Chapter 14 Aggression, Altruism, and Moral Development THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION • Aggression – Any form of behavior intended to injure or harm a living being who is motivated to avoid such treatment • Hostile aggression – goal is to harm a victim • Instrumental aggression – harming another is a means to some other end THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION • Origins of Aggression in Infancy – Instrumental aggression present by end of 1st year • Conflicts over possessions • Declines in second year as sharing becomes more common THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION • Developmental Trends in Aggression – 2 to 3 years, physical retaliation begins – 3 to 5, physical aggression declines, but is replaced by verbal aggression – For most children, physical aggression is normal, but relatively rare by middle childhood • Decline is in instrumental aggression • Hostile aggression increases slightly • Figure 14.1 Draft 2 Trajectories of mother-rated aggression for children from age 2 to age 9 years. ADAPTED FROM NICHD EARLY CHILD CARE RESEARCH NETWORK, 2004. THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION – Sex Differences • By 2 ½ - 3, males more physically and verbally aggressive than females –Due to rougher play with parents –More negative parental reaction to aggressive behaviors of daughters –Gender-typing of toys • Females are more relationally aggressive (covert aggression) THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION – From Aggression to Antisocial Conduct • Overt aggression declines from middle childhood through adolescence • Relational aggression in girls, and indirect aggression in males increases THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION – Is Aggression a Stable Attribute? • Yes for many individuals –Aggressive toddlers likely to be aggressive 5 year olds –Aggression between 3 and 10 predicts aggression and antisocial behavior later in life • Figure 14.2. Aggression in childhood predicts criminal behavior in adulthood for both males and females. FROM HUESMANN, ERON, LEFKOWITZ, & WALDER, 1984. THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION • Individual Differences in Aggressive Behavior – Few individuals are highly aggressive – 10-15% of classmates are abused by bullies – Proactive aggressors – aggression will produce tangible benefits; enhances selfesteem – Reactive aggressors – hostile, retaliatory aggression; wary of others THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION – Dodge’s Social Information-Processing Theory of Aggression • Encoding social cues • Interpretation of social cues • Formulates a goal to resolve situation • Generates possible strategies • Evaluates strategies for achieving goal • Selects response • Enacts response • Figure 14.3 Dodge’s social information-processing model of the steps children take when deciding how to respond to harmdoing or other social problems. The boy whose creation is destroyed by another boy’s nudging the table must first encode and interpret the social cues (i.e., did he mean it or was it accidental?) and then proceed through the remaining steps to formulate a response to this harmdoing. ADAPTED FROM CRICK & DODGE, 1994; LEMERISE & ARSENIO, 2000. THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION • Reactive aggressors are likely to develop a hostile attribution bias –Attribute ambiguous situations as serious and react aggressively • Proactive aggressors – plan an aggressive response to achieve an instrumental goal –Expect positive outcomes –Feel capable of dominating others • Figure 14.4 A social-cognitive model of the reactive aggressor’s biased attributions about ambiguous harmdoing and their behavioral outcomes. FROM ROBERT S. SIEGLER, 1996. THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION – Perpetrators and Victims of Peer Aggression • 17% of students were bullied • 19% reported bullying others • Boys more likely to be physically bullied • Girls more likely to be verbally or psychologically bullied • Bullying most common in 6th – 8th grades • Bullies more likely to use drugs THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION • Habitual bullies – observed adult conflict and aggression at home –Proactive aggressors – it works • Chronic victims – generally disliked –Passive victims – withdrawn, weak, but do not invite aggression »Overprotective mothers –Provocative victims – irritate peers, fight back unsuccessfully THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION – Popularity and Aggression • Popularity –Being well-known and accepted by other children –Having high status attributes –Highly desirable possessions –Being liked is not part of the definition • Popular children use aggression to maintain status THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION • Cultural and Subcultural Influences on Aggression – Differences between cultures are evident Lower SES – more aggression • Due to child-rearing differences –Physical punishment is higher in low SES families – modeling aggression –Difficulty monitoring children THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION • Coercive Home Environments: Breeding Grounds for Aggression – Families as Social Systems • Families of aggressive children focused on interactions that stopped irritation • Negative reinforcement • Ignore prosocial behavior • Use coercive tactics to deal with misconduct THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION • Methods of Controlling Aggression in Young Children – Creating Nonaggressive Environments • Remove aggressive toys • Provide enough space for play • Provide enough toys to reduce competition for scarce resources THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION – Eliminating the Payoffs for Aggression • Teach that aggression does not result in desired outcome • Incompatible-response technique –Ignore aggressive behaviors – eliminates reward of attention –Time out technique –Reinforce prosocial actions THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGGRESSION – Social-Cognitive Interventions • Use with older children and adolescents –Can teach individuals to »Regulate anger »Increase empathizing with others, taking their perspectives; reduces hostile attributions »Generate nonaggressive solutions to conflict ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF • Origins of Altruism – concern for the welfare of others and willingness to act on that concern – 12 to 18 month olds offer toys to peers – Toddlers can express sympathy • Verbally rebuking children and physically punishing them reduces compassion • Discipline based on affective explanation increases compassion ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF • Developmental Trends in Altruism – 2-3 year olds show sympathy/compassion • Rarely engage in spontaneous acts of self-sacrifice; but did during pretend play – 4-6 year olds – more real helping acts, fewer during pretend play ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF • Sex Differences in Altruism – Girls are more likely to be helpful, generous, and compassionate than boys (small difference) – Boys more interested in looking good or attaining status over others ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF • Social-Cognitive and Affective Contributions to Altruism – Children with well-developed role-taking skills are more helpful – Prosocial moral reasoning • Preschoolers’ tend to be self-serving • Older adolescents are much more responsive to the needs of others ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF – Empathy: An Important Affective Contributor to Altruism • Empathy – person’s ability to experience the emotions of other people. –Personal/self-oriented distress can lead to ignoring others in need –Sympathetic empathetic arousal – concern for distressed others increases altruism ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF • Socialization of Empathy –Model empathetic concern –Rely on affectively-oriented forms of discipline –Use of positive facial expressions when modeling sympathy ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF • Age Trends in the Empathy-Altruism Relationship –Little relationship between empathy and altruism among preschool and young grade school children –Stronger for older individuals »Need to understand why others are distressed »Need to suppress own distress ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF • The Felt-Responsibility Hypothesis –Sympathetic empathetic arousal causes one to reflect on altruistic lessons –Result is assuming personal responsibility for aiding a person in distress ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF • Cultural and Social Influences on Altruism – Cultural Influences • Altruism more common in less industrialized societies – where children help more with the family • Assigning chores improves altruism in Western societies • Table 14.1 Prosocial Behavior in Six Cultures: Percentages of Children in Each Culture Who Scored above the Median Altruism Score for the Cross-Cultural Sample as a Whole. SOURCE: Based on Whiting & Whiting, 1975. ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF • Collectivist societies –Encourage more cooperation and altruism –Less likely to take credit for altruistic acts; actually more likely to deny their role in the positive action • Figure 14.5 Rating by Canadian and Chinese children of actors who told the truth or lied about committing good deeds. ADAPTED FROM LEE ET AL., 1997. ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF – Social Influences • Reinforcing Altruism –Verbal reinforcement increases altruism –Tangible rewards decreases altruism • Practicing and Preaching Altruism –Altruistic modeling is also important ALTRUISM: DEVELOPMENT OF THE PROSOCIAL SELF • Who Raises Altruistic Children? – Altruistic parents – Parents who discipline children in ways that encourage children to accept personal responsibility for the harm they caused • Urge a helpful response to the victim MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Morality – Distinguish right from wrong – Act on that distinction – Experience pride in virtuous conduct; shame over acts that violate standards – Internalization of standards is vital • Table 14.2 Six Dimensions of Character That Define Moral Maturity for Canadian Adults. SOURCE: Walker & Pitts, 1998. MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • How Developmentalists Look at Morality – Affective component – stressed by psychoanalytic theorists – moral affects – Cognitive component – stressed by cognitive-developmental theorists – moral reasoning – Behavioral component – stressed by social learning and social information-processing theorists – moral behavior MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • The Affective Component of Moral Development – Freud’s Theory of Oedipal Morality • Superego develops during phallic stage • Identifies with same-sex parent • Internalizes same-sex moral standards –Girls have weaker superegos than boys MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR – Evaluation of Freud’s Theory • Pride, shame, guilt are important for ethical conduct • Internalization of standards is vital • Details of theory unsupported –Harsh discipline = less morality –Boys not more moral than girls –Underestimated when children begin expressing morality MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR – Newer Ideas about the Development of the Conscience • Toddlers involved in mutually responsive relationships with parents develop –Committed compliance »Motivation to comply with rules »Sensitivity to parent’s emotional signals of right and wrong »Beginning of internalization MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • The Cognitive Component of Moral Development – Piaget’s Theory of Moral Development • Studied respect for rules and conceptions of justice • The Premoral Period –Preschool age – little concern for, or awareness of, rules MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Heteronomous Morality –5 to 10 years old – strong respect for rules; they cannot be altered –Actions are judged by consequences, not intent –Believe in punishment for its own sake –Immanent justice – violations of social rules will be punished MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Autonomous Morality –Ages 10 or 11, rules are arbitrary agreements that can be changed –Rules can be violated to help others –Intent is now important in determining right and wrong –Favor reciprocal punishments – tailored to the transgression –Immanent justice is not accurate MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Moving From Heteronomous to Autonomous Morality –Cognitive maturation – decline in egocentrism, increase in role-taking –Social experience – equal status with peers is vital »Lessen respect for adult authority »Increases self and peer respect »Shows rules are arbitrary MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR – An Evaluation of Piaget’s Theory • Cross-culturally replicated results if his methods are used • Underestimated moral capacities of preschool and grade-school children –Difference between moral rules and social-conventional rules • Parents can retard moral development, but most foster growth MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR – Kohlberg’s Theory of Moral Development • Used dilemmas requiring choice between obeying rules or disobeying rules while serving a human need MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Level 1: Preconventional Morality –Stage 1: Punishment-and-Obedience Orientation »Goodness or badness depends on consequences of act – bad acts are punished –Stage 2: Naïve Hedonism »Conform to rules to gain rewards MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Level 2: Conventional Morality –Stage 3: “Good Boy” or “Good Girl” Orientation »Moral behavior pleases, helps, or is approved of by others –Stage 4: Social-Order-Maintaining Morality »Right conforms to legal authority; rules maintain social order MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Level 3: Postconventional (or Principled) Morality –Stage 5: The Social-Contract Orientation »Laws should express will of majority, and further human welfare; if not, challenge them MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR –Stage 6: Morality of Individual Principles of Conscience »Individual abstract moral guidelines that transcend laws »Rare (a hypothetical construct) »No longer measured MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR – Support for Kohlberg’s Theory • Are Kohlberg’s Stages an Invariant Sequence? –Individuals do proceed through stages in order –Stages are not skipped –Stage 3 or 4 is highest level for most people MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Cognitive Prerequisites for Moral Growth –Preconventional child reasons from an egocentric point of view –Conventional reasoning requires roletaking abilities –Postconventional reasoning requires formal operations –All have received support MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Evidence for Kohlberg’s SocialExperience Hypothesis –Parental and Peer Influences »Support for peer influences, especially transitive interactions »Parents contribute more than Kohlberg thought if they presented their reasoning in supportive ways MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR –Advanced Education »More education, higher level of moral reasoning »Contributes to cognitive growth »Exposes students to diverse moral perspectives MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR –Cultural Influences »Complex, diverse, democratic societies stimulate moral development MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR – Criticisms of Kohlberg’s Approach • Is Kohlberg’s Theory Culturally Biased? –Some aspects of moral development vary among societies »Cultural beliefs define morality • Is Kohlberg’s Theory Gender Biased? –Morality of justice for males, versus morality of caring for females »Not supported by research MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Is Kohlberg’s Theory Incomplete? –Emphasizes moral reasoning, did not focus on moral affect or behavior –Thought mature moral reasoning would lead to moral behavior »Supported by research MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Does Kohlberg Underestimate Young Children? –Yes, as his focus was on legalistic concepts –Did not examine distributive justice MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • The Behavioral Component of Moral Development – How Consistent are Moral Conduct and Moral Character? • Recent research shows that moral behaviors of a particular kind are reasonably consistent • Moral behaviors of different kinds are less consistent MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Learning to Resist Temptation – Reinforcement as a Determinant of Moral Conduct • Children generally comply with wishes of a warm, socially reinforcing adult • Praise is also important MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR – The Role of Punishment in Establishing Moral Prohibitions • Investigating Resistance to Temptation –Punishment should be firm –Administered immediately and consistently by a warm disciplinarian –Reasons for not performing the act should be provided MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Explaining the Effects of Cognitive Rationales –Reasoning can result in internal attributions (guilt, harm self-image) –Punishment can lead to external attributions – avoid punishment »Obey when authority figures are present, not in their absence MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Moral Self-Concept Training –Can teach children to avoid temptations based on internal attributions MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR – Social Modeling Influences on Moral Behavior • Children learn from watching others, if they know the other is resisting the temptation to violate a rule MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Who Raises Children Who Are Morally Mature? – Love withdrawal and power assertion, did not promote moral growth – Induction – explaining why a behavior is wrong and how it should be changed • Fostered moral emotions, reasoning, and behavior MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR – A Child’s-Eye View of Discipline • Children and adolescents preferred induction, even to permissive nonintervention –Recognized a need for occasional power assertion MORAL DEVELOPMENT: AFFECT, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR • Integrating the Components of Moral Development – Arsenio and Lemerise’s social informationprocessing model combines moral affect, moral behavior, and moral reasoning • Figure 14.8 2 A simplified version of Arsenio and Lemerise’s Social Information-Processing Model of Moral Behavior performance. ADAPTED FROM ARSENIO & LEMERISE, 2004.