Theoretical and Methodological Issues Professor Bogat Problems Estimating the Amount of Child Maltreatment I. Actual cases of child maltreatment (????) II. Cases of child maltreatment that come to the attention of mandated reporters III. Cases of child maltreatment reported to CPS (Child Protective Services) VI. State-mandated services VII. Arrests IV. Cases that are screened-in by CPS intake workers, leading to CPS report V. Substantiated cases of child maltreatment From textbook, page 32 VIII. Convictions Problems with Estimation The graph on the prior slide indicates some of the problems with estimating the incidence of child maltreatment. We start with a lack of knowledge about exactly how many cases of child maltreatment occur in the U.S. As the funnel narrows, each step along the way is another challenge to estimating the incidence. By the time we get to actual convictions of perpetrators, we are only considering a small # of the actual cases that occurred. Official Estimates Two primary sources of official statistics on child maltreatment 1. National Child Abuse and Neglect Data System 2. National Incidence Study National Child Abuse and Neglect Data System (NCANDS) Federally-sponsored program collects yearly data Collates reports from State Child Protective Services(CPSs) Best data we have, but . . . Only includes child maltreatment reported to State CPSs (Only a certain % of cases that occur are ever reported) Definitions of child maltreatment may vary by State (see next slide) Federal Definitions and State Definitions regarding NCANDS “Each State has its own definitions of child abuse and neglect that are based on standards set by Federal law. Federal legislation provides a foundation for States by identifying a set of acts or behaviors that define child abuse and neglect. The Child Abuse Prevention and Treatment Act (CAPTA), (42 U.S.C. §5101), as amended by the CAPTA Reauthorization Act of 2010, retained the existing definition of child abuse and neglect as, at a minimum: Any recent act or failure to act on the part of a parent or caretaker which results in death, serious physical or emotional harm, sexual abuse or exploitation; or an act or failure to act, which presents an imminent risk of serious harm. Most States recognize four major types of maltreatment: neglect, physical abuse, psychological maltreatment, and sexual abuse. Although any of the forms of child maltreatment may be found separately, they also can occur in combination.” Quotations taken from U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Administration on Children, Youth and Families, Children’s Bureau. (2011). Child Maltreatment 2010. Available from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/cb/stats_research/index.htm#can. NCANDS I encourage you to visit the government’s website http://cwoutcomes.acf.hhs.gov/data/overview and look at the child maltreatment data for individual states and overall. The data is broken down by age of victim, race/ethnicity of victim, type of maltreatment, and time to investigation. National Incidence Study (NIS) Began before NCANDS Purpose was to determine true national incidence of child maltreatment, not incidence based on CPS reports Latest NIS (2010) surveyed 11,000 professionals who work with children and may have knowledge of child maltreatment Positives: employ uniform definition of different types of child maltreatment that includes both an endangerment standard and a harm standard (see next slide) NIS: Harm vs. Endangerment Under the Harm Standard, children can be considered mistreated only if they have experienced some type of harm from abuse or neglect by parents. These are typically the cases that come to the attention of CPS. Under the Endangerment Standard, children who have not yet experienced abuse or neglect, but are at risk of such, are included. The Endangerment Standard also includes children who are in the Harm Standard. It also includes events that are done by perpetrators other than parents. Self-Report Surveys Another way to get an estimate of the incidence of maltreatment is by administering a self-report survey to a representative sample of the population. Advantage: ability to ascertain events that might not reach the attention of professionals or CPS Disadvantage: The self-report will necessarily be retrospective. Usually questions are phrased to ask whether certain actions occurred in the past year. Disadvantage: Memory can be faulty Disadvantage: Respondents might not tell the truth Three Widely-Used Self Report Surveys Conflict Tactics Scale (see textbook for discussion and copies of items) National Crime Victimization Survey (NCVS) Semi-annual survey of 60,000 households Includes questions about all types of crime, including child maltreatment Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire (JVQ) Measures all types of childhood victimization (e.g., bullying) as well as child maltreatment The questionnaire is given via interview. When screening questions about a particular type of victimization are answered “yes” then follow-up questions are asked (e.g., who was the perpetrator). Explaining Child Maltreatment Cultural Context Most Americans believe that it is ok to hit your child Violence exists on a continuum Our society condones much violence (tv, movies, cartoons). Condoned violence may lead to uncondoned violence. Parents who abuse their children often say that the violence escalated—they only planned to discipline the child (maybe hit them on the hand or behind), not abuse them. Explaining Child Maltreatment (con’t) Structural characteristics of the family make children vulnerable Family interactions can be stressful We spend a lot of time with our families—young children can’t just leave the household if aggression escalates Children are subordinate to parents and taught to obey their parents, not fight back Even when parents are reported for abuse, every effort is made to keep the children with the family. Parents are usually given a few chances to change. Explaining Child Maltreatment (con’t) Low Costs of Child Maltreatment There is little cost for abusing children Many people believe that childhood victimizations are somehow less serious than other aggression or are an expected part of childhood The low costs are slowly changing There are many mandatory reporting laws More pressure to remove children from abusive homes Your textbook authors argue, however, that the costs are still low Etiology of Child Maltreatment 4 broad etiological theories/models Social Ecology/Social Bonding Theories Social class, strain and frustration Social learning theories Attachment and parent-child interaction theories Social Ecology/Social Bonding Theories Social Ecology Theory--child maltreatment occurs as an interaction between the child and his/her environment When there is a breakdown in community and shared responsibility then adults in entire neighborhoods can be more likely to abuse children There are many factors that lead to child maltreatment—not just one. Individual factors don’t predict child maltreatment, but the accumulation of many risk factors does Social Bonding theory asks why adults don’t abuse children when they live in an environment that leads many to do so Hirschi (1969) argues that when there are strong social bonds, people are less likely to act in deviant ways Social Class, Strain, and Frustration Theories Poverty is a direct contributor to child maltreatment Rates of child maltreatment are higher among the poor and unemployed The stressors associated with poverty as well as the unequal distribution of opportunities creates strain and frustration for families. Adults then are more likely to abuse their children. Social Learning Theory Children who are exposed to aggression model these behaviors with their own children (the intergenerational transmission of violence) Children model these behaviors because they learn that violence is an acceptable way to resolve problems and there is reinforcement for violence (child is whining which annoys father, father hits child, child stops whining) Attachment and Parent-Child Interaction Theories These theories propose that an interaction between characteristics of the child and characteristics of the parent lead to a situation in which abuse is more likely. Research shows that children who are difficult or coercive are more likely to elicit corporal punishment from their parents (e.g., Reid, Patterson, & Snyder, 2002) Your textbook cites evidence for this interaction while cautioning that we should not blame the victim—children are never responsible for their victimization However, there is an interesting article, not cited in the text, that sheds more light on this issue (see next slide) Parent-Child Interaction Theories (con’t) Corporal punishment is not the same as child maltreatment. Does children’s difficult and coercive behavior also elicit child maltreatment? Twin studies can help us understand how much influence a child has on his/her maltreatment. One way is to examine the similarity in discipline/maltreatment received by monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic twins (DZ). Recall that MZ twins share all their genes and DZ twins share about ½ of their genes. Therefore, If MZ twins discipline/maltreatment is more similar than DZ twins, it suggests that there must be some genetic characteristic of the child that is leading to that discipline/maltreatment on the part of the parent. Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, Polo-Tomas, Price, & Taylor (2004) examined this question using twin research. They found very interesting results. “There is likely to be a contingent, genetically mediated relation between children’s difficult coercive behavior and their experience of corporal punishment, whereas there is less likely to be such a contingent relation between children’s behavior and their experience of maltreatment.” In other words, parents’ abusive behavior is not provoked by the child; parents’ corporal punishment discipline does seem to be related to the child’s behavior. These authors suggest that “demographic risk factors (e.g., low education, single parenthood, neighborhood poverty), familial risk factors (e.g., marital conflict, parent psychopathology, parents’ negative emotionality), and parenting risk factors (e.g., low parental involvement) predict maltreatment (Azar, 2002; Belsky, 1993; Jaffee, in press).” Methodological Issues: How can we conduct better research on child maltreatment? Universal definitions of different types of child maltreatment do not exist—this may be a difficult problem to solve as one person can look at a behavior differently than another person Difficult to establish cause-and-effect relationships. Most child maltreatment research is retrospective (people are asked to think about their behavior in the past) and correlational (we take variables and associate them with each other) Retrospective, correlational research cannot establish causation. Two variables can be correlated with each other, but one might not cause the other. One way to establish cause-effect relationships is to use an experimental design—but how would we do this with child maltreatment? Longitudinal vs. cross-sectional studies Longitudinal studies allow us to study the behaviors of the same group of people over time to see how their behavior changes and what effects their behavior. Cross-sectional studies are more common—they are just a snapshot of what is happening with a group of people at a particular moment Twin Studies See previous slide. You can see how a twin design can tease apart a number of different issues that other designs cannot