets color information by processing signals from cones and rods in an antagonistic manner. The opponent color theory suggests that there are three opponent channels: red versus green, blue versus yellow, and black versus white. Responses to one color of an opponent channel are antagonistic to those to the other color. Therefore, a green image will produce a magenta afterimage. The green color fatigues the green photoreceptors, so they produce a weaker signal. Anything resulting in less green is interpreted as its paired primary color, which is magenta.