Two Essays on Social Media Usage: The Impact Social Media Mere Exposure on Brand Choice and The Profile of the Social Media Maven by William F. Humphrey, Jr., M.B.A. A Dissertation In Marketing Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Texas Tech University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Debra A. Laverie, Ph.D. James B. Wilcox, D.B.A. Roy D. Howell, Ph.D. Shannon B. Rinaldo, Ph.D. Mark Sheridan Dean of the Graduate School May, 2015 Copyright 2015, Humphrey, Jr., William ii Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Dedicated to the five women in my life who have inspired my career and encouraged my personal development over the years. I would not be here without your influence and support. Joyce Humphrey, Mable Shelnutt, Dr. Louise Luchsinger, Vicki Freed, and Dr. Debbie Laverie iii Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Much guidance and support was provided throughout the dissertation and defense from the committee, and I would like to recognize all four members who graciously served on this committee: Chairperson Debbie Laverie, Jim Wilcox, Roy Howell, and Shannon Rinaldo. I would first like to thank the Chairperson Debbie Laverie for her unwavering support and guidance; her willingness to take on a new research area of social media was critical to my success. In Essay 1, Studies 1 & 4 were conceived in her consumer behavior seminar, and her encouragement was critical in the completion of the research program. I also wish to thank her for her counsel on identity theory and role-identities. Her 2002 Journal of Consumer Research article inspired Essay 2’s investigation of the social media marketing maven from the viewpoint of social media influencer as a role-identity. Additionally her ongoing support of my development as a scholar has been unwavering; I seek to be the leader and scholar she is as I start my career. Dr. Laverie, thank you for opening up a world of opportunities. Second, I wish to thank Jim Wilcox for his support and training on measurement. The scales used in Essay 2 were refined during his seminar on the topic, and, his guidance on survey design during my time as his research assistant was incredibly valuable; I designed an instrument that was effective and efficient with the respondents’ time based on what I learned during his mentorship. His comments in the final manuscript refinement were of great help as well, and his support of me from beginning to end of my program have been vital to my growth. Dr. Wilcox, thank you for taking a chance on a non-traditional Ph.D. student with equally non-traditional research interests. Third, I value Roy Howell’s training in theory testing, particularly in the method of structural equations modeling. His deep knowledge on this strict test of theory was incredibly helpful as I tested the relationships of the constructs. As some of these constructs had not been tested using SEM, his incredibly deep knowledge helped resolve subtle issues that came up during analysis. His ability to take his vast iv Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 knowledge on this method and make it applicable and understandable was a key contribution to the completion of this research. Dr. Howell, thank you for teaching me a method that will serve me throughout my career and always letting me interrupt your Sunday work-time. Next, I thank Shannon Rinaldo for her support and mentorship during the design and execution of the experiments in Study 1. Her wealth of knowledge on experimental design guided my work, and she challenged me to carefully structure the studies with the greatest contributions in mind. Additionally, Dr. Rinaldo created the student research panel that introduces students to the research of the faculty through participation in studies. Without the creation and supportive structure of the panel, the studies executed here would have not been possible. She has always been very supportive with her guidance and generous with her time. Dr. Rinaldo always had time to guide me, even during the busiest time of her academic career. Dr. Rinaldo, thank you for pushing me to be better and to establish a research stream to support my faculty career. As I move to the next stage of my career with my first faculty position, I know that the guidance I received from my committee has prepared me for a productive career in research. Each has mentored me in different ways that will serve me well in my future career. I cannot overstate the generosity and contribution of Drs. Laverie, Wilcox, Howell, and Rinaldo. Thank you all! v Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ................................................................................... iv ABSTRACT ........................................................................................................ viii LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................................ x LIST OF EXHIBITS ............................................................................................ xi LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................ xii CHAPTER I ........................................................................................................... 1 Introduction ....................................................................................................... 1 Statement of Problem ........................................................................................ 2 Overview of Essay 1 ......................................................................................... 3 Overview of Essay 2 ......................................................................................... 6 CHAPTER 2 .......................................................................................................... 9 ESSAY 1: BRAND CHOICE VIA INCIDENTAL SOCIAL MEDIA EXPOSURE ........................................................................................................... 9 Involvement..................................................................................................... 13 Word of Mouth................................................................................................ 15 Types Of Social Media Interactions ................................................................ 15 Brand Updates ........................................................................................... 16 Social Media Ads ...................................................................................... 17 Brand Endorsements and Consumer-Generated Content .......................... 18 From Incidental Brand Exposures to Brand Outcomes................................... 18 Method ............................................................................................................ 19 Study 1: Mere Exposure in Social Media ................................................ 26 Study 2: Ad Units and Brand Stories ....................................................... 28 Study 3: Consumer and Brand Generated Content ................................... 31 Study 4: In-Group Versus Out-Group ...................................................... 33 Discussion and Limitations ............................................................................. 37 vi Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 CHAPTER 3 ........................................................................................................ 43 ESSAY 2: IN SEARCH OF THE SOCIAL MEDIA MAVEN: THE ROLE OF COMMITMENT, APPRAISAL AND IDENTITY SALIENCE IN PROPENSITY TO SHARE ON SOCIAL MEDIA SITES ........................................................ 43 Introduction ..................................................................................................... 43 Conceptual Framework: Propensity to Share in Social Media and Personal Factors ......................................................................................................................... 45 Methodology ................................................................................................... 57 Sample and Procedures ............................................................................. 57 Construct measures ................................................................................... 58 Analysis ........................................................................................................... 61 Findings ..................................................................................................... 66 Discussion and Limitations ....................................................................... 72 CHAPTER 4 ........................................................................................................ 76 Conclusion ...................................................................................................... 76 BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................................................................... 81 APPENDIX A: ESSAY 1 CONSTRUCT DEFINITIONS ............................. 92 APPENDIX B: ESSAY 1 SUMMARY OF HYPOTHESES .......................... 93 APPENDIX C: ESSAY 1 MEASURES & CONCEPTUALIZATIONS ....... 94 APPENDIX D: ESSAY 2 CONSTRUCT DEFINITIONS ............................. 95 APPENDIX E: ESSAY 2 SUMMARY OF HYPOTHESES .......................... 96 APPENDIX F: ESSAY 2 MEASURES ............................................................ 97 vii Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 ABSTRACT As consumers rapidly adopt social media, marketers recognize social media comprises an important part of the integrated marketing communications (IMC) mix. Social media is used to cultivate relationships with consumers; in addition other possible outcomes include consumer word of mouth, brand-to-consumer problem resolution, and influence on consumer attitudes toward a brand, such as purchase intention. In Essay 1, implications related to mere exposure to brands are explored through an experiment with four studies conducted with rapid presentation of a social media site. These studies test differences between types of social media exposure and brand choice in both high-involvement and low-involvement product scenarios. Further, content originated by brands for both high- and low-involvement products is tested against content originated by consumers as it relates to brand choice. Finally, the impact of reference group is tested on consumer and brand generated content about high and low involvement product brand choice. In Essay 2, the concept of the market maven in social media is explored through identity theory. Social media breadth (including network size, media commitment, and frequency of usage) is investigated in relation to two types of appraisal (self- and reflected) of perceived influence on social media sites. These appraisals are examined against identity importance; in the hierarchy of role-identities, participants who identify that social media is identity salient are posited to have a propensity to share product information in social media (as measured by the market maven construct). The outcome of this study provides initial exploration of antecedents of the social media market mavenism and what appraisals influence propensity to share product information online. Together, these viii Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 two initiatives apply the theoretical lenses of incidental exposure, social identity, and market maven theory to better understand how consumers behave in regards to brands given this new context of social media communities. ix Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 LIST OF TABLES Essay 1 Table 1: Summary of Hypotheses Table 2: Summary of Measurement Items Essay 2 Table 3: Summary of Hypotheses Table 4: Measurement items for Essay 2 Table 5: Estimation of Structural Model: Modification History Table 6: Parameter Estimates Table 7: Factor Loadings Table 8: Model Fit x Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 LIST OF EXHIBITS Essay 1 Exhibit A: Construct Definitions Exhibit B: Summary of Hypotheses Exhibit C: Measurement Items Essay 2 Exhibit D: Construct Definitions Exhibit E: Summary of Hypotheses Exhibit F: Measurement Items xi Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: Sample social media screen with high-involvement ad unit. Figure 2: Study 1 Comparison of brand choice Figure 3: Study 2 Ads, Brand Stories, and Involvement Figure 4: Study 3 investigation of product involvement and consumer or brandgenerated content Figure 5: Study 4 investigation of product involvement, content type, reference group, and brand choice Figure 6: Conceptual model of antecedents of social media mavenism Figure 7: SEM Results xii Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 CHAPTER I Introduction In relationship marketing, firms seek to establish and maintain profitable relationships with customers indefinitely (Morgan and Hunt 1994). To cultivate and grow relationships, marketers have many different ways in which to communicate with consumers and other partners as part of an integrated marketing communications (IMC) approach (Garretson and Burton 2005; Naik and Raman 2003). This integrated approach combines a number of diverse and ongoing marketing communications methods to engage consumers across channels. An emerging way that marketers engage consumers is via online social media websites, such as Facebook, Twitter, and Instagram. As of 2011, 83% of brands engaged consumers through some form of social media (Naylor, Lamberton, and West 2012), and social media marketing has been synonymized with word of mouth marketing (Kozinets, Valck, Wojnicki, and Wilner 2010). In this form of marketing communications, the firm seeks to influence consumer-to-consumer (C-to-C) brand conversations (Kozinets et al. 2010); a variety of communication types exist, including advertising and brand-originated content. Consumers have adopted social media in vast numbers; exemplar social network Facebook claims over 1.2 billion active users monthly, with sixty percent of users visiting the site at least every other day (Facebook.com 2014a). Additionally, time spent on social media websites is nearly double the time spent of other types of websites (Wilcox and Stephen 2013). As a result of this shift in online media consumption, brand managers recognize that social media must be considered in 1 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 integrated marketing communications efforts; many are allocating firm resources and successfully cultivating large communities. Exemplar brands that have cultivated large online followings include: Coca-Cola – 79 million fans (Coca-Cola 2014), McDonald’s – 29.6 million fans (McDonald’s 2014), Skittles – 25 million fans (Skittles 2014), Southwest Airlines – 4.1 million (Southwest Airlines 2014), Secret Antiperspirant – 1.8 million fans (Secret and Proctor & Gamble 2014). It can be argued that each of these brands invests resources into social media as a communication channel as part of a comprehensive customer relationship management approach. Each brand has cultivated large online networks in social media, and the respective product categories of each brand likely vary in consumer involvement; for example, consumers may rate airlines as higher involvement than a candy brand. From a scholarly research standpoint, the academy has recognized the importance of research into the emerging social media phenomenon (Kotler 2011; Sheth 2011). Day (2011) argues that consumer-to-consumer conversations in social media increase the complexity of marketing information that a firm must process to adapt its strategy. Despite these calls for research, investigations related to the impact of social media remain nascent in the marketing literature. Statement of Problem While researchers and practitioners both have called for research on social media consumer behavior because of the rapid consumer adoption, questions on the impact of social media marketing remain. Recent research on 2,500 of the top social media brands found that across six of the top seven networks only 0.1% of consumers 2 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 who follow a brand actually interact with that brand in social media (Elliott 2014). This trend implies that while marketers are cultivating large communities, explicit engagement with brands (such as commenting or endorsing brand posts) remains extremely low; justifying a significant investment in human capital for these activities becomes more difficult. Despite this low explicit engagement, do other measures of success that researchers can corroborate as resulting from brand social media activities through academic research exist? Is there an impact on brand attitudes and intentions, such as brand choice, from mere exposure to a brand in a social media context? Further, what factors influence the sharing of word of mouth in social media channels by influential consumers? If brand interactions are low, do other factors exist that further justify brand participation (and consequently, allocation of resources towards social media marketing and service)? These two essays seek to tie these ideas together through an investigation of two key points of brand advocacy, product purchase and sharing online word of mouth on social media sites, to provide insight on these questions. Overview of Essay 1 The first essay explores whether consumers who see a brand in a mere exposure scenario (incidental exposure without time for conscious processing) are influenced when faced with a brand choice at a later point in time. Much research exists on how consumers process information in high-involvement and lowinvolvement states; scholars have explored various durations of exposure as well. 3 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Additionally, investigations into the impact of reference groups, or consumers perceived to be similar to the observer, on product choice have been cited in the literature (Ferraro et al. 2009). To date, an integrative exploration on the impact on brand choice in a social media context when exposure is incidental (operationalized as mere exposure) has not been completed. Moreover, it can be argued that not all social media brand interactions are equal. Some social media brand efforts closely resemble banner advertising, while others appear as content similar to that shared by a consumer, known as sponsored content. Researchers have established that incidental exposure to brands has been associated with later brand choice (Ferraro, Bettman, and Chartrand 2009) when presented in very controlled photographic images. Some questions remain. Does this relationship change in a more complex social media environment where advertising, brand content, and consumer posts all co-exist in the same space? Are there differences in the likelihood of choosing a brand when viewed as a traditional online advertisement versus when the content is presented as a story on the news and updates section of a social media site? How does the influence of reference group (in-group versus out-group) affect brand choice? How do these relationships change in relation to a high involvement versus a low involvement product category? Essay 1 seeks to address these questions. In Ferraro et al. (2009), photographic images of consumers were presented. A number of images included a water bottle subtly placed with a logo shown. The scholars tested whether participants viewing a logo in mere exposure settings influences brand choice, which was corroborated. Additionally, the influence of reference group was tested, which indicated that whether the consumers represented in 4 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 the picture (either in-group or out-group) influenced brand choice. Building on this research, the present investigation employs an experiment that rapidly presents images with brand mentions or images embedded in the context of social media, in this case a Facebook-inspired fictitious social network. The studies within this research test whether incidental exposure in social media is related to future brand choice, while extending the previous work to include content generated by brands versus consumers and in scenarios with products pretested as high- versus low-involvement by consumers. The strength of the effect will be tested against the type of presentation, including display ad unit, consumer-generated posts, and sponsored ad/contextual presentation. Additionally, the concept of reference group will be tested. This research is designed to corroborate the work of Ferraro et al. (2009) that exposure to brands in mere exposure settings influences brand choice and that the influence of reference group further influences choice. It extends this work by testing the mere exposure effects in a visually and socially complex setting of social media websites. Additionally, it further extends the initial work on mere exposure that despite exposure to many brands (in the form of ads and brand stories) in social media settings, consumers are still likely demonstrate an influence in brand choice when mere exposure to the focal brand occurs. This work also examines the influence in both high and low involvement product categories, and it tests a variety of content presentation styles, including visual style (advertising units versus brand stories), source of content (consumer versus brand-generated posts), and reference group in consumer generated stories (in-group versus out-group). 5 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Overview of Essay 2 Continuing the investigation of outcomes beyond explicit consumer engagement with brands on social media sites, Essay 2 shifts from brand choice to an investigation of predictors of propensity to share brand-related content in social media environments. While brand choice is critical for firms, stimulating positive word of mouth from consumers is gaining significant focus (Brown, Broderick, and Lee 2007; Keller 2007; Kozinets et al. 2010; Lim and Chung 2011; Zhang, Craciun, Shin 2010). First, commitment has been defined as “an enduring desire to maintain a valued relationship” (Moorman et al. 1992 p. 316); it can be argued that the relationships fostered on social media facilitate this commitment. The concept of commitment to media consumption has been researched by Laverie, Kleine, and Kleine (2002) as an index that encapsulating the number of media types consumed by an activity participant. For the present study, social media participation will be investigated, including network size, frequency of participation, and media commitment as measured by the number of social networks on which a consumer participates. Together, these concepts will be referred to collectively as social media breadth, representing the degree of commitment that a participant has to online social media. Next, two types of appraisals will be explored; self appraisal provides insight into how social media participants view themselves; while reflected appraisal provides insight on how participants perceives others view them (Laverie et al. 2002). The importance to the participant’s identity of social media community participation is then explored through the concept of identity salience, which focuses on the importance of a roleidentity on the global self (Callero 1985; Laverie and Arnett 2000; Laverie et al. 2002; 6 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Reed II 2004). Finally, the concept of propensity to share online with a social network as a market maven will be explored (Feick and Price 1987). For researchers, understanding what factors predict sharing online word of mouth holds value, particularly insights related to heavy users of social media who have cultivated large networks. Word of mouth has been cited as important when consumers lack adequate information for a product purchase (Liu 2006), and social media and online channels provide a “megaphone” effect (McQuarrie, Miller, and Phillips 2013); consumers now have capabilities to broadcast messages to not only their network but also to myriad other consumers online. Accordingly, what influence does this identity salience have on propensity to share product information online? If sustained social media participation reinforces role-identity of an influential consumer in online communities, does that influence specific online behaviors (like sharing product information) (Wilson, Gosling, and Graham 2012; Zhao, Grasmuck, and Martin 2008)? If participants appraise their own influence online (and posit that others view their expertise in a particular light), how do these appraisals influence the propensity to share product information in social media environments? How does social media usage, including frequency, network size, and length of time participating in the communities impact a participant’s appraisal? Essay 2 seeks to provide insight to these questions. The contribution of this research ties identity theory to social media participation and the likelihood of sharing brand information with an online community, which provides a new perspective in research into predictors of word of mouth. Further, tying back to the overall research aims of this dissertation, insights 7 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 into word of mouth generators provides an advance in knowledge to compensate for the existing lack of consumer-brand engagement in social media. As Essay 1 establishes the importance of consumer-generated word of mouth on brand choice (Studies 3 and 4), Essay 2 further explores the profile of influential diffusers of product information through social media. Together, these essays aim to increase the academy’s understanding of the phenomenon of social media usage in brand choice and word of mouth scenarios. 8 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 CHAPTER 2 ESSAY 1: BRAND CHOICE VIA INCIDENTAL SOCIAL MEDIA EXPOSURE Consumers continue to adopt social media in staggering numbers; according to a Pew Internet Research study in 2013, 71% of U.S. adult consumers participate on social media sites (Duggan and Smith 2013); this number soars to 94% when examining U.S. teenage consumers (Madden 2013). Time spent on social media sites across all users is double that of other online activities (Wilcox and Stephen 2013). Facebook lists its active user base as exceeding 1.23 billion active participants monthly, while Twitter boasts 241 million monthly active users (Facebook.com 2014a; Twitter.com 2014). Facebook facilitates frequent sharing and consumption of shared updates via mobile; over 945 million users access Facebook via mobile (Facebook.com 2014a). Users are adopting social media in significant numbers, and their time spent on the activity merits investigation. That said, consumer interactions with brand-generated content prove minimal; research indicates that only 0.1% of consumers interact with brand content for the top 2,500 brands on social media (Elliott 2014). Based on this statistic, consumers are indicating brand preference through following brands on social media sites, but explicit follow-up consumer-to-brand interactions are rare. Given this fact, are there other tangible benefits for the resources brands allocate to maintaining a social media marketing strategy as part of a larger integrated marketing communication strategy? 9 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Every day, active social media participants log in to their account to interact with their friends, colleagues, and acquaintances. In some cases, they interact with brands explicitly, such as the act of liking a brand page on Facebook. In this case, consumers will see a subset of the updates that a brand releases. In other cases, the interaction with brands may be incidental and not intentional, such as seeing a brand advertised or mentioned by people on Facebook. Ferraro et al. (2009) call these brief brand encounters incidental consumer brand exposures (ICBE), while other scholars reference these brief exposures as mere exposure (Fang, Singh, and Ahluwalia 2007; Janiszewski 1993). Most social media participants see brands discussed, advertised, and mentioned through various sources, including the brand itself, strangers, and friends, each time they visit a social media site. The present research seeks to investigate whether incidental exposure to a brand in a social media context results in brand choice in varying contexts, including visual presentation, source of message, and interpersonal influence of reference group. Within social media sites, top consumer brands have opportunities to develop a large base of online brand advocates, providing an incremental consumer touch point within a brand’s marketing strategy (either via an opt-in relationship with the consumer or through incidental exposure). These brand followers are described as ‘liking’ a brand (Naylor et al. 2012). Within the brand community literature, such online brand forums have been characterized by traits of traditional communities, such as a sense of belonging, devotion to the brand, and lack of geographical-constraint (Muñiz Jr and O’Guinn 2001). Brand-generated content and brand participation in the community differentiate contemporary social media sites from those described in the initial brand 10 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 community research; early researchers defined initial brand communities as being built and maintained by the brand’s supporters with minimal formal interaction from the brand (Muñiz Jr and O’Guinn 2001). Initial research indicates that simply exposing the composition of a brand’s consumer fan base influences consumer brand choice (Naylor et al. 2012). While the effect of the composition of a brand’s fan base has been investigated, the influence of different types of brand exposure in social media on brand choice has not been considered. The purpose of this paper is to further Ferraro et al.’s (2009) work related to mere exposure and brand choice, extended into the context of social media, with consideration given to the source of content. In this prior research, consumers were shown a number of images for short durations; select images for some participants showed a branded water bottle, while others did not see the embedded brand. After viewing the sequence of images, study participants were offered a bottle of water of their choice as they exited the lab. Their brand choice was recorded and associated with the condition they viewed (brand v. no brand). Additionally, later studies added the influence of reference group, where in-group and out-group were signaled by university logos on clothing in the images. The out-group was portrayed wearing logo wear apparel from a competitor’s university to measure whether reference group further influenced brand choice after incidental exposure to a brand in a series of images. While the aforementioned research held empirical support of the impact of mere exposure on later brand choice, the complexity of the information processed by participants was low. Extending research by taking into account added complexity of different levels of product involvement, content presentation types (including the more complex visual elements found in social media 11 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 sites), and consumer versus brand generated content allows for an investigation of the impact of source of message (word of mouth versus brand advertising in story format), while the experimental design controls for potentially confounding influences. The present work aims to corroborate and extend the aforementioned research with investigations related to display ads or consumer-to-consumer conversations and brand choice, while increasing the visual complexity of factors consumers likely encounter online (including ads, consumer posts, website navigation elements, and other visual content). While investigations related to social media interactions may be an emerging area of the literature, the consequences of banner advertising online have been explored previously. Online advertising has been described as inducing “online clicking, which links the consumer to the brand’s target communication (usually the target company’s website), and ultimately on to a purchase” (Shamdasani, Stanaland, and Tan 2001 p. 7). Sherman and Deighton (2001) find that context of the site on which a banner is placed affects click-though (the act of the consumer clicking the banner ad to visit the site being advertised) with relevant fit between site content and visitor interests resulting in the greatest consumer click-through. Brand awareness and ad recall are proposed as appropriate measures of success for banner advertising, compared to click-through rates (Varadarajan and Yadav 2009). In their review of the impact of online marketing communications, Shankar and Batra (2009) call for additional investigation into advertising effectiveness and processing in light of consumer-to-consumer online communications, such as those found in social media conversations. Calder, Malthouse, and Schaedel (2009) argue that context affects 12 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 advertising effectiveness; websites that engage visitors to read content are more likely to have effective advertising efforts. Additionally, repeated exposure to an online advertisement has been shown to result in greater brand name memory, while varied ad executions have been associated with intention to click compared with same ad repetition (Yaveroglu and Donthu 2008). Levels of exposure to online advertising have been cited as important for brand outcomes. Varied attention levels (attentive versus inattentive consumers) to web advertising have resulted in varying levels of favorable brand attitudes (Yoo 2007). A positive relationship between level of exposure and brand attitude has been found, including attitude towards the ad, attitude towards the brand, and purchase intention (Cho, Lee, and Tharp 2001). To some consumers, research indicates that a “blindness” to certain ad placements may develop over time, including ads on the right side of a page like used on Facebook when compared with more centrally placed advertising (Hsieh, Chen, and Ma 2012; Hsieh and Chen 2011; Owens, Chapparo, and Palmer 2011). Involvement Involvement with brands has been posited as a relevant factor in online advertising; Shamdasani et al. (2001) propose that relevance of a product ad to its website environment is most important for high-involvement product advertisements where reputation is most critical when involvement with a product is low. Zaichkowsky (1995) outlines three types of involvement that consumers experience, including: 13 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 1. Personal—inherent interests, valued, or needs that motivate one toward the object 2. Physical—characteristics of the object that cause differentiation and increase interest 3. Situational—something that temporarily increases relevance or interest toward the object. (Bloch and Richins 1983; Houston and Rothschild 1978; Zaichkowsky 1995 p. 342) Each of the three types of involvement may influence differences in attitudes and intended behaviors towards a brand. From a digital perspective, online branded content may elicit higher levels of involvement (situational) in comparison to banner ads, which may influence consumer brand attitudes (Becker-Olsen 2003). Further, this involvement has been shown to offset the consumer processing efforts by influencing brand attitudes (Becker-Olsen 2003). Mollen and Wilson (2010) posit that involvement (personal) may extend beyond the traditional definition of consumer relevance and importance in relations to products; instead, involvement may extend to the context of an online website environment (situational), as evaluated by the consumer. The typical website consumer experience is described as low-involvement (situational) towards ads, as consumers allocate attention to their main activities of information search and entertainment (Cho 2003; Yoo 2009). Familiarity with a brand has demonstrated higher product involvement levels (personal) in online exposure than expected in other contact settings (Wansink and Ray 2010). 14 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Word of Mouth A desired outcome for brands allocating resources to social media marketing is consumer electronic word of mouth. Word of mouth has been associated with increased sales (Berger and Schwartz 2011), being an antecedent and consequence of retail sales (Duan, Wenjing, and Whiston 2008a), resulting in greater enjoyment of product consumption experience (He and Bond 2013), and serving as a surrogate to personal brand or product experience (Jones, Aiken, and Boush 2009). Online advertising has been associated with stimulating offline brand advocacy (like word of mouth), online brand searches, and website visits (Graham and Havlena 2007). The classic profile of a consumer spreading word of mouth has been described as diffuser of consumer information related to product experiences and consumption, also known as the market maven (Feick and Price 1987; Goldsmith, Flynn, and Goldsmith 2003). These consumers pride themselves on early adoption product experiences and trust earned through opinions shared with their network (Clark and Goldsmith 2005). For advertising efforts executed online, the market maven’s propensity to share with their network is of particular interest. If firms invest in social media advertising, propensity to share product information as an outcome may hold positive consequences, beyond the standard advertising effective measures of click through, purchase, or attitude towards the ad. Types Of Social Media Interactions Within this research, the context of the brand exposure will be tested to determine if there exists a contextual influence on brand choice. To structure this 15 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 experiment, a fictitious Facebook-like social network site was presented as the exemplar for social media interactions because it mimics the leading social network and provides a variety of brand communication styles. For the purposes of reviewing the types of exposure consumers encounter, Facebook is discussed in the coming sections. Branded “Fan Pages” epitomize the core of a brand’s presence on Facebook; this page type allows the brand to have a community ‘home’ and a forum for consumer discussions. Consumers typically show support for brands on Facebook by selecting the “like” button, and using this feature adds the brand to the user’s profile as a favorite. Also fundamental to the Facebook experience is the News Feed, which features status updates from a user’s friends and updates from brands consumers follow, and specific user-generated activity types (such as becoming a fan of a brand’s page or writing on a brand’s wall) will show up on that user’s wall (a message boardlike feature that displays a social media participant’s activity on the site and public notes left by friends) (Facebook.com 2014b). Brand Updates A promotional update acts as the most obvious form of brand activity within social media. The site participant’s News Feed broadcasts these updates, typically shared by brands to provide a tactical or promotional message to the fan base. Facebook allows brand fans to provide immediate feedback to the brand through comments or a “like” option. Additionally, a share feature allows participants to spread a brand’s updates, along with any comments, to their followers via the News 16 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Feed, assuming that their followers have not filtered out the participant’s updates (Facebook.com 2014c) Social Media Ads The next type of brand/consumer social media exposure represents paid social media advertising: the banner advertisement. For owners of a branded fan page, a link for “Promote with an Ad” leads to these display opportunities, paid through CPC (Cost Per Click) or CPM (Cost by Impressions). Ads are created within the online advertising module, and rich targeting opportunities are available. Brands may tailor ads targeted with exacting precision to users based on demographics, psychographics, or key features of their social network (Facebook.com 2014c). A second nascent type of advertising is identified as a sponsored story, which has not had thorough investigation in the literature like standard banner ads. This form of brand communication is also known as brand stories or brand-generated posts. In this form of advertising, a brand update is shown in a social media participant’s News Feed. The participant may not have liked this page, but the brand has paid to show up in the consumer’s News Feed incidentally. In this case, the brand update shows up in the main content feed and is not isolated on the right hand side of the user’s page (Facebook.com 2014d). 17 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Brand Endorsements and Consumer-Generated Content Another, more personal, brand exposure occurs when friends like (or become a fan of) a particular brand. As discussed earlier, liking a page will add the brand into the “Likes” section of a Facebook user’s profile page, and Facebook broadcasts this brand endorsement via the News Feed to the user’s friends while simultaneously appending the user’s “Recent Activity.” As users discuss brands on Facebook, they can tag a brand (which provides a link to the brand’s Facebook page) in their posts. These types of consumer discussions are how electronic word of mouth is shared on Facebook (Facebook.com 2014e). In the present research, brand endorsements are portrayed as consumer-generated posts mentioning a brand. From Incidental Brand Exposures to Brand Outcomes The conceptual descriptions and definitions provided above detail the most common ways brand and consumers interact in social media and key potential outcomes of these interactions; these concepts also outline the avenues by which consumers may be exposed to brand messages without active participation or opt-in. Much has been written regarding how consumers process information via central or peripheral routes of persuasion (Petty and Cacioppo 1986). Each of the identified social media exposures can be classified into either central route or peripheral routes of persuasion, depending on the context of exposure (quickly browsing versus deliberately reading a page of friend updates). Within the research proposed here, the focus on interactions is narrowed to incidental exposure processed peripherally, while 18 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 taking into account levels of involvement and content presentation style of exposure. To date, most research has focused on the frequency of exposure to online advertising in a digital environment versus the duration of exposure (Wang, Shih, and Peracchio 2013). An investigation into the influence on mere exposure to brand outcomes in an online, social media setting has not been examined in the literature, which the present research addresses. Based on prior studies related to social media mere exposure, it is posited that incidental brand exposures through social media are retained by the consumer, whether the participant remembers seeing the brand name or not; exposure to the brand leads to increased brand selection when social media participants are offered a later choice. As a result, four studies are designed to test the influence of mere exposure to brands in various advertising and word of mouth executions in various conditions, including ads, social media posts, source of content, and reference group, and involvement level. Method A two-part pre-test was executed to determine which product categories were considered high and low involvement and to refine the experiment. The pre-test had three goals. First, it provided insight into involvement levels by product category. Second, it also provided input on refining the experiment directions that guided participants. Finally, it provided corroboration that the research goals were obscured from the participants. The survey portion of the pre-test was administered to 139 19 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 participants at a Southwestern United States university via Qualtrics online survey software, while the experiment portion of the pre-test was administered to 15 participants using MediaLab & DirectRT software. In part one, the survey focused on involvement levels with various product categories to determine high and low involvement products for Studies 1 to 4. Categories included cellphones, pizza, athletic shoes, streaming movie services, cruise vacations, and beach vacations. Based on the findings of the survey portion of the pre-test, beach vacations were chosen for high involvement and pizza was chosen for low involvement as measured by (Zaichkowsky 2004, 2005) (M=4.081 v. 3.374, df=35, F=3.47, p<0.00001). The second portion of the pre-test ran a small sample (N=15) of test participants through the social media screens using the laboratory software. This exercise focused on clarity of instructions and resolving any confusion the participants may encounter. Participants were asked what they thought the research was investigating, and none of the initial participants guessed the research questions. Additionally, instructions were refined so that required actions (such as mouse clicks or user input) were evident without additional verbal instructions during the experiment. As heavy consumers of social media, viewing a number of social media proved to be familiar task to university students. The results of the laboratory section of the pre-test were excluded from analysis and were only used to ensure the studies were executed correctly. The pre-test and the existing literature in mere exposure informed the four studies executed within this research. Each study was conducted in a laboratory setting using a pool of student participants who participate for course credit. Participants who were part of the pre-test or any study within this research were 20 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 excluded from inclusion in later studies. The hypotheses in this exploration were tested using data gathered using MediaLab and DirectRT version 2014 research software, and analysis was performed using STATA 13.1. The data-gathering software enabled the studies to randomly display various social media screens for investigation at set durations to millisecond precision to simulate mere exposure and control for order effects. Because this research focuses on mere exposure, the duration was set and did not vary within the study or between studies. Ferraro et al. (2009) tested their studies on mere exposure using duration of several seconds (between 1-3 seconds), and the researchers deemed that duration did not have an impact on their focal construct of brand choice. Additionally, a meta-analysis of a mere exposure effects found that durations between one to five seconds showed a significant effect in tests (Bornstein 1989), and five seconds was selected based on the visual complexity of the stimuli compared to the work of Ferraro et al. (2009). The research conducted here employed an adapted version of testing incidental exposure successfully used by Ferraro et al. (2009) in their review of incidental exposure within the context of photographs. Instead of photographs, these studies simulated a number of social media screens to create the incidental brand exposures in the cited research (Ferraro et al. 2009). Only social media exposures that would be considered incidental were tested; and any opportunity for active processing of displayed brand exposure was limited based on the short duration of each screen. Established constructs from marketing and consumer behavior were adapted for this research. To measure the difference between ad units and posts on the fictitious social media news feed, the concepts were drawn from de Vries, Gensler, 21 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 and Leelfang (2012); ad units are represented in a right hand presentation with image and text, while consumer or brand posts appear in the central column on the page. Ingroup and out-group follow this work of Ferraro et al. (2009), with implied physical consumer differences shown visually. Whereas the reference literature presents clothing for determination of reference group, the present work focuses on age as a differentiator. Unaided recall was tested following existing protocol for online advertising (Danaher and Mullarkey 2003). The measure of brand choice was drawn from Karremans et al. (2006), which provided a proxy for product selection used by Ferraro et al. (2009). Table 1 depicts the hypotheses to be tested in this research, while Table 2 depicts a summary of measures employed in this research. 22 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Table 1: Summary of Hypotheses Tested in Essay 1 Studies 1-4 H1 Social media users exposed to a brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those not exposed to the brand. Social media users exposed to a low involvement product’s brand H2a sponsored stories via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to ad units for the brand. Social media users exposed to a high involvement product’s brand H2b sponsored stories via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to ad units for the brand. Social media users exposed to consumer posts about a low involvement H3a product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to sponsored stories for the brand. Social media users exposed to consumer posts about a high involvement H3b product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to sponsored stories for the brand. Social media users exposed to in-group consumer posts about a low H4a involvement product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to out-group consumer posts for the brand. Social media users exposed to in-group consumer posts about a high H4b involvement product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to out-group consumer posts for the brand. 23 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Table 2: Measures and Adapted Measures for Mere Exposure and Brand Choice Experiment Construct Involvement Measures or method tested To me, X is important To me, X is interesting To me, X is relevant To me, X is exciting To me, X is meaningful To me, X is appealing To me, X is fascinating To me, X is valuable To me, X is needed Drawn/Adapted from Zaichkowsky (1994) In-Group/Out-Group Represented by age (college versus middle age) Ferraro et al. (2008) Ad Unit/Post Represented as right-hand brand generated ad unit versus a post in the news feed (either consumer-generated or brandgenerated) de Vries et al. (2012) Unaided Recall Please list any brands you remember seeing from the social network presented to you. Danaher and Mullarkey (2003) Brand Choice I would like XYZ at this moment XYZ is something I would purchase if available If given the opportunity, I would choose XYZ 24 Karremans et al. (2006) Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Once in the laboratory setting, participants each saw directions guiding them that they would see twenty screens of a fictitious social media site, and they were instructed that they should focus on the layout to distract from the actual research purpose. To avoid a pre-existing bias towards an established social media site (such as Facebook), participants were told the social network they were viewing was fictitious. Because the layout of Facebook allows both advertising and posts from consumers and brands, this design arrangement was mimicked. Visual elements used on the page included a top navigation bar, a central area for consumer and brand-generated stories or statuses, and a right-hand section for traditional online advertising units. The layout used in each of the studies is presented in Figure 1. Figure 1: Layout of Fictitious Social Media Site 25 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Any identifying information on the site, such as the social media brand logo and other marks proprietary to Facebook were edited from images. Additionally, participants were exposed to two ads per screen, similar to Facebook. For Studies 1 and 2, consumer names and images were removed to avoid effect of reference group (which was subsequently tested in Studies 3 and 4). Upon completion of the viewing of screens, participants answered the manipulation check “What do you think this study was investigating?” before answering questions related to focal constructs. As a distractor task, participants were asked which brands they recalled seeing (unaided recall). Next, participants were shown a logo for the brand being tested (either a pizza restaurant brand or beach resort brand), and the participants answered three Likert-type questions about brand choice (1= strongly disagree, 5=strongly agree) drawn from Karremans et al. (2006). The design of the four studies to test the various conditions is discussed below. Study 1: Mere Exposure in Social Media In this initial study, the research seeks to establish that mere exposure to a brand in social media influences brand choice, thereby corroborating the relationship established by Ferraro et al. (2009) and providing a basis for the additional three studies. Testing of this hypothesis informs the subsequent studies that testing on brand choice via mere exposure extends to a visually complex social media context. Thus the hypothesis tested in Study 1 is: H1: Social media users exposed to a brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those not exposed to the brand. 26 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Method Fifty-four participants from the research participant pool completed this study, and 47% of participants were female. Consistent with the research design described above, Study 1 participants saw twenty screens of the fictitious social network. Half of the participants saw content related to a pizza restaurant across five screens, while half of the participants viewed no content related to that pizza restaurant. Twenty-nine study participants completed the eight-minute experiment for both conditions during regular business hours. Once seated, the twenty screens were displayed randomly using the five-second-display duration. Upon completion of the sequence of screens, the manipulation check was asked, along with unaided brand recall using five blank spaces for participants to type in any brands they remembered. Then the five choice Likert-type items related to brand choice drawn from past mere exposure choice studies were administered, and the reliability of this construct was measured using Cronbach’s Alpha (α=0.8087). These items measured the influence on brand choice if consumers saw the brand messages in a mere exposure setting. Results The results of Study 1 indicate that participants who encounter mere exposure to a brand’s advertising in social media are more likely to choose a brand compared to those that do not see an that brand incidentally. In other words, brand advertising in social media was effective. Participants who saw an ad reported a greater desire to choose the brand tested in the brand choice questions (M=3.075, vs. 2.787; df = 15, F = 5.6, P < 0.00001). This result is consistent with the findings of Ferraro et al. (2009) 27 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 regarding mere exposure to a brand and extends it into the more complex visual setting of social media websites with multiple items on which the participant can focus. Additionally, this initial corroboration of the influence of mere exposure on brand choice provided validation that Studies 2-4 will test differences in brand choice under varying conditions, including source of message and reference group. Study 2: Ad Units and Brand Stories Study 2 builds on the initial study in that it tests brand choice when brands are encountered in mere exposure settings, but it expands the test to compare the effectiveness of traditional advertising units versus social media posts originated by the brand (also known as brand stories) with low and high involvement products. In a 2x2 design, participants saw either the focal brand in ad units or brand stories for either high or low involvement products using the two product categories chosen in the pre-test. This design is represented in Figure 3. High Involvement Ad Unit High Involvement Brand Story Low Involvement Ad Unit Low Involvement Brand Story Figure 3: Study 2 Ads, Brand Stories, and Involvement 28 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 It is posited that brand stories will drive greater brand choice in mere exposure scenarios due to a number of reasons. First, the amount of space the content occupies in comparison to ad units is much greater, with the limited exposure to likely exert a greater influence. Second, brand stories resemble posts originated by a consumer, or these brand-originated stories may be perceived as content the consumer opted to see. Finally, taking into consideration ad blindness that has been shown in past studies, I anticipate the smaller ad units displayed on the right-hand side of the screen to result in lower brand choice post mere exposure. Finally, this study seeks to integrate Becker-Olsen (2003)’s examination of involvement and online advertising into a context of social media sites (versus traditional websites). Consequently, in this study, I test the following hypotheses related to brand choice, product category involvement levels, and mere exposure: H2a: Social media users exposed to a low involvement product’s brand sponsored stories via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to ad units for the brand. H2b: Social media users exposed to a high involvement product’s brand sponsored stories via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to ad units for the brand. Method Participants from the research pool completed the second study; 50% of participants were female. As with Study 1, twenty social media screens were displayed in random order. For five of the screens, the tested brand was displayed. For low involvement conditions, a pizza restaurant was shown, while high 29 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 involvement conditions displayed content for a beach resort. Ad units replaced one of the right hand ads, while brand stories replaced a social media participant post with a post originated by the tested brand. Manipulation check, unaided recall, and brand choice were again tested. For low involvement conditions, thirty participants per condition completed the experiment, while high involvement included twenty-nine participants per condition. Results Participants who saw an ad for a low involvement product showed a lower desired brand choice than participants who saw a brand story. (M=2.578 v. 2.944; df=12 F=1.27, P<0.2533), and the brand choice construct showed acceptable reliability as measured by Cronbach’s Alpha (α =0.8530). While the hypothesis was supported, this did not reach significance. The results of Study 2 indicate that ads influence brand choice more than brand stories in the high involvement scenario (M=3.022, vs. 2.793; df=12 F = 5.21, P < 0.00001), and reliability for brand choice was (α =0.8085). Surprisingly, the smaller ad unit performed better on influencing brand choice in mere exposure settings than the larger brand stories when the product advertised was classified as high involvement. While neither H2a nor H2b were supported while reaching significance, these findings provide insights into possible differences between mere exposure, brand choice, and involvement levels. The low involvement scenario supported the proposed scenarios but the results were not significant. This mixed result suggests that additional research as to why differences 30 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 exist in low involvement and high involvement scenarios could further uncover the nuances on brand choice based on mere exposure in complex settings. Study 3: Consumer and Brand Generated Content Building on Study 2, the third study continues the exploration of social media stories originated by brands in high and low involvement product categories. However, this study compares these conditions with content about a brand generated by a consumer to see whether consumer or brand generated content in mere exposure settings influences brand choice more. It tests the same brand-created content such as the sponsored posts in Study 2, but this exploration manipulates the source of content. In essence, I investigate the impact of consumer word of mouth versus brand advertising in a social media setting and its impact on brand choice where the visual execution is very similar. In this scenario, images of consumers are included on the posts, along with fictitious names. The conditions to be tested in Study 3 are represented in Figure 4. High Involvement Brand Story High Involvement Consumer Story Low Involvement Brand Story Low Involvement Consumer Story Figure 4: Study 3 investigation of product involvement and consumer or brandgenerated content 31 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Accordingly I posit: H3a: Social media users exposed to consumer posts about a low involvement product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to sponsored stories for the brand. H3b: Social media users exposed to consumer posts about a high involvement product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to sponsored stories for the brand. Method As with the prior studies, twenty social media screens were displayed in random order. Fifty-three participants viewed the low involvement scenarios, while forty-four saw the high involvement conditions. Participants sourced from the participant pool were 39% female. For five of the screens, the brand being tested was displayed. On those five screens, participants either saw a post originated by a consumer about a brand or a post originated by the brand. The latter were the same images used in Study 2, and a pizza restaurant and beach vacations served as low and high involvement brands again, respectively. No ad units featuring the brands tested were displayed. Manipulation check, unaided recall, and brand choice were tested consistent with the other studies. Results When testing the source of a message about a brand, the low involvement scenario was not supported comparing consumer versus brand-generated content (2.881 v. 2.874, df=12, F= 1.2, P<0.293) with reliability for the brand choice construct of (α =0.8430) as measured by Cronbach’s Alpha. Conversely, the high involvement 32 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 scenario did hold to the hypothesized effects, with consumer generated posts influencing brand choice more than brand-generated posts (M=3.023 v. 2.793; df=19, F=2.20, P<0.0075) with a reliability for the brand choice construct of (α =0.8714) as measured by Cronbach’s Alpha. Thus, in line with Study 2, low involvement did not demonstrate a difference in brand choice, while high involvement scenarios did. This finding indicates social media content generated by consumers discussing brands have a greater impact on brand choice than brand-generated content when the product is considered high-involvement. This outcome raises interesting implications of consumer-generated content and the impact on the attitudes of those who view this content. With the ubiquitous channels of consumer generated reviews and product feedback, consumers may trust other consumers (even strangers, as in the case with this research) more than they trust brands related to brand content.. Study 4: In-Group Versus Out-Group From the posited outcomes of Study 3, consumer-generated content will have a greater impact on brand choice than brand-originated content (supported in high involvement scenarios). However, not all consumers hold equal influence with social media participants, thus it is necessary to further consider the impact of reference group on choice (leveraging the high and low involvement conditions as with Studies 2 and 3). In this scenario, the relevance of the originator of the content is hypothesized to have an impact on the effects of the incidental exposure on choice (Zajonc 2001, 1968). Within a social network, contacts may ranges from close friends 33 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 to acquaintances with which little is shared in common. Both qualify as types of reference groups, which Bearden, Etzel, and Etzel (1982) define as “a person or group of people that significantly influences an individual's behavior” (Bearden et al. 1982 p. 184). The social media user’s relationship with the reference group, whether strong or weak, likely exerts a varying moderating influence on the strength of incidental exposure on future choice decisions (Ferraro et al. 2009). Furthermore, consumers are possibly driven to make future product and brand choices to conform to the tastes of others, which may be attributed to these online incidental social endorsements (Lee, Cotte, and Noseworthy 2010). In this scenario, it is posited that if a social media interaction is observed from either a friend or member of a strong reference group (such as employee of the same firm or alumni of the same university), the rate of brand choice influenced by incidental exposure will be stronger than for brands not exposed in this context. Ferraro et al. (2009) operationalized reference groups by varying university attire in a prior examination of incidental exposure and brand choice within photographs. Due to the challenges of portraying reference groups using real acquaintances of participants in an experiment, different age groups were chosen to portray in- and out-groups. The research design for Study 4 is presented in Figure 5. 34 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 High Involvement In-Group Consumer Story High Involvement Out-Group Consumer Story Low Involvement In-Group Consumer Story Low Involvement Out-Group Consumer Story Figure 5: Study 4 investigation of product involvement, content type, reference group, and brand choice Based on the above review, I posit the following related to high involvement product consumer posts by in-group consumers: H4a: Social media users exposed to in-group consumer posts about a low involvement product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to out-group consumer posts for the brand. H4b: Social media users exposed to in-group consumer posts about a low involvement product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to out-group consumer posts for the brand. See Table 1 for a summary of all hypotheses tested in this research. Method Participants in the fourth study were 39% female. Consistent with the prior three studies, twenty social media screens were displayed in random order with five screens showing the focal brand. On those five screens, participants either saw a post originated by a consumer of similar age to the participant or a post originated by a 35 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 consumer who appears to be decades older than the participant. No ad units or brandoriginated stories were displayed for the focal brands. The in-group images were the same images used in Study 3 as consumer-generated content. A pizza restaurant and beach resort served as low and high involvement brands again, respectively. Manipulation check, unaided recall, and brand choice were tested consistent with the other studies. For low involvement conditions, fifty-three participants per condition completed the experiment, while high involvement included forty-four participants per condition. Results When testing the reference group influence on brand choice, the low involvement scenario was not supported comparing in-group versus out-group content (2.992 v. 2.867, df=10, F= 1.2, P<0.3176), with a reliability of (α =0.8616) using Cronbach’s Alpha. Conversely, the high involvement scenario did hold to the hypothesized effects, with in-group posts influencing brand choice more than outgroup generated posts (M=3.319 v. 2.957; df=12 F=2.57, P<0.005), with a reliability of (α =0.8564) using Cronbach’s Alpha for brand choice. The findings from this study remain consistent with Studies 2 and 3 with low involvement scenarios not being supported; and in line with Study 3, high involvement scenarios demonstrate support. This study also corroborates the findings of Ferraro et al. (2009) that reference group influences brand choice, and it extends the effects of mere exposure into the more visually complex context of user-generated social media posts about a brand. 36 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Discussion and Limitations This work substantiates that mere exposure exerts an influence on consumer attitudes, specifically brand choice, when brand content is seen incidentally on social media sites. This reinforces and extends the work of Ferraro et al. (2009) regarding the influence of mere exposure on brand choice into a more complex visual brand setting with many elements competing for the observer’s attention. Additionally, it builds upon the prior work related reference group to social media posts as portrayed by age in both consumer-generated posts (Naylor et al. 2012). This exploration adds complexity from the referenced research from photos with brand placements to rendered screens of a proposed fictitious social network. It can be argued that the content on a social network, including advertising, posts, and other common website elements, creates a more complex environment for testing mere exposure. Additionally, this line of research extends the impact of brand exposure to include high and low involvement product categories, along with a comparison of consumer word of mouth (consumer posts) compared with both stories posted by the brand and online advertising appearing on social media sites. As (Naylor et al. 2012) demonstrate, revealing other brand supporters in an online social media setting has an influence on brand choice intentions, and this work supports this research in a mere exposure context. Moreover, this research sought to eliminate potential bias that participants may have towards an existing social network. Facebook, in particular, has been cited as losing engagement with teens and college-aged students (Madden 2013), and the scenario of a fictitious social network under investigation seems to have controlled this issue (at least for high involvement conditions). Also, these 37 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 investigations respond to calls from the marketing scholar community to investigate the impact of social media on brand marketing and consumer behavior (Day 2011; Sheth 2011). While the present research did not corroborate the differences in social media brand messaging types in low involvement contexts, it provides some interesting theoretical implications. First, high involvement products may benefit more from specific executions in a content-heavy social media site like the one portrayed. The visual executions that performed best were ads (compared to brand stories), consumer generated posts (versus brand stories), and in-group consumer-generated posts (versus out-group posts). Low-involvement products such as those tested here, by their nature, may not trigger the subconscious processes to demonstrate a difference between these executions. Thus low involvement products may be better suited to an alternate visual execution not available on the social media layout tested here, such as the more isolated content layout of Instagram. This visually simple layout aligns more closely with the single images tested by Ferraro et al. (2009), which were also limited to a low involvement product category. The findings from Study 2 related to the performance of ads versus the more visually prominent brand stories are particularly intriguing. This outcome runs counter to conventional wisdom that the larger brand stories would perform better due to prominence in the midst of other visual elements. Additional studies to determine advertising’s performance against user generated content and variations in the length and format of brand stories when tested against traditional banner advertising may yield additional insights. 38 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 As with all research, limitations exist. While college age students are avid users of social media, they are not representative of other age brackets; an opportunity for future research exists in establishing any effects on the findings of this research on various age groups. This varying of participant ages also allows for additional research into reference group effects with varying methods of operationalization. A sample including older consumers may also show different effects between advertising and brand stories due to the lower social media usage patterns. Also, as this research focuses on mere exposure, not various durations of ad exposure, the impact of duration of observation is not studied. For a more integrative examination, future research may wish to extend the present studies with varied duration times of brand exposure on social media. Further, because social media sites vary in context and format, less structured environments, such as Twitter or visual network SnapChat, may elicit different effects. Testing these unstructured environments in future research may provide value to future researchers. Finally, this research was executed using a standard web layout as viewed on a computer, and the influence of mere exposure in the more compact mobile device layout was not tested. This research holds significant potential for practitioners as well. As firms allocate resources to stimulate social media conversations and manage advertising on social media sites, managers may question the return on investment of these efforts. Standard measures of performance in online environments such as click through and online purchase conversions may well be supplemented with the investigated brand intention of future purchase. Also, the previously discussed banner blindness issue noted in the literature may be not be as prevalent in social media, at least in high 39 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 involvement product categories, as shown in Study 2. Additionally, consumer word of mouth exhibited greater influence on brand choice than brand-originated posts, reinforcing the value of positive word of mouth for brands participating on social media. This finding suggests that brands may wish to leverage customer testimonials sourced through social media as an authentic source of influence on future brand choice. Studies 2 through 4 also indicate that high involvement products may benefit more from specific executions, namely advertising over sponsored stories, word of mouth over brand posts, and in-group over out-group word of mouth posts. It also reinforces the value of rich segmentation and targeting to reflect the influence of reference group in social media. By portraying in groups relevant to target audiences in social media advertising, these ads may better influence later actions. Most importantly, this research suggests that brands that simply participate and advertise on social media may exert an influence on later consumer choice, and the duration of exposure required by the consumer can be quite short. If consumers are likely to quickly scroll through content on a social media website without purposely viewing ads or brand content, this research implies that this type of brief, unfocused exposure impacts attitudes towards consumer choice. Thus brands may benefit from simply having an active presence on social media. While not as easy to measure as clickthrough or website conversions, knowing that mere exposure can influence a later brand choice further validates the allocation of marketing human resources towards social media community management and advertising. The findings (both the hypotheses corroborated and not corroborated) raise interesting future directions for research. First, greater research on low involvement 40 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 products in social media in mere exposure settings may hold value, including varying the amount of content shown. This goal may also be realized by using less visually complicated social media sites as a template for the research, such as Instagram, which includes only a single photo per screen. These alternate approaches may later support the hypotheses not supported in the present research. Additionally, the findings related to advertising being more influential than brand posts merits additional investigation to understand in which conditions this holds true. Further research on reference group with alternate operationalization of in- and out-groups may provide additional value to both researchers and practitioners, and varying the method of reference group presentation holds value. For instance, if a brand uses images of a reference group in social media stories, how does the influence on brand choice differ from consumer-generated post from the same reference group? Additionally, varying content presentations extending the present research to represent other social media types (such as visual networks like Instagram or video-centric sites like Vine and SnapChat) merits investigation. Finally, comparing the influence of mere exposure to brand content on desktop versions of social media sites compared to brand content viewed mobile layouts is relevant as the majority of social media participants access these sites using a mobile device. In conclusion, the research conducted here sought to extend the academy’s understanding of the impact of different brand exposures on product choice in social media, which is now a prominent consumer activity. While the a priori hypothesized outcomes related to low involvement products did not hold, it suggested a number of relevant research avenues. The findings, however, suggest that simply seeing brand 41 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 messages in varying forms affects consumer decision making related positively to brand choice. This insight helps justify brand participation, including investing advertising spend in social media channels, when other metrics of return on investment may remain unclear. The future research directions prescribed here will further refine the understanding of both the academy and practitioners to the outcomes of brand social media exposures. 42 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 CHAPTER 3 IN SEARCH OF THE SOCIAL MEDIA MAVEN: THE ROLE OF COMMITMENT, APPRAISAL AND IDENTITY SALIENCE IN PROPENSITY TO SHARE ON SOCIAL MEDIA SITES Introduction Relationship marketing theory states that successful firms seek positive relationships with stakeholders indefinitely (Morgan and Hunt 1994); One of these key relationship types is between marketers and end-consumers (Hunt, Arnett, and Madhavaram 2006). More and more, marketers seek the aid of consumers in strengthening relationships through consumer-to-consumer sharing. Driving electronic word of mouth (eWOM) has long been cited as a goal for marketers, with the result of positive and significant word of mouth being greater sales (Liu 2006). Word of mouth also has another significant consequence, trust. A recent Ad Age study indicated that consumers trust the endorsement of their own networks versus endorsements of celebrity spokespersons (Daboll 2011), so tangible advantages exist to encouraging positive consumer conversations. To facilitate these consumer-toconsumer discussions, many marketers have begun to dedicate resources to managing conversations in social media. These online communities are now used to build relationships though customer interactions (Szmigin, Canning, and Reppel 2005). Online community members share product information, knowledge, product experience, and identity (Wang, Butt, and Wei 2011). Traditionally, consumers who view themselves as trusted product experts share product information. Whether these consumers view themselves as product experts or 43 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 a trusted advisor to their networks, word of mouth is spread. Feick and Price (1987) investigated the concept of the market maven, which they define as consumers who have market information (such as product information, location information) and either initiate conversations or respond to requests for feedback on purchases. These consumers are viewed as having an influence on purchase decisions by the network of these mavens Feick and Price (1987). Accordingly, these consumers perceive themselves as sophisticated consumers and pundits who have influence on their social networks. In the original formulation of this concept, the social network messaging was executed offline, via face-to-face, phone, or other traditional communication means. These consumers signal their product usage and consumption by making recommendations of the products and services they use. Today, social media sites provide a rich environment for mavens to share their message. (McQuarrie, Miller and Phillips (2013) describe the online social media sites as a form of amplification, taking word of mouth from a maven’s own network to a reach of friends and strangers, a much greater reach than the original conceptualization of the market maven. In the present research, I seek to update the concept of the market maven as an influential consumer with a propensity to share in social media, with a much greater reach than in prior conceptualizations. As social media participants are connected to others on various social media channels (such as friends on Facebook and followers and those they follow on Twitter), market mavenism holds value to an investigation into the likeliness to share product information to a social network. Central to this investigation establishing the role of identity salience of social media participation and propensity to share in social media, as defined by market mavenism, in relation to the 44 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 consumer’s commitment to social media and their contribution of appraisals to their sense of self. Conceptual Framework: Propensity to Share in Social Media and Personal Factors McQuarrie et al. (2013) explore the megaphone effect, or the ability for ordinary consumers to reach a wide audience through digital channels. Usage of social media sites for self-presentation and self-expression has been associated with positive self-esteem in community participants (Wilcox and Stephen 2013). Word of mouth has traditionally been defined as one consumer influencing another regarding a product or service they have tried, and this sharing of information from person to person has been called the most influential sources of information of all time (Duan, Bin, and Whinston 2008a; b). Word of mouth marketing defines the scenario further when marketers encourage these peer-to-peer discussions, often via consumer forums or on social networks such as Facebook or Twitter (Kozinets et al. 2010). These same needs fulfilled by word of mouth sharing have been associated with the purchase of luxury goods as a form of network signaling to foster self-esteem (Wilcox and Stephen 2013). Additionally consumers who lack expert knowledge may try to compensate for this shortcoming through sharing word of mouth with their network, known as compensatory knowledge signaling (Packard & Wooten 2013). Word of mouth has been found to impact the message receiver’s attitude towards a product (Martin and Lueg 2013). Lovett, Peres, and Shachar (2013) found that sharing online word of mouth recommendations first fulfilled social needs for the sharer, followed by functional and emotional needs. 45 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 The classic distributor of word of mouth investigated in the marketing literature is the market maven. Feick and Price (1987) identified consumers likely to share shopping and marketplace information to other consumers. The scholars suggest that these consumers are recognized and trusted by other consumers regarding product recommendations (Feick and Price 1987). These consumers have also been equated with “generalized opinion leaders” (Steenkamp and Gielens 2003). If market mavens can be targeted effectively, research has further shown that messages related to changes in a product’s marketing mix can be a useful strategy for retailers (Abratt, Nel, and Nezer 1995). Additionally, mavens have been noted as readily using coupons for personal use and sharing coupons with their network (Price, Feick, and Guskey-Federouch 1988). While the initial research on mavenism focused on offline sharing of word of mouth prior to the existence of the internet and online social networks, recent research indicates that mavenism spans medium type, both online and offline (Barnes and Pressey 2012). In this paper, I investigate key antecedents hypothesized to influence the updated concept of market mavenism in the context of online social networking sites. First, I discuss the factors representing commitment to social media activities in the form of breadth, including network size, social media participation frequency, and media commitment (as represented by the number of social media sites on which a user participates). Next, this breadth will be explored in relation to the identity theory concept of appraisals, including what the participants thinks about their performance on social media (self-appraisal) and what they perceive others say about their usage of social media (reflected appraisal). Next, these appraisals are tied to the hierarchical 46 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 concept of the varying importance of identities within the global self, and identity salience as influenced by these two appraisal types are investigated. Finally, the focal construct of marketing mavenism in the context of online social network sharing is investigated in context of the two appraisal types and the identity salience of social media participation. Drawing on the work of Laverie et al. (2002), this study seeks to extend research on the role of appraisals and identity salience with propensity to share. It also seeks to establish a new profile of influential social media participants based on social media usage factors tied with identity theory. If corroborated, this new social media influencer profile will inform marketing practitioners as they formulate strategy related to driving positive word of mouth. The model explored in this paper is presented in Figure 6. 47 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Figure 6: Conceptual model of antecedents of social media mavenism While marketers acknowledge that market mavens hold influence on other consumers and that online social networks serve as a nascent and growing channel of communication, little research exists in relation to propensity to share via social media. Value exists in establishing initial corroboration into what personal characteristics are related to inclination to share on social media sites (in the form of a social media maven). Social media usage factors, including observable features of social media participants (such as network size and social media participation frequency), in addition to media commitment are referred to as consumer commitment to social media in the present research. Commitment has been viewed as a recognition 48 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 of the value of an ongoing effort to maintain a relationship evermore (Morgan and Hunt 1994). One way that commitment to social media has been explored previously is through the frequency of participation, which is explored in the present research (Chen 2011; Hall and Pennington 2013; Jansen, Zhang, and Sobel 2009; Junco 2012; Ong, Ang, Ho, Lim, Goh, Lee, and Chua. 2011; Ryan and Xenos 2011). A further measure of commitment to social media is measured as media commitment. Laverie et al. (2002) define media commitment as consumption of media relevant to salient identities. Put in simpler terms, the number of media types consumed by the participant that reflects back on their identity can be considered his or her media commitment. For this paper, media commitment relates to the number of social networks on which a consumer participates. Thus if one consumer participates in Facebook, Twitter, and Instagram and another participates on Facebook, Instagram, Vine, Snapchat, and Google+, the latter consumer is considered to have a higher media commitment. A second form of breadth of social media participation would be the size of a consumer’s network across all social media sites. The number of friends in a study participant’s network has been studied in relation to personality factors (Moore and McElroy 2012), positive self-appraisal (Gentile, Twenge, Freeman and Campbell 2012), a source of bonding and social capital (Ellison, Steinfield, and Lampe 2007), a form of social compensation and self-consciousness (Lee, Moore, Park, and Park 2012), fulfillment of social attention and approval (Bergman, Fearrington, Davenport, and Bergman 2011), and a source of identity construction (Zhao et al. 2008). Personality has also been linked to the number of friends a participant has cultivated 49 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 on Facebook (Moore and McElroy 2012). In measurement, network size has been conceptualized as the total number of ‘friends’ a social media participant has cultivated on a particular social network site (Gentile et al. 2012). The final form of social media participation breadth to be investigated is the frequency with which these consumer participation on various networks. Frequency of participation on social media has been examined in relation to engagement, particularly with students; Junco (2012) investigated the link between Facebook frequency of usage with engagement in other student social activities (Junco 2012). Junco, Heiberger, and Loken (2011) similarly established a positive relationship between Twitter usage frequency and student engagement. Wilcox and Stephen (2013) linked frequency of Facebook usage to negative life consequences such as higher body mass index (BMI) and credit card debt load, establishing a relationship between frequency of participation and offline repercussions. The frequency of usage of Twitter has been tied positively to the gratification of the need for connection with others (Chen 2011); the implication of the aforementioned study is that social media is less a cacophony of online voices and more of a sharing and communication medium (Chen 2011). Additionally, frequent social media users were found to be less agreeable than less frequent users in a study investigating the Five Factor Model of Personality (Moore and McElroy 2012). Predicting the frequency of usage, a link between a extroversion and unconscientiousness and the frequency of social network usage (Wilson et al. 2010). Thus frequency of usage has a theoretical base for investigation in relation to the constructs of interest in this research. 50 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Given the stated importance of self-presentation and perceptions of the self by others as expressed through social media, it is logical to infer that both self-appraisal (or how a participant views his- or herself) and reflected appraisal (how a participant speculates others view his- or herself) holds value to the investigation of social media site usage. Richins (1991) argues that consumers compare themselves to an idealized self through advertising images; these same social comparisons can be found in social media participation (Hogan 2010) or creation of an idealized self as constructed through curation of social media artifacts such as Facebook Timeline milestones (Belk 2013). These views of the self hold clear importance to the investigation of social media usage and views of role identity importance. Laverie et al. (2002) define appraisal as a self-evaluation of identity performance. Identity serves as the answer to “Whom am I?” assessment versus global self-assessment (Laverie et al. 2002). (Kleine et al. 1993) posit that roles are consensual behaviors of those participants within a society, which is derived from social identity. It can be argued that within social media usage, participants take on roles salient to the identities in which they wish to enact. In scholarly research related to social media usage, Ong et al. (2011) contend that adolescents use four features of the social network Facebook to manage the appraisal of others, including profile picture, status updates, number of friends on the network, and photo count. The scholars argue that Facebook allows participants to facilitate identity construction and influence peer acceptance (Ong et al. 2011). Selfpresentation through social networking sites has been attributed with the search for both self-expression and self-actualization by participants (Livingstone 2008). Agreeableness and neuroticism have been associated with belongingness motivations 51 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 in social media participation (Seidman 2013), while narcissism, or a sense of inflated self-concept, has been identified as a motivation for participation on social networks (Bergman et al. 2011; Gentile et al. 2012; Mehdizadeh 2010; Nadkarni and Hofmann 2012; Ong et al. 2011). In an exploration of identity salience of mundane possessions, Laverie et al. (2002) investigated the concept of how appraisal contributes to a consumer’s sense of self. The authors establish a positive link between the two types of appraisal and the importance to one’s identity (in these studies mediated by ownership of identityrelated possessions) (Laverie et al. 2002). Drawing from this research, I posit the following relationships related to social media commitment and self-appraisal: H1a: The network size of a social media participant is positively related to self-appraisal. H1b: The frequency of social media participation of a social media participant is positively related to self-appraisal. H1c: The media commitment of a social media participant is positively related to self-appraisal. In line with symbolic interactionism, individuals view themselves as others significant to the individual view them (Felson 1985). While reflected appraisal is closely related in relationship to self-appraisal, the findings of the research upon which this present investigation is drawn found that reflected appraisal and self-appraisal are distinct constructs. Consistent with Laverie et al. (2002) because both measures use adaptations of the same items, these constructs are assumed to covary. Reflected appraisal has been associated with a greater attribution of response to positive views 52 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 versus perceived negative appraisals (Felson 1980). Reflected appraisal has also been investigated in association with the influence of media, indirectly affecting self-esteem (Milkie 1999). Further reflected appraisals have been viewed as a generalized other versus perception of appraisal of specific individuals (Felson 1989; Milkie 1999). Reflected appraisal has been associated with both real and imagined feedback, and the presence of others is not required (Laverie et al. 2002). In social media participation, the network or reference group of a participant serve as the referents for this reflected appraisal process, which in turn impacts views of the global self. Therefore I posit the following relationships between a consumer’s commitment to social media and reflected appraisal: H2a: The network size of a social media participant is positively related to reflected appraisal. H2b: The frequency of social media participation of a social media participant is positively related to reflected appraisal. H2c: The media commitment of a social media participant is positively related to reflected appraisal. With the widespread adoption and frequent usage of social media by consumers, it can be argued that the activities related to sharing and connecting with others online has gained importance to participant identities. Revisiting the concept of self-concept, Callero (1985) states that individuals prioritize different role-identities in hierarchies based on importance to the self. Important or salient identities are seen as taking prominent positions within the hierarchy, and identities are seen as remaining 53 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 stable over time (Callero 1985). In marketing, identity salience has been investigated in relation to establishing long-term business relationships by appealing to salient identities of prospective customers (Arnett, German, and Hunt 2003). Object relevance to the salient identities has corroborated the hypothesis that salient identities can be enacted when marketers attempt to persuade (Reed II 2004). Additionally, identity salience in relation to race and ethnic heritage has been shown to be related to favorable evaluation or behavioral shifts in the presence of heritage targeted advertising (Benjamin, Choi, and Strickland 2010; Chattaraman, Rudd, and Lennon 2009). While salient identities are viewed as stable over time, behaviors based on the salient identities may change contextually; individuals still retain flexibility in reaction within a group interaction even though their role-identities do not normally shift (Hogg, Terry, and White 1995). Laverie et al. (2002) investigated identity salience (termed identity importance) in relation to pride and shame possessions associated with participation in fitness activities. The scholars state that self-definition (and higher identity salience) arises from use and contemplation of possessions in association with identity-related activities (Laverie et al. 2002). Reed II, Forehand, Putoni, and Warlop (2012) extend the concept of identity salience to social media usage. The authors argue, “identity-related discourse in these groups also affords an efficient selection of concepts (objects, people, places, brands, slang words, etc.) that come to connote the identity and will receive the associated affective meaning. The continuously reinforced array of meaningful associations will in turn strengthen the identity itself” (Reed II et al. 2012 p. 315). Brands and pages liked in social media can be used to influence others’ impressions of the social media 54 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 participant; self-editing through social media sharing has been posited as a way of communicating role-identities (Hollenbeck and Kaikati 2012). Identifying with (or avoiding) specific brands via social media has been shown to be a coping mechanism to resolve conflicts between actual self and idealized self (Hollenbeck and Kaikati 2012). Establishing a link on appraisal and identity salience, I posit H3a: The reflected appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to identity salience. H3b: The self-appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to identity salience. As previously discussed, the classic marketing concept of the marketing maven focuses on consumers who are closely identified with new products and new product categories and who choose to share related information to their social networks. An association between identity salience and market mavenism can be established, as Feick and Price (1987) classify market maven as a role a consumer adopts. As Laverie et al. (2002) contend, sustained enacting of a role-identity reinforces the identity salience, and media commitment has been associated with identity salience (Kleine et al. 1993). Thus I posit the following relationship between identity salience and market mavenism as a propensity to share product information via social media: H4: The identity salience of a social media participant is positively related to market mavenism. 55 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 A further link to appraisal and market mavenism can be explored; Clark and Goldsmith (2005) explore the limits of market mavenism as it relates to appraised norms. The authors contend that despite market mavens being leaders within their networks, they will constrain themselves based on perceived normative influences (Clark and Goldsmith 2005). Further, mavens have been posited to seek to maintain status through their consumption of conspicuous products, indicating a concern for the appraisal by others (Goldsmith, Clark, and Goldsmith 2006). In this view, consumers express a desire for unique products to distance themselves or express dissimilarity from other consumers who are recipients of the maven’s shopping expertise (Goldsmith et al. 2006). Related to self-appraisal, self-involvement has been cited as a motivation for sharing word of mouth online (Hennig-Thurau, Gwinner, Walsh, and Gremler 2004). Further, word of mouth has been cited as a mechanism for selfenhancement (Engel, Blackwell, and Miniard.; Hennig-Thurau et al. 2004), which can serve as a way for consumers to potentially enhance self- or reflected appraisal. Perceived similarity and expertise have been identified as key apparent communicator characteristics for persuasive word of mouth communications, as enacted by mavens (Wangenheim and Bayón 2004). Thus, an argument can be made for both types of appraisals (self- and reflected) to influence propensity to share word of mouth in social networks by market mavens; I posit: 56 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 H5a: The reflected appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to market mavenism. H5b: The self-appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to market mavenism. Methodology Sample and Procedures This study utilizes a survey design administered to students at a southwestern United States university. University-aged students are a logical sample frame for this type of research as they are frequent participants in social media, with estimates of Facebook participation between 85 and 99% of college students, with 97% of those using Facebook (Junco 2012). Thus, the wide and deep usage of social media sites by the target respondent helps ensure the necessary number of relevant respondents. Students participated in completing the survey in exchange for course credit. In total, the sample included 317 participants took the survey over a two-month period. Of the 317, a total of 311 participants completed the survey and are included in the analysis (a 98% completion rate). Ages ranged from 18 to 55, with a mean age of 21.6 and median age of 21. The participant mix was 39.6% male to 60.4% female. Majors varied, and students could list a double major. Majors identified included 116 marketing students, 80 management, 12 MIS, 100 accounting, 30 finance students, 7 international business, 14 energy commerce, while 19 claimed another major. To participate in the survey, respondents had to be a member (active or inactive) of at least one social media site. For the sample observed, the mean number of sites was 57 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 five. The most popular network reported by participants was Facebook (95%), followed by Snapchat (82%) Twitter (78%), Instagram (74%), LinkedIn (63%), and Pinterest (51%). Additionally, 87% of respondents who use Facebook visit the site at least daily; with 87% of these respondents indicated they check social media more than once a day. Of these latter participants, 61% estimated checking social media more than 3 times a day. Construct measures As this research draws heavily from Laverie et al. (2002), measures for media commitment, reflected appraisal, self-appraisal, and identity salience were adapted for the present study. Consistent with the aforementioned research, self-appraisal and reflected appraisal items mirror one another, with context adapted by construct. For accessibility, the bipolar scales (notable, not notable) were adapted to a five item, fivepoint Likert-type scale. Media commitment was measured as the number of social media sites on which a participant was a member, whether or not they remain active on the site. Additionally, identity salience was measured with a four-item adaptation of Callero (1985) on a five-point scale, measuring the importance of an activity to a specific role-identity. Measures related to network size and frequency of participation were adapted from Junco (2012). Frequency was measured based on the number of times a user visited a site, including if he or she visited more than once per day. Network size was initially measured as a sum total of the size of people in the participant’s network. Certain social media sites include both a follower count (such 58 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 as the people that see the participant’s content) and following count (such as people whose content the participant follows). For the present research, only the people who follow the participant are aggregated in network size. Due to the extreme variance in network size (with a range of 35 to 4,759 people in a participant’s network), this measure is reported per 100 people in the study participant’s network, which facilitates model convergence. The market maven scale is adapted from the five item Feick and Price (1987) scale with five points. Each construct was tested via pre-test to assess reliability of the measures and identify any issues with the survey design. Construct definitions are summarized in Appendix D, and survey measures are listed in Appendix F. Hypotheses to be tested are included in Table 3. To test these hypotheses, the measurement items presented in Table 4 were administered by survey to participants. 59 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Table 3: Essay 2 hypotheses Hypotheses H1a: The network size of a social media participant is positively related to self-appraisal. H1b: The frequency of social media participation of a social media participant is positively related to self-appraisal. H1c: The media commitment of a social media participant is positively related to self-appraisal. H2a: The network size of a social media participant is positively related to reflected appraisal. H2b: The frequency of social media participation of a social media participant is positively related to reflected appraisal. H2c: The media commitment of a social media participant is positively related to reflected appraisal. H3a: The self-appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to identity salience H3b The reflected-appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to identity salience H4: The identity salience of a social media participant is positively related to market mavenism. H5a: The reflected appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to market mavenism. H5b: The self-appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to market mavenism. 60 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Table 4: Measurement items for Essay 2 Construct Market Maven Media Commitment Measures or method tested I like introducing new brands and products to my friends. I like helping people by providing them with information about many kinds of products. People ask me for information about products, places to shop, or sales. If someone asked where to get the best buy on several types of products, I could tell him or her where to shop. My friends think of me as a good source of information when it comes to new products or sales. Which of the following networks do you actively use? Choices: Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, Vine, Foursquare, Snapchat, Pinterest, LinkedIn, Google+, Other Drawn/Adapted from Feick & Price (1987) Laverie et al. (2002) Network Size How many <insert social media site> friends do you have? (divided by 100). Junco (2012) Frequency of Use How often do you use an online social network such as Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, Snapchat, or Google+? Junco (2012) Reflected Appraisal How would others describe your social media influence? Laverie et al. (2002) Notable, Excellent, Spectacular, Influential, Leader, Self Appraisal How would you describe your social media influence? Laverie et al. (2002) Notable, Excellent, Spectacular, Influential, Leader, Identity Salience Using social media is an important part of who I am Using social media is dear to who I am I would feel at a loss if I couldn't visit social media sites For me, social media is more than just visiting a website. Analysis 61 Callero (1985) Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 This analysis was conducted consistent with the two-step method proposed by Anderson and Gerbing (1988); the measures tested underwent a purification process (maximum likelihood confirmatory factor analysis) followed by an analysis of the relationships between constructs (structural equations models). Both the confirmatory factor analysis and structural equations modeling (SEM) analysis were performed using STATA 13.1. Table 6 includes parameter estimates, while Table 7 includes factor loadings of individual items retained post measure purification. Following Badrinarayanan and Laverie (2011), goodness of fit statistics of Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and Comparative Fit Index (CFI), along with Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) is reported to assess the fit of the proposed model; a summary of these fit statistics appear in Table 10. SEM was selected for this analysis because it is considered a strict theory test, testing the strength of each relationship given all the other constructs in the model along with guidance as to the fit (or lack of fit) of the model being tested. A path-by-path analysis is presented in Figure 7. The measurement model featured a X2 (134) =274.200, with RMSEA of 0.58, CFI of 0.969, and TLI of 0.970. For the initial structural model, the first loading of the error term is set to 1, and only latent constructs that have a previous theoretical relationship were allowed to correlate, specifically reflected and self-appraisal. All construct reliabilities exceeded 0.84 using Cronbach’s Alpha, indicating the items within each construct performed consistently with one another. Correlations between residuals were set to zero. Additionally, tests of assumptions associated with maximum (skewness and kurtosis) likelihood SEM were performed. 62 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 The initial structural model had a X2 (197)=937.814, P<0.00001, RMSEA =0.110, CFI =0 .839, and TLI = 0.814. Modification indices were run one at a time for each iteration of the structural model, and items indicating a high index were reviewed for conceptual similarity or closely similar wording. In the initial review of the modification indices, a high index of 134.919 indicated a fit improvement if the second and third reflected appraisal error terms were correlated. In review of the items, the terms specular and excellent were closely related, and the adjustment was made (lowering X2 (196) to 808.332). Similarly, the second and third self-appraisal items had an index of 102.675, and these items are based on the same descriptors (excellent and spectacular). Accordingly, these error terms were correlated, bringing X2 (195) to 707.979. Next, the first and second market maven items had an index of 87.852, and both of these items were worded very closely. Correlating these error terms lowered X2 (194) to 632.323. Next, items 5 in both reflected appraisal featured an index of 49.033. As these descriptors mirror one another (leader), these error terms were allowed to correlate, lowering X2 (193) to 544.165. Similarly, items 1 of reflected appraisal and self-appraisal (both using the descriptor notable), showed an index of 40.926, and the correlated error terms resulted in a X2 (192) of 493.229. Next, identity salience items 4 and 5 showed a 32.234 modification index, which was likely related to closely worded items. The correlated error terms in the revised model resulting in a X2 (191) = 460.321. Next, market maven items 4 and 5 presented a modification index of 34.176. As both items refer to consumers asking for product information and are closely related, the error terms were correlated (resulting in a X2 (190) = 430.562). To remain consistent with the other modifications allowing error 63 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 correlations between identical descriptors in reflected appraisal and self-appraisal, the error term between both construct’s item 3 were correlated, with a resulting X2 (189) of 409.964. Similarly, items 2 (spectacular) of both constructs were correlated, with a resultant X2 (188) of 408.584. The final error terms of items between self and reflected appraisals were correlated (items 4), with a X2 (187) = 382.572. Finally, errors of items 3 and 5 were correlated, based on closely worded items related to information about products or sales, resulting in a final X2 (186)=37.416. See Table 5 for the estimation of the structural model and modification history and resulting fit statistics. Consistent with SEM, all estimates were performed simultaneously. 64 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Table 5: Estimation of Structural Model: Modification History 65 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Following modification of the structural model guided by theory, the model exhibited adequate fit with Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) at 0.057, with a 90% confidence interval lower bound of 0.049 and upper bound lf 0.065. The Comparative Fit Index (CFI) was 0.959 and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) was 0.950, while X2(186) = 374.816 and these measures of fit are summarized in Table 8. Findings Self-Appraisal Network size (β=0.297, z=4.31, p <0.0001) and frequency of social media participation (β=0.180, z=3.52, p <0.0001) are related to reflected appraisal, while media commitment does not relate to self-appraisal (β=0.251, z=0.95, p <0.343). These findings demonstrate support for hypotheses H1a and H1b, while H1c is not supported. Reflected Appraisal Network size (β=0.030, z=4.86, p <0.0001) and frequency of social media participation (β=0.166, z=3.53, p <0.0001) are related to reflected appraisal, while media commitment does not relate to self-appraisal (β=0.011 z=0.47, p <0.632). Consistent with self-appraisal, these findings demonstrate support for hypotheses H2a and H2b, while H2c is not supported. 66 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Identity Salience Self-appraisal (β=0.292, z=1.87 p <0.05) and reflected appraisal (β=0.466 z=2.6, p <0.009) both positive relate to identity salience, demonstrating support for hypotheses H3a and H3b. Market Mavenism Identity salience (β=0.17, z=3.04 p <0.002) positively related to market mavenism, providing support for hypothesis 4. Self-appraisal (β=0.077, z=0.60, p <0.55) did not positively relate to market mavenism, while reflected appraisal (β=0.363, z=2.39 p <0.02) did positively relate to market mavenism. These results do not support hypothesis 5a and support hypothesis 5b. Table 5 summarizes these parameter estimates, while Table 6 highlights the factor loadings for individual items within a latent variable. 67 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Table 6: Parameter Estimates Parameter Estimates Dependent Variables with Predictors Reflected Appraisal Unstandardized Standardized Standard Error Z p>Z Network Size 0.03 0.292 0.006 4.86 0.000 Media Commitment Frequency 0.011 0.029 0.024 0.48 0.632 0.165 0.216 0.046 3.58 0.000 Network Size 0.297 0.253 0.007 4.31 0.000 Media Commitment Frequency 0.251 0.057 0.027 0.95 0.343 0.18 0.21 0.051 3.52 0.000 Reflected Appraisal Self Appraisal 0.466 0.317 0.168 2.6 0.009 0.292 0.224 0.146 1.87 0.050 0.37 0.329 0.153 2.42 0.015 0.077 0.077 0.128 0.6 0.549 0.17 0.223 0.056 3.04 0.002 Self Appraisal Identity Salience Market Mavenism Reflected Appraisal Self Appraisal Identity Salience Note: 0.00 indicates p<0. 0001 68 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Table 7: Factor Loadings Parameter Estimates Constructs and Indicators Standardized Unstandardized Standard Error Reflected Appraisal α 0.903 Item 1 0.732 1 constrained Item 2 0.801 1.001 0.071 Item 3 0.769 0.963 0.072 Item 4 0.876 1.256 0.081 Item 5 0.833 1.244 0.085 Self Appraisal 0.923 Item 1 0.796 1 constrained Item 2 0.789 0.98 0.06 Item 3 0.827 1.007 0.061 Item 4 0.909 1.24 0.066 Item 5 0.844 1.181 0.069 Identity Salience 0.848 Item 1 0.794 1 constrained Item 2 Item 3 0.741 0.88 0.046 0.715 0.996 0.095 Item 4 0.715 0.909 0.895 Market Mavenism 0.864 Item 1 0.737 1 constrained Item 2 0.75 1.021 0.065 Item 3 0.755 1.106 0.11 Item 4 0.651 0.938 0.104 Item 5 0.669 0.924 0.101 69 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Table 8: Fit Statistics X2 (187) p<0.0001 RMSEA 90% CI, lower bound upper bound 381.845 0.058 0.050 0.067 Baseline comparison Comparative Fit Index Tucker-Lewis Index 0.957 0.947 70 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Figure 7: SEM Results 71 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Discussion and Limitations This work builds on Laverie et al. (2002)’s investigation of identity salience, appraisal, and media commitment with a new context of social media users and their propensity to make product recommendations on social media sites on which they participate. This research also extends the market maven literature from its initial incarnation of a limited word of mouth spreader in the Feick & Price (1987) conceptualization to propensity to share in social media to an audience beyond a small circle of friends to potentially thousands of acquaintances. Social media users in this study had a range of friends and acquaintances from 35 to over 4,000. Product information and recommendations shared to online audiences this size eclipses the potential reach from the initial impact of the initial conceptualization of the market maven. Further, with nearly 90% of respondents visiting Facebook at least once a day or more, the opportunities to share product information recommendations and to be exposed to social media recommendations are myriad. This research ties these personal social media usage factors using measures adapted from existing measures tested in the literature with constructs related to identity theory, and ultimately, the marketing maven; together, these constructs represent a social media participant’s propensity to share product experience and consumption feedback in an online social network setting, bringing the established maven concept and updating it for the new digitally and socially-connected consumer. While media commitment, as measured as an index of the number of social media sites on which a participant holds membership did not show significance, insight into this audience (and their digital footprint) was gained. Participants on 72 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 average were members of five social media communities, with very little variance, which may differ in future research extended to different age ranges. Additional research may yield further clues as to the validity in measuring media commitment in an online setting for identity theory research, and a different range of ages may result in greater variance in media commitment compared to the heavy social media users represented by the university student sample leveraged here. Further, while the hypothesis related to reflected appraisal and propensity to share online was corroborated and the hypothesis related to self-appraisal and propensity to share did not show support, these results provide insight as well. I intuit that from an identity theory perspective, the perception of role-identities as a participant perceives others hold greatly informs their propensity to share online. Additionally, appealing to a social media participant’s self-appraisal may have little impact in stimulating sharing of product information based on the present findings. In a world where average social media participant visits at least one social media site more than once day, these connected consumers receive constant feedback and validation. Content posted may receive validation through the forms of likes, shares, and comments, and the importance of reflected appraisal makes intuitive sense. Limitations on the present research include the potential influence of one social network having differential contribution to appraisal versus all social media usage in aggregate. Additionally, a sample that has a greater variety of age ranges may allow for media commitment to have greater variance and corroborate the hypothesized relationships with self and reflected appraisal. Future replications of this research may show higher media commitment reports as well, as new online social media websites 73 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 launch regularly and are added to a social media participant’s regularly visited site. This, in turn, may affect propensity to share, as spreading the attention of a consumer across more networks may affect overall sharing (or how content is shared). Finally, this sample focuses on social media users in the United States. Multinational research may yield different results, as select countries have limited social media sites available and restricted use of these sites. For instance, a user in China may not have the same propensity to share based on cultural norms or restrictions enacted by the government, and the present research does not encompass these advanced scenarios. Accordingly, the present research suggests a number of additional areas for future research. First, replication of the current research with different age ranges can provide insights into generational differences. Much focus has been given to the millennial consumer, and a comparison with other generations may yield useful theoretical insights. Additionally, an extension of the current research by type of social media site frequented by users could provide value. For example, some social media sites are visual in nature (such as Pinterest and Instagram), where others focus on a mix of visual content with both text and images (Facebook and Twitter). A study looking at propensity to share by type of social media content type could yield deeper insights into identity salience and propensity to share. Finally, a qualitative look at what social media participants share (types of products, specific brands, high or low involvement products) and how they share (social media status, location-sharing, photography of products, sharing social media updates posted by firms, and usergenerated reviews) may provide additional layers to this research. 74 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Marketing practice will benefit from a new framework for potential to share online, as executed by the “social media maven.” By understanding the media consumption factors and how avid social media users view themselves, more effective social media messaging strategies can be crafted. If reflected appraisal influences propensity to share more than a participant’s self views, then practitioners may seek to test messaging and content that reinforces the reflected view of the social media maven role-identity. Further, as word of mouth and authentic consumer endorsements continue to hold value for firms in sales and other key metrics, understanding the psychology of the emergent social media maven holds merit in further investigation. With this research, I sought to extend the marketing maven concept into the twenty first century model of the social media maven, who has a large extended network with whom to share product experiences, recommendations, and images. Additionally, I sought to apply identity theory’s concept of salient role-identities to the influential online consumer who participates regularly in social media. By measuring social media breadth (through frequency, combined network size, and media commitment), Laverie et al. (2002)’s work was extended from traditional media to digital media as it relates to self- and reflected appraisals. While media commitment did not support the a priori hypotheses related to appraisals, this disconfirmation provided interesting future directions for research. Also, understanding the antecedents of propensity to share product information on social media sites provides new insights into the salient identities of social media users. 75 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 CHAPTER 4 Conclusion Together, the two essays that comprise this research contribute to the overall understanding of the phenomenon of the social media user in contemporary marketing. Social media is a complicated, evolving phenomenon that includes both complex visual stimuli (with text, images and videos) interpersonal influences from friends, acquaintances, strangers, and brands. Browsing most social media sites adds to the complexity and sheer number of brand messages viewed by consumers each day. These take the form of digital advertisements, posts originated by brands, posts originated by consumers, social check-ins, etc. Each of these distinct elements competes for participants’ limited attention, and different influences and psychological processes are at play. Mere exposure influence mimics the casual scrolling of a social media participant where exposure to a message is very limited in duration with limited time to process the message; rare is the occasion when consumers will pause and thoughtfully process a brand message. Mere exposure has been shown to influence brand choice, which is a key outcome brands desire. Further, consumers trust word of mouth of friends and strangers, as evidenced by the numerous travel and dining review sites, along with product ratings and reviews on ecommerce sites. These recommendations also exist in social media where participants share their product opinions through sharing content such as photos, text, location, video, and content sourced on the Internet. Social media participants who view this shared content make judgments on the worthiness of the content and message through evaluating in-group and out-group status of the source. In other words, participants judge the likeness of 76 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 the originator of the content. These reference group influences have been shown to influence product choice in mere exposure settings and purchase intention as measured from social media sites. To date, little research has been done on these complex influences on consumer behavior based on social media participation, and the present research sought to investigate these influences. This research tests the impact of mere exposure, reference group, and source of social media content (whether brand or consumer) as part of the first study. It extends the work set in offline environments of Ferraro et al. (2009) and corroborates their findings in a more visually complex social media setting. It also corroborates the findings of Naylor et al. (2012) on the influence of reference group on social media brand pages. The present research extends the works cited and adds in the complexity of various brand message formats (ads versus brand-originated stories), source of message (brand-originated stories versus consumer-generated stories), and reference group (in group versus out group) in the visually crowded environment of a social media site. Together, the studies in Essay 1 provide new knowledge on differences in brand choice in mere exposure settings for high-involvement and low involvement brands. Surprisingly, it also indicates that in the tested conditions, traditional rectangular ad units perform better in brand choice than brand-originated stories. It reinforces the value of word of mouth in high involvement products, as consumer-generated content performed better at influencing brand choice than brand-generated content. An additional insight gained from the present research is that low involvement products did not seem to exhibit a difference 77 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 between brand executions tested in this research on brand choice. This stimulates additional research questions for future studies. As consumer generated content in the form of word of mouth demonstrated greater influence on brand outcomes than similar brand messaging, additional research on online influencers holds value. Essay 2 aimed to further uncover the psychological processes at play with these diffusers of product information, and the research examines the concept of the market maven in context of a connected, frequent social media user and their propensity to share word of mouth. This extends the work of Studies 3 & 4 from Essay 1 and provides insight as to what influences propensity to share product information to an extended network on social media sites. In a surveydesigned study, this research examined social media breadth (as measured by the number of social media sites on which participants had joined, network size across all of those networks, and frequency of participation on all social media sites). It tied these usage factors to identity theory and the concept of role-identities, as measured by reflected appraisal, self-appraisal, and identity salience. In turn, each of these identity constructs was examined in relation to the propensity to share word of mouth product information (as measured by the market maven construct items). This research provides two key advances in theory. First, it is the first examination to date tying identity theory to social media usage and propensity to share product information online. It considers Feick & Price (1987)’s view of the market maven as a role-identity where product information is shared with a network. The second contribution is that it re-examines the market maven in the context of contemporary connected consumers. When the initial concept of the market maven 78 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 was explored, word of mouth was limited to traditional means (in-person, telephone, written) to a limited audience. Today, social media participants cultivate large networks ranging from close ties to strangers, and each of these audience members is a potential recipient of word of mouth recommendations. As McQuarrie et al. (2013) argue, the content of today’s connected consumer can reach thousands across multiple messaging platforms. The findings of this research indicate that reflected appraisal, or how participants assume others view them, directly influences propensity to share word of mouth, while self-appraisal only affected propensity to share as mediated by identity salience. This corroborates the underlying assumptions of the research questions that these new social media mavens spread word of mouth, and that complex psychological influences inform these sharing behaviors. As knowledge increases related to the psychological factors influencing consumer behavior in social media settings, brand social media marketing will cease to be considered new media and become an established strategy in the marketing practitioner’s IMC toolkit. By establishing the influence on brand choice in high involvement, mere exposure settings, it is implied that brands may see positive outcomes by simply participating and advertising in social media. Extending this study, the impact of consumer word of mouth was shown to perform better than brandoriginated posts in high involvement settings. The psychological antecedents of why this happens were explored through an identity-theory based examination of social media participants who embrace the social media maven role-identity. This provides a number of theoretically rich avenues for future investigation, which will benefit the academy’s understanding of this phenomenon. This additional research suggested by 79 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 these two research efforts will benefit practitioners as well by providing tangible insights on why brands may benefit from participating with consumer on social media, instead of just relying on the adoption statistics to dictate strategy. As brands understand what psychological processes influence consumers in social media and in sharing word of mouth, more skillful marketing with new success measures will be a likely outcome. 80 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 BIBLIOGRAPHY Abratt, Russell, Deon Nel, and Christo Nezer (1995), “Role of the market maven in retailing: A general marketplace influencer,” Journal of Business and Psychology, 10(1), 31–55. Anderson, James C., and David W. Gerbing (1988), “Structural Equation Modeling in Practice: A Review and Recom-mended Two-Step Approach,” Psychological Bulletin, 103(May), 411–23. Arnett, Dennis B., Steve D. German, and Shelby D. Hunt (2003), “The identity salience model of relationship marketing success: the case of nonprofit marketing,” Journal of Marketing, 67(April), 89–105. Badrinarayanan, Vishag, and Debra A. Laverie (2011), “Brand Advocacy and Sales Effort by Retail Salespeople: Antecedents and Influence of Identification with Manufacturers’ Brands,” Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 31(2), 123–40. Barnes, Stuart J, and Andrew D Pressey (2012), “In Search of the ‘Meta-Maven’: An Examination of Market Maven Behavior across Real-Life , Web , and Virtual World Marketing Channels,” Psychology &Marketing, 29, 167–85. Bearden, William O, Michael J Etzel, and J Etzel (1982), “Reference Group Influence on Product and Brand Purchase Decisions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 9(2), 183–94. Becker-Olsen, Karen L (2003), “And Now, A Word From Our Sponsor: A Look at the Effects of Sponsored Content and Banner Advertising,” Journal of Advertising, 32, 17–32. Belk, Russell W (2013), “Extended Self in a Digital World,” Journal of Consumer Research, 40(October), 477–501. Benjamin, Daniel J, James J Choi, and A. Joshua Strickland (2010), “Social Identity and Preferences,” American Economic Review, 100, 1913–28. Berger, Jonah, and Eric M Schwartz (2011), “What Drives Immediate and Ongoing Word of Mouth?,” Journal of Marketing Research, …, 48, 1–48. Bergman, Shawn M., Matthew E. Fearrington, Shaun W. Davenport, and Jacqueline Z. Bergman (2011), “Millennials, narcissism, and social networking: What narcissists do on social networking sites and why,” Personality and Individual Differences, 50(5), 706–11. 81 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Bloch, Peter H., and Marsha L. Richins (1983), “A Theoretical Model for the Study of Product Importance Perceptions,” Journal of Marketing, 47, 69–81. Bornstein, Robert F. (1989), “Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968-1987.,” Psychological Bulletin, 106, 265–89. Brown, Jo, Amanda Broderick, and Nick Lee (2007), “Word of Mouth Communication Within Online Communities: Conceptualizing the Online Social Network,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 21(3), 2–20. Calder, Bobby J., Edward C. Malthouse, and Ute Schaedel (2009), “An Experimental Study of the Relationship between Online Engagement and Advertising Effectiveness,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 23(4), 321–31. Callero, Peter L. (1985), “Role-Identity Salience,” Social, 48(3), 203–15. Chattaraman, Veena, Nancy A. Rudd, and Sharron J. Lennon (2009), “Identity salience and shifts in product preferences of Hispanic consumers: Cultural relevance of product attributes as a moderator,” Journal of Business Research, 62, 826–33. Chen, Gina Masullo (2011), “Tweet this: A uses and gratifications perspective on how active Twitter use gratifies a need to connect with others,” Computers in Human Behavior, 27(2), 755–62. Cho, Chang-Hoan (2003), “Factors influencing clicking of banner ads on the WWW.,” Cyberpsychology & behavior : the impact of the Internet, multimedia and virtual reality on behavior and society, 6, 201–15. Cho, Chang-hoan, JG Lee, and M Tharp (2001), “Different forced-exposure levels to banner advertisements,” Jounal of Advertising Research of Advertising …, (August), 45–56. Clark, Ronald A., and Ronald E. Goldsmith (2005), “Market mavens: Psychological influences,” Psychology and Marketing, 22, 289–312. Coca-Cola (2014), “Coca-Cola,” Facebook.com, <www.facebook.com/cocacola> (Mar. 4, 2014). Daboll, Peter (2011), “Celebriities in advertising are almost big waste of money,” Ad Age. Danaher, Peter J., and Guy W. Mullarkey (2003), “Factors Affecting Online Advertising Recall: A Study of Students,” Journal of Advertising Research, 43(03), 252–67. 82 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Day, GS (2011), “Closing the Marketing Capabilities Gap,” Journal of Marketing, 75(July), 183–95. Duan, Wenjing, Bin Gu, and Andrew B. Whinston (2008a), “The dynamics of online word-of-mouth and product sales—An empirical investigation of the movie industry,” Journal of Retailing, 84(2), 233–42. ——— (2008b), “Do online reviews matter? — An empirical investigation of panel data,” Decision Support Systems, 45(4), 1007–16. Duggan, Maeve, and Aaron Smith (2013), “Social Networking Fact Sheet | Pew Research Center’s Internet & American Life Project,” Pew Research Center’s Internet & American Life Project, <http://www.pewinternet.org/factsheets/social-networking-fact-sheet/> (Mar. 22, 2014). Elliott, Nate (2014), “Instagram Is The King Of Social Engagement | Forrester Blogs,” Forrester.com, <http://blogs.forrester.com/nate_elliott/14-04-29instagram_is_the_king_of_social_engagement> (May. 13, 2014). Ellison, Nicole B., Charles Steinfield, and Cliff Lampe (2007), “The Benefits of Facebook ‘Friends:’ Social Capital and College Students’ Use of Online Social Network Sites,” Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12(4), 1143–68. Engel, J.F., Roger D Blackwell, and P.W. Miniard (n.d.), Consumer Behavior, Fort Wroth: Dryden Press. Facebook (2014), “Ads Product Guide Overview | Facebook Help Center,” Facebook Help Center, <https://www.facebook.com/help/458369380926902/> (Apr. 12, 2014). Facebook.com (2014a), “Facebook Press Room Statistics,” Facebook.com, <http://www.facebook.com/press/info.php?statistics>. ——— (2014b), “Products | Facebook Newsroom,” Facebook Newsroom, <http://newsroom.fb.com/products/> (Apr. 12, 2014). ——— (2014c), “News Feed FYI: Showing Stories About Topics You Like | Facebook Newsroom,” Facebook Newsroom, <http://newsroom.fb.com/news/2014/02/news-feed-fyi-showing-stories-abouttopics-you-like/> (Apr. 12, 2014). ——— (2014d), “Facebook Creative Specs (as of March 18, 2014).” 83 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 ——— (2014e), “What’s a Like button? | Facebook Help Center,” Desktop Help, <https://www.facebook.com/help/276361262451038?sr=14&sid=0wTESqTIG5X zAo887> (Apr. 12, 2014). Fang, Xiang, Surendra Singh, and Rohini Ahluwalia (2007), “An Examination of Different Explanations for the Mere Exposure Effect,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34(June), 97–103. Feick, Lawrence F., and Linda L. Price (1987), “The Market Maven: A Diffuser of Marketplace Information,” Journal of Marketing, 51(1), 83–97. Felson, Richard B. (1980), “Communication Barriers and the Reflected Appraisal Process,” Social Psychology Quarterly, 43, 223. ——— (1985), “Reflected Appraisal and the Development of Self,” Social Psychology Quarterly, 48, 71. ——— (1989), “Parents and the reflected appraisal process: A longitudinal analysis.,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Ferraro, Rosellina, JR Bettman, and Tanya L. Chartrand (2009), “The Power of Strangers: The Effect of Incidental Consumer Brand Encounters on Brand Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35(5), 729–41. Garretson, JA, and Scot Burton (2005), “The Role of Spokescharacters as Advertisement and Package Cues in Integrated Marketing Communications,” Journal of Marketing, 69(October), 118–32. Gentile, Brittany, Jean M. Twenge, Elise C. Freeman, and W. Keith Campbell (2012), “The effect of social networking websites on positive self-views: An experimental investigation,” Computers in Human Behavior, 28(5), 1929–33. Goldsmith, Ronald E, Leisa Reinecke Flynn, and Elizabeth B Goldsmith (2003), “Innovative consumers and market mavens,” Journal of Marketing Theory & Practice, 11, 54–64. Goldsmith, Ronald E., Ronald A. Clark, and Elizabeth B. Goldsmith (2006), “Extending the psychological profile of market mavenism,” Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 5, 411–19. Graham, Jeffrey, and William Havlena (2007), “Finding the ‘Missing Link’: Advertising’s Impact on Word of Mouth, Web Searches, and Site Visits,” Journal of Advertising Research, 47(4), 427. 84 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Hall, Jeffrey a., and Natalie Pennington (2013), “Self-monitoring, honesty, and cue use on Facebook: The relationship with user extraversion and conscientiousness,” Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1556–64. He, Stephen X., and Samuel D. Bond (2013), “Word-of-mouth and the forecasting of consumption enjoyment,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23, 464–82. Hennig-Thurau, Thorsten, Kevin P. Gwinner, Gianfranco Walsh, and Dwayne D. Gremler (2004), “Electronic word-of-mouth via consumer-opinion platforms: What motivates consumers to articulate themselves on the Internet?,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 18, 38–52. Hogan, B. (2010), “The Presentation of Self in the Age of Social Media: Distinguishing Performances and Exhibitions Online,” Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 30(6), 377–86. Hogg, Michael A., Deborah J. Terry, and Katherine M. White (1995), “A Tale of Two Theories: A Critical Comparison of Identity Theory with Social Identity Theory,” Social Psychology Quarterly, 58, 255. Hollenbeck, Candice R., and Andrew M. Kaikati (2012), “Consumers’ use of brands to reflect their actual and ideal selves on Facebook,” International Journal of Research in Marketing, 29(4), 395–405. Houston, M, and M Rothschild (1978), “Conceptual and methodological perspectives on involvement,” in Research frontiers in marketing: Dialogues and directions, 184–87. Hsieh, Yu Chen, Kuo Hsiang Chen, and Min Yuan Ma (2012), “Retain viewer’s attention on banner ad by manipulating information type of the content,” Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1692–99. Hsieh, Yu-Chen, and Kuo-Hsiang Chen (2011), “How different information types affect viewer’s attention on internet advertising,” Computers in Human Behavior, 27(2), 935–45. Hunt, Shelby D., Dennis B. Arnett, and Sreedhar Madhavaram (2006), “The explanatory foundations of relationship marketing theory,” Journal of Business & Industrial Marketing, 21(2), 72–87. Janiszewski, Chris (1993), “Preattentive Mere Exposure Effects,” Journal of Consumer Research, 20(December), 376–92. 85 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Jansen, Bernard J., Mimi Zhang, Kate Sobel, and Abdur Chowdury (2009), “Twitter Power: Tweets as Electronic Word of Mouth,” Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 60, 2169–88. Jones, Scott A., K. Damon Aiken, and David M. Boush (2009), “Integrating Experience, Advertising, and Electronic Word of Mouth,” Journal of Internet Commerce, 8, 246–67. Junco, R., G. Heiberger, and E. Loken (2011), “The effect of Twitter on college student engagement and grades,” Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 27(2), 119–32. Junco, Reynol (2012), “The relationship between frequency of Facebook use, participation in Facebook activities, and student engagement,” Computers and Education, 58, 162–71. Karremans, Johan C., Wolfgang Stroebe, and Jasper Claus (2006), “Beyond Vicary’s fantasies: The impact of subliminal priming and brand choice,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 792–98. Keller, Ed (2007), “Unleashing the Power of Word of Mouth: Creating Brand Advocacy to Drive Growth,” Journal of Advertising Research, 47, 448. Kleine, Robert E, Susan S Kleine, and Jerome B Kernan (1993), “Mundane Consumption and the Self: A Social-Identity Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2(3), 209–35. Kotler, Philip (2011), “Reinventing Marketing to Manage the Environmental Imperative,” Journal of Marketing, 75(July), 132–35. Kozinets, Robert V, Kristine De Valck, Andrea C Wojnicki, and Sarah J S Wilner (2010), “Networked Narratives : Understanding Word-of-Mouth,” Journal of Marketing, 74(March), 71–89. Laverie, Debra A, and Dennis B Arnett (2000), “Factors affecting fan attendance: The influence of identity salience and satisfaction,” Journal of Leisure Research, 32, 225–46. Laverie, Debra A, Robert E Kleine, and Susan Schultz Kleine (2002), “Reexamination and Extension of Kleine, Kleine, and Kernan’s Social Identity Model of Mundane Consumption: the Mediating Role of Appraisal Process,” Journal of Consumer Research, 28(March), 659–69. 86 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Lee, Jong-Eun Roselyn, David Clark Moore, Eun-A Park, and Sung Gwan Park (2012), “Who wants to be ‘friend-rich’? Social compensatory friending on Facebook and the moderating role of public self-consciousness,” Computers in Human Behavior, 28(3), 1036–43. Lee, Seung Hwan (Mark), June Cotte, and Theodore J. Noseworthy (2010), “The role of network centrality in the flow of consumer influence,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20, 66–77. Lim, Boon C., and C. M Y Chung (2011), “The impact of word-of-mouth communication on attribute evaluation,” Journal of Business Research, 64, 18– 23. Liu, Yong (2006), “Word of Mouth for Movies: Its Dynamics and Impact on Box Office,” Journal of Marketing, 70(July), 74–89. Livingstone, S. (2008), “Taking risky opportunities in youthful content creation: teenagers’ use of social networking sites for intimacy, privacy and selfexpression,” New Media & Society, 10(3), 393–411. Lovett, Mitchell J., Renana Peres, and Ron Shachar (2013), “On Brands and Word of Mouth,” Journal of Marketing Research, 50, 427–44. Madden, Mary (2013), “Teens Haven’t Abandoned Facebook (Yet) | Pew Research Center's Internet & American Life Project,” Pew Research Center’s Internet & American Life Project, <http://www.pewinternet.org/2013/08/15/teens-haventabandoned-facebook-yet/> (Mar. 22, 2014). Martin, William C., and Jason E. Lueg (2013), “Modeling word-of-mouth usage,” Journal of Business Research, 66, 801–8. McDonald’s (2014), “McDonald’s,” Facebook.com, <https://www.facebook.com/McDonalds> (Mar. 4, 2014). McQuarrie, Edward F, Jessica Miller, and Barbara J. Phillips (2013), “The Megaphone Effect: Taste and Audience in Fashion Blogging,” Journal of Consumer Research, 40(1), 136–58. Mehdizadeh, Soraya (2010), “Self-presentation 2.0: narcissism and self-esteem on Facebook.,” Cyberpsychology, behavior and social networking, 13(4), 357–64. Milkie, Melissa A. (1999), “Social Comparisons, Reflected Appraisals, and Mass Media: The Impact of Pervasive Beauty Images on Black and White Girls’ SelfConcepts,” Social Psychology Quarterly, 62, 190. 87 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Mollen, Anne, and Hugh Wilson (2010), “Engagement, telepresence and interactivity in online consumer experience: Reconciling scholastic and managerial perspectives,” Journal of Business Research, 63(9-10), 919–25. Moore, Kelly, and James C. McElroy (2012), “The influence of personality on Facebook usage, wall postings, and regret,” Computers in Human Behavior, 28(1), 267–74. Moorman, Christine, Gerald Zaltman, and Rohit Deshpande (1992), “Relationships Between Providers and Users of Market Research: The Dynamics of Trust Within and Between Organizations,” Journal of Marketing Research, 29, 314–28. Morgan, Robert M., and Shelby D. Hunt (1994), “The Commitment-Trust Theory of Relationship Marketing,” Journal of Marketing, 58(3), 20–38. Muñiz Jr, AM, and Thomas C O’Guinn (2001), “Brand community,” Journal of Consumer research, 27(March), 412–32. Nadkarni, Ashwini, and Stefan G. Hofmann (2012), “Why Do People Use Facebook?,” Personality and individual differences, 52(3), 243–49. Naik, PA, and Kalyan Raman (2003), “Understanding the Impact of Synergy in Multimedia Communications,” Journal of Marketing Research, XL(November), 375–88. Naylor, R, C Lamberton, and PM West (2012), “Beyond the ‘Like’ Button: The Impact of Mere Virtual Presence on Brand Evaluations and Purchase Intentions in Social Media Settings,” Journal of Marketing, 76(November), 105–20. Ong, Eileen Y.L., Rebecca P. Ang, Jim C.M. Ho, Joylynn C.Y. Lim, Dion H. Goh, Chei Sian Lee, and Alton Y.K. Chua (2011), “Narcissism, extraversion and adolescents’ self-presentation on Facebook,” Personality and Individual Differences, 50(2), 180–85. Owens, Justin W., Barbara S. Chaparro, and Evan M. Palmer (2011), “Text Advertising Blindness: The New Banner Blindness?,” Journal of Usability Studies, 6, 172–97. Packard, Grant, and David B. Wooten (2013), “Compensatory knowledge signaling in consumer word-of-mouth,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23(4), 434–50. Petty, Richard E, and John T Cacioppo (1986), “The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion,” Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 88 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Price, Linda L., Lawrence F. Feick, and Audrey Guskey-Federouch (1988), “Couponing behaviors of the market maven: profile of a super couponer,” Advances in Consumer Research, 15, 354–59. Reed II, Americus (2004), “Activating the Self‐ Importance of Consumer Selves: Exploring Identity Salience Effects on Judgments,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 286–95. Reed II, Americus, Mark R. Forehand, Stefano Puntoni, and Luk Warlop (2012), “Identity-based consumer behavior,” International Journal of Research in Marketing, 29, 310–21. Richins, Marsha L (1991), “Social Images Comparison of and the Idealized Advertising,” Journal of Consumer Research, 18(1), 71–83. Ryan, Tracii, and Sophia Xenos (2011), “Who uses Facebook? An investigation into the relationship between the Big Five, shyness, narcissism, loneliness, and Facebook usage,” in Computers in Human Behavior, 1658–64. Secret, and Proctor & Gamble (2014), “Secret,” Facebook.com, <https://www.facebook.com/secret>. Seidman, Gwendolyn (2013), “Self-presentation and belonging on Facebook: How personality influences social media use and motivations,” Personality and Individual Differences, 54(3), 402–7. Shamdasani, Prem N, Andrea J S Stanaland, and Juliana Tan (2001), “Location, Location, Location: Insights for Advertising Placement on the Web,” Journal of Advertising Research, 41, 7–21. Shankar, Venkatesh, and Rajeev Batra (2009), “The Growing Influence of Online Marketing Communications,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 23(4), 285–87. Sherman, Lee, and John Deighton (2001), “Banner advertising: Measuring effectiveness and optimizing placement,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 15(2), 60–64. Sheth, JN (2011), “Impact of Emerging Markets on Marketing: Rethinking Existing Perspectives and Practices,” Journal of Marketing, 75(July), 166–82. Skittles (2014), “Skittles,” Facebook.com, <https://www.facebook.com/skittles> (Mar. 4, 2014). Southwest Airlines (2014), “Southwest Airlines,” Facebook.com, <www.facebook.com/southwestairlines> (Apr. 3, 2014). 89 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Steenkamp, Jan‐ Benedict E. M., and Katrijn Gielens (2003), “Consumer and Market Drivers of the Trial Probability of New Consumer Packaged Goods,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30, 368–84. Szmigin, Isabelle, Louise Canning, and AE Reppel (2005), “Online community: enhancing the relationship marketing concept through customer bonding,” International Journal of Service Industry Management, 16(5), 480–96. Twitter.com (2014), “About Twitter, Inc. | About,” Twitter.com, <https://about.twitter.com/company>. Varadarajan, Rajan, and Manjit S. Yadav (2009), “Marketing Strategy in an InternetEnabled Environment: A Retrospective on the First Ten Years of JIM and a Prospective on the Next Ten Years,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 23(1), 11– 22. De Vries, Lisette, Sonja Gensler, and Peter S.H. Leeflang (2012), “Popularity of Brand Posts on Brand Fan Pages: An Investigation of the Effects of Social Media Marketing,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 26(2), 83–91. Wang, Kai-Yu, Eric Shih, and Laura a. Peracchio (2013), “How banner ads can be effective: investigating the influences of exposure duration and banner ad complexity,” International Journal of Advertising, 32, 121–41. Wang, Yong J, Osama J Butt, and Jie Wei (2011), “My identity is my membership: A longitudinal explanation of online brand community members’ behavioral characteristics,” Journal of Brand Management, 19, 45–56. Wangenheim, Florian v., and Tomás Bayón (2004), “The effect of word of mouth on services switching: Measurement and moderating variables,” European Journal of Marketing, 38, 1173–85. Wansink, Brian, and Michael L Ray (2010), “Advertising Strategies to Increase Usage Frequency,” Journal of Marketing, 60(1), 31–46. Wilcox, Keith, and Andrew T. Stephen (2013), “Are Close Friends the Enemy? Online Social Networks, Self-Esteem, and Self-Control,” Journal of Consumer Research, 40(1), 90–103. Wilson, Kathryn, Stephanie Fornasier, and Katherine M White (2010), “Psychological predictors of young adults’ use of social networking sites.,” Cyberpsychology, behavior and social networking, 13(2), 173–77. Wilson, R. E., S. D. Gosling, and L. T. Graham (2012), “A Review of Facebook Research in the Social Sciences,” Perspectives on Psychological Science. 90 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 Yaveroglu, Idil, and Naveen Donthu (2008), “Advertising Repetition and Placement Issues in On-Line Environments,” Journal of Advertising. Yoo, C. Y. (2007), “Implicit Memory Measures for Web Advertising Effectiveness,” Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly. Yoo, Chan Yun (2009), “Effects beyond click-through: Incidental exposure to web advertising,” Journal of Marketing Communications, 15, 227–46. Zaichkowsky, Judith L (1995), “Measuring the Involvement Construct,” Journal of Consumer Research, 12(December), 341–52. Zajonc, R.B. (2001), “Mere Exposure: A Gateway to the Subliminal,” Current Directions in Psychological Science. Zajonc, Robert B. (1968), “Attudinal Effects of Mere Exposure,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2), 1–27. Zhang, Jason Q., Georgiana Craciun, and Dongwoo Shin (2010), “When does electronic word-of-mouth matter? A study of consumer product reviews,” Journal of Business Research, 63, 1336–41. Zhao, Shanyang, Sherri Grasmuck, and Jason Martin (2008), “Identity construction on Facebook: Digital empowerment in anchored relationships,” Computers in Human Behavior, 24(5), 1816–36. 91 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 APPENDIX A: ESSAY 1 CONSTRUCT DEFINITIONS CONSTRUCT Mere Exposure Brand Choice Involvement Ad Unit (adapted from Banner Advertising) Brand & Consumer Posts CITATION DEFINITIONS Zajonc, R. B. (1968). ATTITUDINAL EFFECTS OF MERE EXPOSURE. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2), 1–27. Ferraro, R., Bettman, J., & Chartrand, T. L. (2009). The Power of Strangers: The Effect of Incidental Consumer Brand Encounters on Brand Choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(5), 729–741. Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1995). Measuring the Involvement Construct. Journal of Consumer Research, 12(December), 341–352. Bloch, P. H., & Richins, M. L. (1983). A Theoretical Model for the Study of Product Importance Perceptions. Journal of Marketing, 47, 69–81. Houston, M., & Rothschild, M. (1978). Conceptual and methodological perspectives on involvement. In Research frontiers in marketing: Dialogues and directions (pp. 184–187). Shamdasani, P. N., Stanaland, A. J. S., & Tan, J. (2001). Location, Location, Location: Insights for Advertising Placement on the Web. Journal of Advertising Research, 41, 7–21. De Vries, L., Gensler, S., & Leeflang, P. S. H. (2012). Popularity of Brand Posts on Brand Fan Pages: An Investigation of the Effects of Social Media Marketing. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 26(2), 83–91. 92 “Mere repeated exposure of the individual to a stimulus is a sufficient condition for the enhancement of his attitude toward it” (P. 1) Selection of brands influenced after incidental brand exposure (mere exposure) without conscious awareness or intent 1. Personal—inherent interests, valued, or needs that motivate one toward the object 2. Physical—characteristics of the object that cause differentiation and increase interest 3. Situational—something that temporarily increases relevance or interest toward the object. (Zaichkowsky 1995) “…Ads induce online clicking, which links the consumer to the brand’s target communication (usually the target company’s website), and ultimately on to a purchase.” P.7 Brand posts are content shared by a brand in social media, while consumer posts are generated by participants on a social media site. Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 APPENDIX B: ESSAY 1 SUMMARY OF HYPOTHESES H1 Social media users exposed to a brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those not exposed to the brand. Social media users exposed to a low involvement product’s brand sponsored stories via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those H2a exposed to ad units for the brand. Social media users exposed to a high involvement product’s brand sponsored stories via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those H2b exposed to ad units for the brand. Social media users exposed to consumer posts about a low involvement H3a product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to sponsored stories for the brand. Social media users exposed to consumer posts about a high involvement H3b product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that brand than those exposed to sponsored stories for the brand. Social media users exposed to in-group consumer posts about a low involvement product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that H4a brand than those exposed to out-group consumer posts for the brand. Social media users exposed to in-group consumer posts about a high involvement product’s brand via mere exposure are more likely to choose that H4b brand than those exposed to out-group consumer posts for the brand. 93 Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 APPENDIX C: ESSAY 1 MEASURES & CONCEPTUALIZATIONS Construct Involvement Measures or method tested To me, X is important To me, X is interesting To me, X is relevant To me, X is exciting To me, X is meaningful To me, X is appealing To me, X is fascinating To me, X is valuable To me, X is needed Drawn/Adapted from Zaichkowsky (1994) In-Group/Out-Group Represented by age (college versus middle age) Ferraro et al (2008) Ad Unit/Post Represented as right-hand brand generated ad unit versus a post in the news feed (either consumer-generated or brandgenerated) de Vries et al (2012) Unaided Recall Please list any brands you remember seeing from the social network presented to you. Danaher and Mullarkey (2003) Brand Choice I would like XYZ at this moment XYZ is something I would purchase if available If given the opportunity, I would choose XYZ 94 Karremans et al (2006) Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 APPENDIX D: ESSAY 2 CONSTRUCT DEFINITIONS CONSTRUCT CITATION DEFINITIONS Market Maven Feick, L. F., & Price, L. L. (1987). Maven: Market A Diffuser of Information Marketplace. Journal of Marketing, 51(1), 83–97. “Individuals who have information about many kinds of products, places to shop, and other facets of the market, and initiate discussions with and respond to information requests from other consumers.” (P. 1) Identity Salience Callero, P. L. (1985). Role-Identity Salience. Social, 48(3), 203–215. Selection of brands influenced after incidental brand exposure (mere exposure) without conscious awareness or intent Self-Appraisal ReflectedAppraisal Media Commitment Network Size Frequency of Use Laverie, D. A., Kleine, R. E., & Kleine, S. S. (2002). Reexamination and Extension of Kleine, Kleine, and Kernan’s Social Identity Model of Mundane Consumption: the Mediating Role of Appraisal Process. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(March), 659–669. Felson, R. B. (1985). Reflected Appraisal and the Development of Self. Social Psychology Quarterly, 48, 71. Laverie, D. A., Kleine, R. E., & Kleine, S. S. (2002). Reexamination and Extension of Kleine, Kleine, and Kernan’s Social Identity Model of Mundane Consumption: the Mediating Role of Appraisal Process. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(March), 659–669. Gentile, B., Twenge, J. M., Freeman, E. C., & Campbell, W. K. (2012). The effect of social networking websites on positive self-views: An experimental investigation. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(5), 1929–1933. Junco, R. (2012). The relationship between frequency of Facebook use, participation in Facebook activities, and student engagement. Computers and Education, 58, 162–171. 95 “Self-appraisal is a person’s independent, personal evaluation of her identity-relate actions, especially applicable to freely chosen identities” (p. 662) “We come to see us as significant others see us.”(p. 71) Conceptualized as an extensiveness dimension, media commitment is the sum of media consumed by a study participant (how many social media networks). Conceptualized as the number of “friends” on a social network site How often various activities are undertaken on social media sites (in this case Facebook). Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 APPENDIX E: ESSAY 2 SUMMARY OF HYPOTHESES H1a: H1b: H1c: H2a: H2b: H2c: H3a: H3b: H4: H5a: H5b: The network size of a social media participant is positively related to self-appraisal The frequency of social media participation of a social media participant is positively related to self-appraisal. The media commitment of a social media participant is positively related to self-appraisal. The network size of a social media participant is positively related to reflected appraisal. The frequency of social media participation of a social media participant is positively related to reflected appraisal. The media commitment of a social media participant is positively related to reflected appraisal. The reflected appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to identity salience. The self-appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to identity salience. The identity salience of a social media participant is positively related to market mavenism. The reflected appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to market mavenism. The self-appraisal of a social media participant is positively related to market mavenism. 96 Supported Supported Not Supported Supported Supported Not Supported Supported Supported Supported Supported Not Supported Texas Tech University, William F. Humphrey, Jr., May, 2015 APPENDIX F: ESSAY 2 MEASURES Construct Market Maven Media Commitment Measures or method tested I like introducing new brands and products to my friends. I like helping people by providing them with information about many kinds of products. People ask me for information about products, places to shop, or sales. If someone asked where to get the best buy on several types of products, I could tell him or her where to shop. My friends think of me as a good source of information when it comes to new products or sales. Which of the following networks do you actively use? Choices: Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, Vine, Foursquare, Snapchat, Pinterest, LinkedIn, Google+, Other Drawn/Adapted from Feick & Price (1987) Laverie et al. (2002) Network Size How many <insert social media site> friends do you have? (divided by 100). Junco (2012) Frequency of Use How often do you use an online social network such as Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, Snapchat, or Google+? Junco (2012) Reflected Appraisal How would others describe your social media influence? Laverie et al. (2002) Notable, Excellent, Spectacular, Influential, Leader, Self Appraisal How would you describe your social media influence? Laverie et al. (2002) Notable, Excellent, Spectacular, Influential, Leader, Identity Salience Using social media is an important part of who I am Using social media is dear to who I am I would feel at a loss if I couldn't visit social media sites For me, social media is more than just visiting a website. 97 Callero (1985)