How Controversy, Misconceptions, and Personal Beliefs Contribute to the failure of Educators to Effectively Teach the Theory of Evolution Patty McGinnis Advanced Seminar in Evolution Fall 2012 Miami University Abstract Key words: evolution, misconception, creationism 2 Introduction Perhaps no other science topic is as mired in passion and misinformation than evolution; an idea promoted by Charles Darwin with his publication of On the Origins of Species in 1859. In it, Darwin advanced the theory that evolution occurs via the process of natural selection; a process by which individuals best suited for the environment survive, reproduce, and pass on their traits. Darwin’s evidence, which came from observations of the natural world, did not explain the mechanism behind inherited traits since he lacked knowledge of Mendelian genetics, a theory that explains how traits are passed from parent to offspring. Today, neo-Darwinism refers to the modern theory of evolution which utilizes the principles of genetics to explain how variations in populations arise ("Neo-Darwinism," 2012). Today evolution is considered to be the greatest unifying idea in the biological sciences, much in the way that the laws of gravity are to physics (McComas, 1997). Up until the 1960’s, however, the topic of evolution was not addressed in high school biology textbooks. In 1956, a committee of distinguished biologists representing AIBS (American Institute of Biological Sciences) met and recommended that biology instruction be reformed to include the topic of evolution (Bybee, 2001). The National Defense Act, passed in 1958, provided funding for the development of updated science books. As a result of these events, the Biological Sciences Curriculum Study (BSCS) was the first to produce a book that addressed the controversial topic of evolution (Bybee, 2001). Despite fifty years of the inclusion of evolution into high school biology, the theory of evolution remains plagued with misconceptions (Gregg, Janssen, & Bhattacharjee, 2003; Gregory & Ellis, 2009) and continues to face opposition from creationists (Bybee, 2001). Unlike other scientific theories, such as the atomic theory or Copernicus’ heliocentric theory, the theory of evolution remains one that is heavily questioned, even after 150 years of discussion (Ayala, 2000). Misconceptions regarding the theory, combined with the emotional response it triggers, often translate into controversy for school systems (Moore, 2004). Statements such as “evolution is just a theory” or “evolution is far from being proven scientifically” continue to perpetuate the idea that there is little scientific basis for evolution and point to a lapse in understanding what is meant by the nature of science and the role that evidence has in the development of a theory (Bybee, 2001). This paper will examine the state of evolution in today’s classrooms, including the difficulties and misconceptions encountered when teaching evolution, and the role that teachers have in promoting misconceptions related to evolution. Challenges to Teaching Evolution Teaching evolution is a challenging and often emotionally charged endeavor; some teachers report being harassed by parents, school board members, and community members when the topic of evolution is discussed within the high school classroom (Oliver, 2011). Additionally, teachers often do not understand the legal precedents associated with evolution education (Moore, 2004); although communities, school boards, and parents may challenge a teacher who teaches evolution, it is against the law to ban the teaching of evolution (Epperson v. Arkansas, 1968). Studies have found that many teachers do not want to teach evolution because they do not feel fully prepared to teach it (Aguillard, 1999; Griffith & Brem, as cited by (Nehm, Kim, & Sheppard, 2009) because they either lack content knowledge or lack teaching strategies to adequately teach the topic (Oliver, 2011). For the past three decades the evidence indicates that 3 traditional teaching of evolution is ineffective (Terenzini & Pascarella, 1994). This is in part due to the difficulty of the topic; students fail to fully comprehend the theory of evolution (Enderle, Smith, & Southerland, 2009), due to the necessity of understanding complex and abstract concepts such as molecular genetics, biochemistry, and geologic time (Bean, Sinatra, & Schrader, 2010; Burton & Dobson, 2009; Gregg, et al., 2003). Abstract topics require formal reasoning ability and often result in smaller learning gains than concrete topics. Theoretical concepts, such as evolution, are among the most difficult of all to master (Timmerman, Strickland, & Carstensen, 2008). The inability to accurately read cladograms, diagrams designed to show the relationships between organisms, adds to the difficulty of understanding how evolution works (Meir, Perry, Herron, & Kingsolver, 2007). This inability continues well into college; 25% of college students in upper-level undergraduate courses had difficulty reading these commonly-found diagrams present in most biology texts (Meir, et al., 2007). Misconceptions In addition to the challenge of addressing abstract concepts, teachers need to be cognizant that students enter their classroom with numerous misconceptions based on prior knowledge (Oliver, 2011). Misconceptions are constructed when incorrect reasoning occurs as newlyencountered information is analyzed to determine its relationship to already existing information (Abdi, 2006; Gooding & Metz, 2011). Information is either assimilated to the learner’s existing belief system, altered to fit their belief system, or discarded (Smith & Ragan, 2005). Knowledge that is contrary to existing beliefs is either rejected or causes the individual to restructure their thinking (Gooding & Metz, 2011; Winn, 2004). Restructuring of a learner’s thinking, when misconceptions are replaced with correct conceptual understanding, is very challenging since many misconceptions are deeply engrained. Even after having been taught a concept, many students may hold onto their incorrect misconception (Gooding & Metz, 2011). Evolution is no exception; so strong are misconceptions concerning evolution that instead of adopting and accepting new knowledge, the new knowledge may be discarded or altered in such a way that it fits existing misconceptions (Alters & Nelson, 2002). There are several origins of misconceptions related to evolution, including those acquired from experience, self-constructed misconceptions, misconceptions that were taught and learned (such as seeing dinosaurs and humans incorrectly depicted together in books or movies), vernacular misconceptions in which science words such as “theory” are not used correctly, and religious misconceptions (Alters & Nelson, 2002). Most misconceptions can be traced to the origin of new traits, the role of variation, and how the populations change over time due to traits (Bishop & Anderson; Settlage & Odom, as cited by (Oliver, 2011). All misconceptions pose a challenge to the teaching of evolution and their immense variety makes it impossible to address each one (Nelson, 2008), but teachers can design their lessons to address those misconceptions that are the most common. A study by Mayr in 1982 (cited by Alters & Nelson, 2002) revealed three major misconceptions held by college students. The first was that students thought the environment, rather than variation in species, caused traits to change over time. The second was that students did not consider genetic variation in individuals to be important. Thirdly, the students failed to recognize that as individuals with specific traits increase in numbers and pass on their genes, that the traits eventually become established in a population. 4 Students often use teleological and Lamarckian concepts to explain their misconceptions. Teleological explanations imply that a species deliberately sets out to change in response to an environmental factor (Geraedts & Boersma, 2006) while Lamarckian reasoning says that acquired characteristics are passed onto offspring (Oliver, 2011). Explaining evolution via teleological and Lamarckian logic may be easier for students than explaining evolution using molecular genetics (Gregg, et al., 2003). Other misconceptions include that evolution occurs at the population level, not at the individual level (Geraedts & Boersma, 2006), that all mutations are detrimental to organisms, and that the direction of evolution is predetermined toward progressing to more complicated life forms (Alters & Nelson, as cited by Burton & Dobson, 2009). Misconceptions can be found at all levels, even among college students, as is evidenced by the frustration of university professors with the lack of student understanding concerning evolution (Timmerman, et al., 2008). Nehm & Reilly (2007) examined views on evolution of students in an introductory biology course. They discovered that only 3.4% could adequately explain natural selection using scientific concepts to support their thinking despite prior exposure to the concepts of evolution, evidence of the persistence of misconceptions related to evolution. Additional studies support these findings. Gregory & Ellis (2009) examined postdoctoral science students to determine their understanding of evolution. Among the 186 students who participated, although most recognized that evolution is an important concept in biology, the most common misconception was that of teleological thinking; that need drives mutations. An alarming 30% were not personally convinced that evolution occurs. Addressing Misconceptions Addressing misconceptions is a challenging procedure. The process of addressing misconceptions is two-fold; first, misconceptions must be exposed. Secondly, instruction that forces the learner to confront their misconceptions must occur (Oliver, 2011). Exposure of misconceptions can be accomplished via an assessment probe; a multiple-choice question that contains common misconceptions related to a topic. An assessment probe designed to expose misconceptions regarding evolution may be one that prompts students to explain how life originated (Keeley & Tugel, 2009). During the assessment probe, students are asked to construct a written response to explain their thinking. Follow-up lessons are then used to create a sense of discomfort within the student as they confront their misconceptions, question their thinking, and begin to create correct conceptual understanding (Keeley, Eberle, & Farrin, 2005). Firmly engrained misconceptions require numerous hands-on or concrete experiences to correct (Khourey-Bowers, 2011; Olson, 2009), in some cases requiring years of exposure to accurate concepts before a misconception is corrected (Chinn & Brewer, 1993). Methods commonly employed by teachers to address the abstract topics of evolution and natural selection include the use of text, lecture, video, discussions and debate (Riechert, Leander, & Lenhart, 2011). Allowing students to voice their opinions and confront their misconceptions via questioning, inquiry, and investigation is important, however (Sinclair & Pendarvis, 1998). Nehm & Reilly (2007) examined the effectiveness of active learning teaching methods on misconceptions related to evolution. They compared a group of biology majors who participated in an active-learning class to a group of biology majors who were instructed about evolution in a tradition setting. Teaching strategies employed by the active learning class included cooperative learning and inquiry; while the traditional class was taught using a didactic approach. Although active learning was found to be more successful at extinguishing misconceptions, the difficulty of fully understanding evolution and its driving forces was 5 pronounced; 70% of students in the active learning class and 86% of the traditional class retained misconceptions after instruction. This study is particularly alarming since it dealt with future high school biology teachers (Nehm & Reilly, 2007). Utilizing simulation software such as “Spore,” in which students “create and guide a creature through five generations of evolution” (“What is Spore,” n.d.) is another method for teaching about evolution. It is important, however, that regardless of the activity, teachers monitor students to ensure misconceptions are being corrected, rather than reinforced, as could occur when using the game Spore. Spore involves the creation of a successful individual without witnessing how mutations affect subsequent populations. Its use may reinforce the misconception that evolution occurs on an individual, rather than population, basis. Spore may also act to perpetuate and reinforce misconceptions such as the idea of essentialism; that an organism’s qualities are essential to its survival and that change does not occur over time (Bean, et al., 2010). As game-based learning moves to mainstream education as has been predicted within the next two to three years (Johnson, Smith, Willis, Levine, & Haywood, 2011), science teachers will need to carefully evaluate games for misconceptions. The Role of the Science Teacher Despite molecular and genetic information gleaned in the last twenty years, public resistance to evolution has strengthened (Nelson, 2008). Belief in creationism is on the rise; a Gallup poll revealed that when asked about evolution, 45% of Americans chose the response "God created human beings pretty much in their present form at one time within the last 10,000 years or so,” with only 35% of the respondents responding that the theory of evolution is supported by evidence (Brooks, as cited by Moore, Brooks, & Cotner, 2011). In view of this survey, it is perhaps not unsurprising that the controversy caused by evolution seems to be heating up as creationists demand that creationism to be taught in the classroom (Kaplan, as cited by Sinclair & Pendarvis, 1998). When teaching evolution, teachers need to be aware of student beliefs (Sinclair & Pendarvis, 1998) since understanding how evolution works is highly influenced by religious beliefs (Clores & Limjap, 2006) and by the background many students received well before entering a high school biology classroom (Riechert, et al., 2011). McKeachie, Lin, and Strayer (as cited by Clores & Limjak, 2006) found those students that believed in creationism were more likely to drop biology classes or do poorly; students who accepted the theory of evolution often do so based on misconceptions. In light of these facts, it is not surprising that the National Science Teacher s Association’s position statement on the teaching of evolution states that teaching evolution is necessary for science literacy and should be included in K-12 standards (“The Teaching of Evolution,” 2003). The quality of state standards related to evolution, and the level to which they are carried out within individual science classrooms across the nation, vary considerably. A study of state standards found that 30% of the states do not teach evolution satisfactorily; ten states do not even include the word “evolution” in their standards (Lerner, 2000). Personal conflicts between religion and science cause stress for science teachers (Griffith & Brem, as cited by Nehm, Kim, & Sheppard, 2009); in many cases, teachers sacrifice scientific evidence of evolution to personal religious beliefs (Kraemer; Trani; Berkman et al., as cited by Moore et al., 2011). Nearly 25% of biology teachers believe that creationism is scientifically valid Kraemer, 1995; Moore & Kraemer, 2005 as cited by Moore, Brooks, & Cotner, 2011) and 15% believe evolution is not scientifically valid (Moore & Kraemer, 2005). Forty percent of 6 Indiana’s biology teachers (a state that has among the best evolution standards in the country) admitted that they either avoided or only briefly mentioned evolution. In Oregon, 39% of the biology teachers feel creationism should be taught in science class (Moore, 2004), while in Louisiana, 29% of the science teachers want to teach creationism in their classes. Even teachers who participated in a 14-week course designed to increase their content knowledge did not change their opinion that creationism should be taught in school (Nehm, Kim, & Sheppard, 2009). It is alarming that many teachers may be choosing to ignore standards or teach their own beliefs, despite the fact that it is against the law to teach creationism in public school science classes (Edwards v. Aguillard, as cited by Moore, 2004). Exposure to creationism in high school sacrifices time that could have been spent understanding evolution. A study by Moore et al. (2011) identified factors that influenced students who entered college knowing little of evolution. Ignorance of evolution was found to be positively correlated to religious beliefs, to a lack of evolution instruction during high school, and to exposure of creationism-related instruction in high school. Of these factors, the inclusion of creationism in a high school biology class had the greatest weight. Those students who were exposed to creationism in high school science knew less about evolution than their peers who had not been exposed to creationism. These findings extended to science majors as well as nonscience majors. Additionally, students whose high school biology course included evolution topics were more likely to believe in the theory of evolution than students whose high school biology course had included discussions of creationism (Moore & Cotner, as cited by Moore et al., 2011). Conclusions The role of science education is not to produce within students a belief in evolution, but to produce scientifically literate students; students who are capable of understanding the theory of evolution and the mechanisms behind it. Darwin proposed his theory of evolution over 150 years ago, but it continues to be entrenched in misconceptions which are perhaps unwittingly perpetuated by science teachers. Although teachers should make it clear to that accepting evolution does not mean rejecting religious beliefs, it is important to acknowledge that creationism does not belong in the science classroom. Creationism is accepted on faith, not facts; its belief leads to misconceptions which are difficult to change. The argument utilized by creationists that evolution cannot be proven because it is “only a theory” (Nehm, Kim, & Sheppard, 2009) presumes that ideas that cannot be proven by experimentation are not real science, a misconception often reinforced by textbooks that devote the first chapter to a list of the scientific steps. Large numbers of preservice teachers have been found to accept creationalism and reject evolution (Losh & Nzekwe, 2010), indicativative that preservice teachers lack an understanding of the nature of science; a deficit that they are unlikely to be able to adequately address within their future students. The ability to objectively examine evidence is tied closely to understanding the nature of science; the processes and skills scientists utilize throughout their work. Developing the scientific habit of mind by requiriing students to ask questions and learn how to separate fact from fiction is essential if students are to receive proper groundwork to enable them to think scientifically, particularly when it comes to understanding abstract theories such as evolution. Science teachers need to be a better job instilling an understanding of the nature of science within their students, including how to properly differentiate between words such as hypothesis, theory, and law (Bybee, 2000). 7 Successful planning and execution of inquiry lessons designed to allow learners to confront and correct their misconceptions will require that science teachers to be educated regarding common misconceptions related to evolution and natural selection. This education needs to extend to self-examination of the teachers’ personal misconceptions and beliefs to ensure unbiased delivery of content required by state science standards. Only by allowing students to truly begin thinking like scientists will Darwin’s theory become better understood. References Abdi, S. W. (2006). Correcting student misconceptions. Science Scope, 29(4), 39. Alters, B. J., & Nelson, C. E. (2002). Perspective: Teaching Evolution in Higher Education. Evolution, 56(10), 1891-1901. Ayala, F. J. (2000). Arguing for evolution. The Sciencce Teacher, 67(2), 30-32. Bean, T. E., Sinatra, G. M., & Schrader, P. G. (2010). Spore: Spawning Evolutionary Misconceptions? Journal of Science Education and Technology, 19(5), 409-414. Burton, S. R., & Dobson, C. (2009). Spork & Beans: Addressing Evolutionary Misconceptions (Vol. 71, pp. 89-91): National Association of Biology Teachers. Bybee, R. W. (2000). Evolution: Don't debate, educate. The Science Teacher, 67(6), 30-35. Bybee, R. W. (2001). Teaching about Evolution: Old Controversy, New Challenges. BioScience, 51(4), 309-312. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1641/00063568(2001)051[0309:TAEOCN]2.0.CO;2 Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: A theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 63(1), 1-49. Clores, M. A., & Limjap, A. A. (2006). Diversity of Students’ Beliefs about Biological Evolution. Asia Pacific Journal of Education, 26(1), 65-77. Enderle, P. J., Smith, M. U., & Southerland, S. (2009). Does Prior Knowledge Matter? Do Lamarckian misconceptions exist? A critique of Geraedts and Boersma (2006). International Journal of Science Education, 31(18), 2527-2532. doi: 10.1080/09500690802428106 Epperson v. Arkansas, 393 U.S. 97, 37 U.S. Law Week 4017, 89 S. Ct. 266, 21 L. Ed 228 (1968). Geraedts, C. L., & Boersma, K. T. (2006). Reinventing natural selection. International Journal of Science Education, 28(8), 843-870. doi: 10.1080/09500690500404722 Gooding, J., & Metz, B. (2011). From misconceptions to conceptual change. The Science Teacher, 78(4), 34-37. Gregg, T. G., Janssen, G. R., & Bhattacharjee, J. K. (2003). Teaching Guide to Evolution. The Science Teacher, 70(8), 24-31. Gregory, T. R., & Ellis, C. A. J. (2009). Conceptions of Evolution among Science Graduate Students. [Article]. BioScience, 59(9), 792-799. doi: 10.1525/bio.2009.59.9.11 Johnson, L., Smith, R., Willis, H., Levine, A., and Haywood, K., (2011). The 2011 Horizon Report. Austin, Texas: The New Media Consortium. Available from http://www.nmc.org/pdf/2011-Horizon-Report.pdf Keeley, P., Eberle, F., & Farrin, L. (2005). Uncovering student ideas in science. Arlington, VA: NSTA Press. 8 Keeley, P., & Tugel, J. (2009). Uncovering Student Ideas in Science, Volume 4: 25 New Formative Assessment Probes. Alexandria: National Science Teacher Association. Khourey-Bowers, C. (2011). Active learning strategies: The top 10 strategies to help students overcome their naïve conceptions of science. The Science Teacher, 78(4), 38-42. Lerner, L. S. (2000). Good science, bad science: Teaching evolution in the states. Washington (DC): The Thomas B Fordham Foundation. Retrieved from http://www.eric.ed.gov/PDFS/ED447099.pdf Losh, S. C., & Nzekwe, B. (2010). Creatures in the classroom: Preservice teacher beliefs about fantastic beasts, magic, extraterrestrials, evolution and creationism. Science and Education, 20, 473-489. doi: DOI 10.1007/s11191-010-9268-5 McComas, W. F. (1997). The discovery & Nature of Evolution by Natural Selection: Misconceptions & Lessons from the History of Science. American Biology Teacher, 59(8), 492-501. Meir, E., Perry, J., Herron, J. C., & Kingsolver, J. (2007). College Students' Misconceptions About Evolutionary Trees. National Association of Biology Teachers The American Biology Teacher, 69(7), e71-e76. Moore, R., Brooks, D. C., & Cotner, S. (2011). The Relation of High School Biology Courses & Students' Religious Beliefs to College Students' Knowledge of Evolution. ambt The American Biology Teacher, 73(4), 222-226. Moore R and Kraemer K. 2005. The teaching of evolution and creationism in Minnesota. Am Biol Teach, (67):457–67. Moore, R. (2004). State standards and evolution: Are standards relevant to the teaching of evolution in public school biology classrooms? The Sciencce Teacher, 71(6), 41-44. National Science Teachers Association. Position Statement: The teaching of evolution. Retrieved from http://www.nsta.org/about/positions/evolution.aspx Nehm, R. H., Kim, S. Y., & Sheppard, K. (2009). Academic Preparation in Biology and Advocacy for Teaching Evolution: Biology Versus Non-Biology Teachers. Science Education, 93, 1122-1146. doi: DOI 10.1002/sce.20340 Nehm, R. H., & Reilly, L. (2007). Biology Majors' Knowledge and Misconceptions of Natural Selection. [Article]. BioScience, 57(3), 263-272. doi: 10.1641/b570311 Nelson, C. E. (2008). Teaching evolution (and all of biology) more effectively: Strategies for engagement, critical reasoning, and confronting misconceptions. Integrative and comparative biology, 48(2), 213-225. doi: 10.1093/icb/icn027 Neo-Darwinism. (2012). In Wikipedia: Wikipedia. Available from http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Neo-Darwinism Oliver, M. (2011). Teaching and learning evolution: Testing the principles of a constructivist approach through action research. [Article]. Teaching Science: The Journal of the Australian Science Teachers Association, 57(1), 13-18. Olson, J. K. (2009). Being deliberate about concept development: Effectively moving students from experience to understanding. Science Scope, 46(6), 51-55. Riechert, S. E., Leander, R. N., & Lenhart, S. M. (2011). A Role-Playing Exercise that Demonstrates the Process of Evolution by Natural Selection: Caching Squirrels in a World of Pilferers. ambt The American Biology Teacher, 73(4), 208-212. Sinclair, A., & Pendarvis, M. P. (1998). Evolution vs. Conservative Religious Beliefs. Journal of College Science Teaching, 27(3), 167-170. 9 Smith, P. A., & Ragan, T. J. (2005). Instructional Design, Third Edition. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Terenzini, P. T., & Pascarella, E. T. (1994). Living with myths: Undergraduate education in America. Change, 26(1): 28-32. Timmerman, B. E., Strickland, D. C., & Carstensen, S. M. (2008). Curricular reform and inquiry teaching in biology: where are our efforts most fruitfully invested? Integrative and comparative biology, 48(2), 226-240. doi: 10.1093/icb/icn064 What is Spore? (n.d.). [Software]. Available from http://www.spore.com/what Winn, W. (2004). Cognitive perspectives in psychology. In D. H. Jonassen (Ed.), Handbook of research on educational communications and technology (pp. 79-112). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Understanding evolution (n.d.). University of California, Berkley. Retrieved from http://evolution.berkeley.edu/evolibrary/misconceptions_faq.php 10