False Feedback Influencing Self-Monitoring False Feedback on a Personality Test: Influencing Perceptions of Self-Monitoring Lead Author Courtney L. Zinter Co-Researchers Crystal Kroells & Abbey Weiler Winona State University Abstract The effect of giving participants a fake description of their level of self-monitoring based on false personality tests on participants self-perception was examined. The hypothesis stated that participants would view themselves as higher or lower in self-monitoring after receiving feedback about the way they should be based on their scores on false personality tests. This study was based on the theory that people’s self-perceptions can be altered by the ways other people view them and on the Barnum effect. The results did not support the hypothesis. Participant’s scores on their self-monitoring pre-test and post-test did not differ significantly in either condition. False Feedback on a Personality Test: Influencing Perceptions of Self-Monitoring This study was based on the theory that people’s self-perceptions are affected by other people’s views of them. The theory is based on different processes of identity development such as the looking glass, reflected appraisal process and the process of role taking. The hypothesis was that participants would view themselves as higher or lower in self-monitoring after receiving feedback about the way they should be based on their scores on false personality tests. Based on a study done by Snyder, Shenkel and Lowery, the Barnum effect is a phenomenon in which people accept interpretations of their personality when they are found through assessments, especially when the interpretations are rather vague (1977). According to the authors, the majority of research has consisted of people participating in a personality test, waiting for their score, receiving an interpretation based on that score and analyzing the accuracy of the description about themselves. The study looked at all of the research done in this area in the 25 years prior to the study, and discussed the factors of gender, personality attributes and intelligence An archival research study, meta-analysis, done by Swann discusses the process of identity negotiation in which the perceptions others have of a person and that person’s self-perception interact (1987). He found that while people want others to see them the way see themselves, self-constructs are malleable and others play an active role in helping people develop their self-construct. According to a study done by Cast, Stets and Burke, people who are seen as credible or knowledgeable have more influence on others self-perceptions (1999). They used married couples in their study to see if the higher status spouse had more influence on how the lower status spouse perceived themselves. They found that in married couples, the higher status spouse’s views on their significant other had an effect on their significant 1 False Feedback Influencing Self-Monitoring other’s self-perception. It was less likely that the spouse’s self-perceptions would be changed by their spouse’s views if the couple was equal in status. Method Participants Forty-four participants in introduction level college psychology courses signed up to participate in this study. Participants were given extra credit in their psychology courses for their participation. Materials Three personality tests were used for this study. The first was a 10 question personality test that measured self-monitoring. The self-monitoring test was taken from a true or false scale and revised into a six-point scale. The second test was a series of visual tasks; one was a 3-D dancer, one was an image that can be seen as either an old woman or a young woman, one was an image of coffee beans which hid a man’s face within it and the last was a picture with several eagles hidden in it. The third test was a combination of 41 questions taken from different personality tests combined with the 10 questions from the self-monitoring test. The visual personality test was administered on the researcher’s laptops. There was also a 8.5 inch by 2 inch slip of paper in which participants were asked to rate how much a self-monitoring description fit them. There were three different self-monitoring descriptions; one was a description for low self-monitoring, another was a description for high self-monitoring and the last was called “active” self-monitoring which was used so participants would think there were more than just two categories. A personality workbook was used for the researchers to falsely score the visual task as well as a stopwatch to time portions of the visual test. Design and Procedure Participants, who signed up for the study, were randomly assigned to receive either the high self-monitoring description or the low self-monitoring description upon arrival. Participants were told they were going to take three personality tests. They were first asked to fill out the self-monitoring questionnaire. Next they went through a PowerPoint of visual “personality” tasks which the researchers pretended to score. Participants then received a description of their level of self-monitoring. They read their description and the researcher read a copy aloud. On a scale of one to six, participants rated how much the description fit their personality. Next the participants took the 51 question survey of personality. Participants were told that some questions may be similar, but were asked to answer them anyway and told to do all the tests based on initial reactions. Following the third test, participants were debriefed and thanked for their time. Results An independent samples t-test showed that the self-monitoring condition had no significant effect on participant’s scores on a self-monitoring test, t ( 42 ) = -0.895, p = .376. The scores did, however, go in the correct direction. Participants in the high self-monitoring condition had average pre-test scores ( M = 34.13, SD = 5.01) that were lower than their post-test scores ( M = 34.5, SD = 6.41). Participants in the low selfmonitoring condition had average pre-test scores ( M = 33.27, SD = 5.87) that were higher than their post-test scores ( M = 32.77, SD = 5.42). The scores for the high condition increased by an average of .45 points and the scores for the low condition decreased by an average of .5 points. There was a significant effect of the descriptions on how much people agreed with their description, t ( 43 ) = 27.46, p = .000. Figure 1. The average test scores on a self-monitoring pre-test and post-test by participants given high or low self-monitoring descriptions based on a false score on a personality test. 2 False Feedback Influencing Self-Monitoring Self-Monitoring Scores Pre test 3 Post Test 35 34.5 34.5 34.14 34 33.5 33.27 32.77 33 32.5 32 31.5 High Condition Low Discussion The hypothesis stated that participants would view themselves as higher or lower in self-monitoring after receiving feedback about how should be based on their scores on false personality tests. It was predicted that participants would score higher on their self-monitoring post-test if they received the high self-monitoring description and they would score lower on their post-test if they received the low self-monitoring description. The study did not support the hypothesis, but the average scores for each condition went in the direction predicted. Scores for the high self-monitor condition increased during the post-test and scores for the low selfmonitor condition decreased. Also, there was significance for the Barnum effect, meaning participants agreed with the description they received. All but one of the participants agreed with their description. After the study was complete, some potential problems were detected. The differences in pre-test and posttest scores may have been significant had there been more participants in the study. Another problem was that the questions on the self-monitoring survey weren’t as neutral as the other questions on the third personality test. If the questions were re-worded, participants may not have recalled them from the first test to the third test. In the same sense, b y increasing the time between the first and third personality tests, participants may not have recalled them as easily either. Another option would have been to make both tests longer; putting irrelevant questions on the first test would have made it less obvious. References Briggs, S. R., Cheek, J. M., Buss, A. H. (1980). An analysis of the self-monitoring scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 38, 679-686. Cast, A. D., Stets, J. E. & Burke, P. J. (1999). Does the self conform to the views of others? Social Psychology Quarterly. 62, 68-82. Snyder, C. R. (1977). Acceptance of personality interpretations: The "Barnum Effect" and beyond. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 45, 104-112. False Feedback Influencing Self-Monitoring Swann, W. B. (1987). Identity negotiation: Where two roads meet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 53, 1038-1051. 4