The evolutionary importance of heritability Measures of heritability 1. Parent-offspring regression analysis: – Examines the similarity between parents and their offspring in terms of the traits they possess – If a trait has a genetic basis, then the trait values of offspring should be similar to the trait values of their parents: there should be a positive relationship between offspring and parent trait values 2. Selection experiment method: – Different groups of individuals are subjected to differential selection on the trait in question – If artificial selection acting on a trait results in changes in that trait value in subsequent generations, then the trait has a genetic basis From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Hirsch Maze From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Selection experiment • Over four generations of selection, found strong changes in exploratory behavior From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Parus major http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Great_Tit Variation within a population Due to: 1. Differences in genetic composition 2. Differences in environmental conditions 3. Differences in learning And 1. Variation in traits may not affect fitness 2. Frequency-dependent selection can maintain different traits in a population From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Differences in learning • Trial-and-error learning results in bees increasing rate of food delivery From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Variation may not affect fitness • Eastern screech owls show great variation in dispersal direction From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press 2.2 Modes of natural selection describe population changes • Directional selection – Individuals in a population with an extreme trait value at one end of the spectrum possess the highest fitness From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Featured Research Directional selection in juvenile ornate tree lizards • Methods: – Ornate tree lizards (Urosaurus ornatus) – Captured from wild – Tested locomotor performance of individuals on 2 m raceway – Measured body size and mass – Uniquely marked individuals and released back into wild – Recorded survival for six months From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press 2.2 Modes of natural selection describe population changes • Disruptive selection – A situation in which individuals in a population with extreme trait values on both ends of the spectrum have the highest fitness From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Featured Research Disruptive selection in spadefoot toad tadpoles • Methods: – Spadefoot toad (Spea multiplicata) • Detritivores consume detritus and small invertebrates • Carnivores consume invertebrates • Intermediates are generalists – Examined feeding efficiency – Measured survivorship From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Featured Research Disruptive selection in spadefoot toad tadpoles • Results: – Feeding performance and growth rates of each specialized morph were higher than those of intermediate morphs From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Featured Research Disruptive selection in spadefoot toad tadpoles • Results: – Intermediate morphs had lower survivorship than either specialized morph • Conclusion: – Individuals with specialized morphology have higher fitness than individuals with intermediate morphology From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press 2.2 Modes of natural selection describe population changes • Stabilizing selection – A situation in which individuals in a population with intermediate trait values have the highest fitness From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Figure 11-15 Featured Research Sexual selection in house finches • Methods: – House finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) – Experiment 1 • Quantified male plumage color intensity • Allowed females to choose among males – Experiment 2 • Artificially dyed males • Allowed females to choose among males From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Featured Research Sexual selection in house finches • Results: – Females spent the most time with males with the most intense red coloration • Conclusion: – Red is a sexually selected trait From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press Fitness and adaptation • Adaptation – A trait that enhances fitness – An evolutionary process that results in a population of individuals with traits best suited to the current environment • Fitness – Survivorship and reproduction From Nordell and Valone, Animal Behavior: Concepts, Methods, and Applications, © 2014 by Oxford University Press “An adaptation is, thus, a feature of the organism, which interacts operationally with some factor of its environment so that the individual survives and reproduces.” (Bock, 1979) "The sutures in the skulls of young mammals have been advanced as a beautiful adaptation for aiding parturition, and no doubt they facilitate, or may be indispensable for this act; but as sutures occur in the skulls of young birds and reptiles, which have only to escape from a broken egg, we may infer that this structure has arisen from the laws of growth, and has been taken advantage of in the parturition of the higher animals." (Darwin, 1859) "The ground rule -- or perhaps doctrine would be a better term -- is that adaptation is a special and onerous concept that should be used only where it is really necessary . . . A frequent practice is to recognize adaptation in any recognizable benefit arising from the activities of an organism. I believe that this is an insufficient basis for postulating adaptation and that is has led to some serious errors. A benefit can be the result of chance instead of design. The decision as to the purpose of a mechanism must be based on an examination of the machinery and an argument as to the appropriateness of the means to the end. It cannot be based on value judgments of actual or probable consequences." " Consider a fox on its way to the hen house for the first time after a heavy snowfall. It will probably encounter considerable difficulty in forcing its way through the obstructing material. One subsequent trips, however, it may follow the same path and have a much easier time of it, because of the furrow it made the first time. This formation of a path through the snow may result in a considerable saving of time and food energy for the fox, and such savings may be crucial for survival. Should we therefore regard the paws of a fox as a mechanism for constructing paths through the snow? Clearly we should not. It is better, because it avoids the onerous biological principles of adaptation and natural selection, to regard the trail-blazing as an incidental effect of the locomotor machinery, no matter how beneficial it may be. An examination of the legs and feet of the fox forces the conclusion that they are designed for running and walking, not for the packing or removal of snow. At any rate, the concept of design for snow removal would not explain anything in the fox's appendages that is not as well or better explained by design for locomotion. Although the construction of a path through the snow should not be considered a function of the activities that have this effect, the fox does adaptively exploit the effect by seeking the same path on successive trips to the hen house. The sensory mechanisms by which it perceives the most familiar and leas obstructed routes and the motivation to follow the path of least effort are clearly adaptations." (Williams, 1966) “We wish to question a deeply engrained habit of thinking among students of evolution. We call it the adaptationist program, or the Panglossian paradigm. It is rooted in a notion popularized by A.R. Wallace and A. Weismann, (but not, as we shall see, by Darwin) toward the end of the nineteenth century: the near omnipotence of natural selection in forging organic design and fashioning the best among possible worlds. (Gould and Lewontin, 1978) "Master Pangloss taught the metaphysico-theologo-cosmolonigology. He could prove to admiration that there is no effect without a cause; and, that in this best of all possible worlds, the Baron's castle was the most magnificent of all castles, and My Lady the best of all possible baronesses. "It is demonstrable," said he, "that things cannot be otherwise than as they are; for as all things have been created for some end, they must necessarily be created for the best end. Observe, for instance, the nose is formed for spectacles, therefore we wear spectacles. The legs are visibly designed for stockings, accordingly we wear stockings. Stones were made to be hewn and to construct castles, therefore My Lord has a magnificent castle; for the greatest baron in the province ought to be the best lodged. Swine were intended to be eaten, therefore we eat pork all the year round: and they, who assert that everything is right, do not express themselves correctly; they should say that everything is best." (Voltaire’s Candide) “The study of adaptation is the study of adaptedness in an historical context. Adaptedness is a statement about the relationship between fitness and the phenotype . . . In a community context, studying the adaptedness of a particular species to its environment then requires that one (1) identify the interactions affecting fitness (i.e., survival and reproduction), (2) distinguish the phenotypic characters influencing performance in these interactions, and (3) demonstrate that altering the phenotype would lead to a decrease in fitness. These studies of adaptedness should then be place in an historical context to show that changes in the phenotype have occurred when the selective environment has changed. (McPeek, Schrot and Brown, 1996). Enallagma (Odonata: Coenagrionidae) https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ymvb4C6 1VKs Brown, McPeek and May. 2000. Syst. Biol. 49:697-712. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ymvb4C61VKs McPeek, Schrot and Brown 1996 Ecology McPeek, Schrot and Brown 1996 Ecology McPeek and Brown. 2000. Ecology 81:904-920.