Meeting the Needs of English Language Learners in Reading First Classrooms Michael C. McKenna University of Virginia Today’s Goals Examine the nature of the challenge Explore theory and research Identify effective strategies Discuss an action plan at the district, school and classroom levels Learn about Georgia’s ESOL program, regulations, and available resources Some Common Terms and Acronyms Limited English Proficiency (LEP) English-Language Learner (ELL) English as a Second Language (ESL) English as a Foreign Language (EFL) English for Speakers of Other Languages (ESOL) First (Home) Language (L1) Second Language (L2) Test of English as a Foreign Language (TOEFL) Some Common Terms and Acronyms The Plight of ELLs How many Englishlanguage learners are in Georgia schools? ? ELLs in Georgia Schools From 1993 to 2004, the number of English language learners in Georgia rose from 11,877 to 59,126 – an increase of 397.8%. Source: National Center for English Language Acquisition More Georgia Stats . . . Public school students in LEP Programs 4.3% Hispanic students 6.9% Asian/Pacific Islander 2.5% Source: NAEP, 2005 How well are ELLs learning to read? Some Georgia Stats . . . Percent of Georgia Fourth Graders Reading below the “Basic” Level 60 50 40 30 20 White Black Hispanic Asian 10 0 Source: NAEP, 2005 Are things better elsewhere? U.S. v. Georgia at Grade 4 Percent of Hispanic Fourth Graders Reading below the “Basic” Level 100 75 54 56 Georgia U. S. 50 25 0 Source: NAEP, 2005 Nevada REA Study In a Nevada REA study using PALS Composite Scores (Oral Reading in Context plus Spelling, plus Word Recognition) Percentage Below PALS Benchmark Children Grade 1 Grade 2 Grade 3 Native English Speakers 20 18 22 ELLs 56 51 57 – Helman, 2005 Nevada REA Study – Helman, 2005 What types of programs are used to meet the needs of ELLs? There are several. Four Types of ELL Programs Type Characteristics Outcome L1-only L1 only is spoken. Children learn to read in L1. (Rare in U.S.) L1 literacy develops, but spoken and written English do not Transitional Bilingual L1 is exclusively used at first, but transition to English is made as soon as possible. L1 literacy jeopardized in transition, but research favors it over English only. Two-way Bilingual Equal time to L1 and English. Half the children speak each. Parents of English speakers desire their children learn L1. (Rare in U.S.) Reading and writing in both languages develop for both groups. English-only (Immersion) Only English is spoken. Teacher provides limited support to ELLs. (Most common program type in U.S.) English slowly develops Reading growth slowed L1 withers since literacy is never attained in L1 – Tabors & Snow, 2002 Which type of ELL program is best? Let’s look at two recent research summaries. Slavin and Cheung’s 2005 Meta-analysis of 17 Studies1 Program Type Studies in Support Immersion 0 (English only) Bilingual (Both two-way and transitional were included) 1Five 14 studies reported no significant difference. Bob Slavin and friend Developing Literacy in English-Language Learners Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children May 2006 Tim Shanahan Diane August Key Findings In available studies, children in bilingual programs did at least as well as those in English-only programs. Overall, a moderate effect size was observed across studies, favoring bilingual instruction. Moreover, ELLs maintained their first language to a greater extent. Some Questions We Can’t Yet Answer If we teach L1 reading and writing, how soon is it safe to transition to English? Does teaching higher levels of L1 reading make learning to read English any quicker? How proficient must an ELL become in spoken English before reading instruction should begin? How long can reading instruction in English be delayed for ELLs before their reading development is jeopardized? – Tabors & Snow, 2002 Key Findings Learning English is easier for ELL children aged 89 with literacy skills in L1 than for children aged 5-6 without L1 literacy skills. Immersion programs are not significantly better than programs in which L1 is used. Bilingual children outperform monolingual children on metalinguistic tasks through age 6. Oral proficiency in L2 is not a strong predictor of reading proficiency in L2 (due to other factors). (Garcia, 2005) Key Findings L2 teacher read-alouds tend to be ineffective. Prior knowledge often disadvantages ELLs. Middle and high school bilinguals tend to use the same comprehension strategies in either language. Abler bilinguals tend to “translate” more difficult material as they read, to use cognates in confronting unfamiliar words, and to code switch between sentences (i.e., reflect on meaning in L1). (Garcia, 2005) Key Findings Close captioning can help as long as a certain threshold in L2 has been attained. Culturally responsive teaching (through which teachers learn about the L2 culture and use this knowledge) can be helpful. (Garcia, 2005) Second Language Acquisition What happens to young children who are suddenly immersed in an Englishspeaking environment? What happens to young children who are suddenly immersed in an Englishspeaking environment? They go through four phases. Four Phases of Transition to Spoken English 1. ELLs use L1, expecting to be understood. – They are often not understood, however. 2. ELLs grow silent. – They realize L1 is not working for them. 3. ELLs begin using telegraphic and formulaic language. – Telegraphic Examples: Object names, counting – Formulaic Examples: Catch phrases (“Excuse me,” “I don’t know”) 4. ELLs gradually learn to use English productively. – They blend formulaic with telegraphic speech Examples: “I do a ice cream,” “I got a big” – Tabors & Snow, 2002 Two Types of Oral English Proficiency What kind is it? What can a child do? Conversational (Social) • Communicate with peers • Use gestures & body language to aid and complement language Academic • Comprehend oral instruction • Comprehend content materials How long does it take to acquire? About 2 years 5-7 years – Adapted from Drucker, 2003 Reading and Language Development of a Native Speaker Foundation of Spoken English Develops Reading Adds to the Foundation Reading Builds on This Foundation Reading and Language Development of an ELL in an English-Only Program Foundation of Spoken English Is Limited Foundation of Spoken Home Language Is Stronger Reading and Language Development of an ELL in an English-Only Program Foundation of Spoken English Is Limited Reading Must Develop together with Spoken English Foundation of Spoken Home Language Is Stronger Reading Instruction In Home Language Is Not Provided Reading and Language Development of an ELL in an English-Only Program Foundation of Spoken English Is Limited Reading Must Develop together with Spoken English Foundation of Spoken Home Language Is Stronger Reading Instruction In Home Language Is Not Provided Reading Growth Far Slower than English-speakers Home Language Spoken Proficiency Declines The Universal Grammar A “hard-wired” system for oral language Humans use this linguistic system to acquire L1 UG has 3 components (lexicon, rules, phonology) The UG may be modular in nature (i.e., the 3 components may work independently) Adapted from DeKeyser & Juffs (2005) How L1 Affects Learning L2 Learning a new word in L2 requires access to concepts stored in L1 (except for fluent bilinguals) L1 phonology influences L2 pronunciation DeKeyser & Juffs (2005) animal mammal “meow” cat 4 legs c-a-t /kat/ pet lion animal mammal “meow” gato cat 4 legs c-a-t /kat/ pet lion animal mammal “meow” gato cat 4 legs c-a-t /kat/ pet lion animal mammal “meow” gato cat 4 legs c-a-t /kat/ pet lion animal mammal mamífero “meow” gato cat 4 legs cuatro piernas animal doméstico /kat/ pet lion león c-a-t Should ELLs be taught English directly or indirectly? Explicit vs. Implicit Learning Most research supports explicit learning, but these studies are short-term. “Formulas” are words and chunks deliberately memorized – an example of explicit learning. Practice is required to make explicit knowledge more accessible and automatic. Explicit teaching can “jump start” SLA, followed by providing conditions for long-term implicit learning. DeKeyser & Juffs (2005) Explicit vs. Implicit Learning Implicit learning is more difficult for adults “because of restrictions on their implicit learning capacities” (p. 444) L2 learners may not apply explicit knowledge • unless they know the rules well, • care to apply them, and • have the time it takes to do so (Krashen’s view) Use of explicit knowledge can be automatized, but this takes time and practice and may even then not be generalized to other situations. DeKeyser & Juffs (2005) The Competition Model L1 plays a key role in acquiring and processing L2. Learner will use L1 grammar to acquire L2. Research is limited in that experimental conditions oversimplify actual language use. DeKeyser & Juffs (2005) Individual Differences Aptitude • Difficulties in defining; new measures clearly needed • But research validates the concept Age • Puberty remains a key point in SLA • But notion of a “critical period” for SLA is still debated • Usually assumed to be 6-16 years of age • May involve a shift from implicit to explicit learning • Neuroimaging confirms that L2 is represented differently DeKeyser & Juffs (2005) Individual Differences Working Memory • Two constructs: 1. Phonological short-term memory (STM) digit span, etc. 2. Reading Span Task (RST) word recall from sentences • Studies are inconclusive, perhaps due to measures DeKeyser & Juffs (2005) The Input Hypothesis The only necessary and sufficient condition for SLA is comprehensible input. Learners at stage i will move to i + 1 if and when they understand input containing i + 1. Steven Krashen (1982, 1985) But studies of speaking and writing cast doubt on the input hypothesis. (Swain, 2005) The Output Hypothesis Producing L2 in writing and/or speaking is essential to learning it. Last few decades have seen a shift for output as product to output as process. (See p. 480.) Three possible roles for output in L2 learning: 1. Noticing/Triggering Function 2. Hypothesis Testing Function 3. Metalinguistic (Reflective) Function (Swain, 2005) Noticing/Triggering Function “the activity of producing the target language may prompt second language learners to recognize consciously some of their linguistic problems” (p. 474, original emphasis) The student may “notice the gap” in their proficiency and try to fill it. (Swain, 2005) Hypothesis Testing Function Output (speaking or writing) may be a “trial run.” When a teacher or peer seeks clarification or requests confirmation, the L2 learner tends to modify the output in the future. Lesson for teachers: Push ELLs to produce correct formulations of English, rather than accepting incorrect output simply because the meaning is clear. Writing is a good opportunity to “push” for correctness. (Swain, 2005) Metalinguistic (Reflective) Function “[U]sing language to reflect on language produced by others or the self mediates second language learning.” (p. 478) This is a Vygotskyan perspective “Psychological processes emerge first in collective behavior, in cooperation with other people, and only subsequently become internalized as the individual’s own possessions” (Stetsenko & Arievitch, 1997, p. 161). Collaborative writing is especially conducive to reflecting on output. (Swain, 2005) Implications for Classroom Instruction So where do teachers start? Most cores have an ELL resource handbook and related materials. Start there. But let’s think about general advice. So where do teachers start? Let’s look at some key differences between Spanish and English. Spanish vs. English Consonants Pronounced the Same c l m n s Pronounced Differently d h j v sh r z Spanish vs. English Consonants Clusters Not Heard in Spanish st sp sk/sc sm sl sn sw tw qu scr spl spr str squ th Spanish vs. English Spanish vowels always have the same sound: English Long a Spanish e Example Pedro Long e Long i Long o i ai o sí jai-lai no Long u Short o u a usted Pablo Spanish vs. English Short vowels are hard for Spanishspeaking children because most of these phonemes do not exist in Spanish! Spanish vs. English What are the implications of these differences for acquiring (and teaching) phonemic awareness and phonics? Phonemic Awareness for Spanish-Speaking ELLs Children’s knowledge of Spanish phonology may influence how they acquire phonemic awareness in English. They may find it hard at first to distinguish phonemes not heard in Spanish (e.g., v-b, s-sp, ch-sh). Instruction in specific pairs has been shown to have positive results. National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children, 2006 Phonemic Awareness for Spanish-Speaking ELLs Phonemic awareness in Spanish translates into English. That is, children can do similar tasks (segmenting, blending, etc.). However, the specific phonemes are often different. These differences are predictable. Well-planned teaching leads to equal levels of phonemic awareness for ELLs and native English speakers. Gersten & Geva, 2005 Phonics for Spanish-Speaking ELLs Begin with sounds that English and Spanish share. Start with vowels and consonants that represent sounds that are the same as or similar to the sounds they represent in Spanish (listed in previous slides). Use your knowledge of Spanish to interpret misspellings. (Example: da might be written for the) If you’re not comfortable with Spanish, ask the child to read what s/he has written and listen for letter-sound correspondences. Helman, 2004 Phonics for Spanish-Speaking ELLs A pronunciation error may reflect knowledge of Spanish. Example: Saying seat for sit is common when the child has some reading ability in Spanish. It might also be an attempt to come as close as possible using a Spanish vowel sound. Use low-stress activities to practice pronunciations. Examples: choral reading, echo reading, sound sorting of pictures, poetry, songs Helman, 2004 Phonics for Spanish-Speaking ELLs Try using Venns and word walls to underscore similarities and differences in letter-sound correspondences. (See previous slides.) Developmental spelling inventories can provide useful information about phonics skills (e.g., the one in Words Their Way by Bear et al.). Short vowels should be taught before long vowels. Helman, 2004 Phonics for Spanish-Speaking ELLs Conduct think-alouds comparing English and Spanish. “Teachers may verbalize their thinking in a modeled writing activity as they ponder which sounds they hear in a tricky word. They may even model being confused and selfcorrecting based on a Spanish sound.” (p. 458) Helman, 2004 A good source for Spanish phonology concerns . . . Which instructional techniques are consistent with theory and research? The Output Hypothesis suggests that teachers provide many opportunities for ELLs to talk and write. Doing so also provides a window on their development of their English. A central way for teachers to assess the learning and understanding of their ELLs is to give them myriad opportunities to write and talk during lessons. When ELLs are silent during extended periods of lesson times, it is not possible to know if or how much they are learning from lessons. – Brock & Raphael, 2005, p. 51 Sheltered Instruction (SI) “Sheltered instruction is an approach for teaching content to English learners in strategic ways that make the subject matter concepts comprehensible while promoting the students’ English language development.” – Echevarria, Vogt, & Short (2004, p. 2) Sheltered Instruction (SI) Content and language objectives are interwoven. Most commonly applied at grades 4 and higher Sheltered Instruction (SI) Techniques often used in SI: Visual aids Modeling and demonstrations Graphic organizers Vocabulary overviews Predictions Cooperative learning Peer tutoring Multicultural content Modified Discussions Use frequent rewordings. Put key terms on the board as you speak. Translate key terms if you know them. Speak clearly and at a moderate pace. Use frequent review and summaries. Pronounce key terms precisely. Refer to pictures and diagrams. – McKenna & Robinson, 2005 Word Webs Connecting concepts in a web diagram places few demands on English literacy. Relationships among words are stressed. These relationships are made visible and concrete. Webs can be used with fiction or nonfiction. – Farnan, Flood & Lapp, 1994 Scaffolded Reading Experiences (SREs) What is an SRE? “a set of prereading, during-reading, and postreading activities specifically designed to assist English-language learners in successfully reading, understanding, and learning from a particular selection.” – Fitzgerald & Graves (2004, p. 15) Scaffolded Reading Experiences The best activities are often those that research has shown to be beneficial to native speakers as well. Many options are available for use before, during and after reading. You must be selective. Consider the nature of the selection and the needs of your ELLs. Possible SRE Prereading Activities Using motivational approaches Pointing out links to students’ lives Building or activating prior knowledge Let’s consider an example of how important this can be for ELLS. It was the day of the big party. Mary wondered if Johnny would like a kite. She ran to her bedroom, picked up her piggy bank, and shook it. There was no sound. – Eskey, 2002, p. 6 What prior knowledge must the reader have in order to comprehend this brief passage? Eskey, D.E. (2002). Reading and the teaching of L2 reading. TESOL Journal, 11(1), 5-9. Possible SRE Prereading Activities Using motivational approaches Pointing out links to students’ lives Building or activating prior knowledge Providing text-specific knowledge Preteaching vocabulary Preteaching concepts Prequestioning, predicting, and direction setting Suggesting strategies Using students’ native language Involving ELL communities, parents, siblings Possible SRE During-Reading Activities Silent reading Reading to students Supported reading Oral reading by students Modifying the text Possible SRE Postreading Activities Questioning Discussion Writing Drama Artistic, graphic, and nonverbal activities Application and outreach activities Building connections Reteaching An SRE builds on a long tradition of nesting a reading selection in before, during and after activities. Let’s examine which of the major lesson formats seem most promising. Before During After Major Lesson Formats Directed Reading Activity (DRA) Directed Reading-Thinking Activity (DR-TA) K-W-L Listen-Read-Discuss (L-R-D) Before During After DRA Facts Vocabulary Text structure Students read to complete tasks set by teacher Discussion Writing Before During After DRA Facts Vocabulary Text structure Students read to complete tasks set by teacher Discussion Writing Before During After DRA Facts Vocabulary Text structure Students read to complete tasks set by teacher Discussion Writing Before During After DRA Facts Vocabulary Text structure Students read to complete tasks set by teacher Discussion Writing Before During After 5 Steps in a Classic DRA 1. Background (vocabulary, facts) 2. Focus (set specific purposes) 3. Silent Reading 4. Discussion 5. Skills, Extension, Enrichment 5 Steps in a Classic DRA 1. Background (vocabulary, facts) 2. Focus (set specific purposes) 3. Silent Reading 4. Discussion 5. Skills, Extension, Enrichment Before 5 Steps in a Classic DRA 1. Background (vocabulary, facts) 2. Focus (set specific purposes) 3. Silent Reading 4. Discussion 5. Skills, Extension, Enrichment Before During 5 Steps in a Classic DRA 1. Background (vocabulary, facts) 2. Focus (set specific purposes) 3. Silent Reading 4. Discussion 5. Skills, Extension, Enrichment Before During After DR-TA Before During After DR-TA Facts Vocabulary Text structure Students read to test their own predictions Discussion Writing Before During After DR-TA Facts Vocabulary Text structure Students read to test their own predictions Discussion Writing Before During After DR-TA Facts Vocabulary Text structure Students read to test their own predictions Discussion Writing Before During After K-W-L Before During After K-W-L Students Students read to Discussion brainstorm find out what of what they what they Know they Want to know have Learned Before During After K-W-L Students Students read to Discussion brainstorm find out what of what they what they Know they Want to know have Learned Before During After K-W-L Students Students read to Discussion brainstorm find out what of what they what they Know they Want to know have Learned Before During After L-R-D Before During After L-R-D Teacher fully Students read to presents text complete tasks content set by teacher (Children might listen to Spanish version) Before During Discussion After L-R-D Teacher fully Students read to presents text complete tasks content set by teacher (Children might listen to Spanish version) Before During Discussion After L-R-D Teacher fully Students read to presents text complete tasks content set by teacher (Children might listen to Spanish version) Before During Discussion Writing After Which of these formats seem best suited to the needs of ELLs? DRA DR-TA K-W-L L-R-D Might the issue depend on the age and English proficiency of the child? Language Experience Approach (LEA) Teacher plans a group experience, such as a field trip, demonstration, etc. Students afterward dictate a passage based on the shared experience. Teacher writes as students dictate. Dictated passage becomes the basis of discussion and a reading lesson. LEA controls for prior knowledge differences, although unpredictable cultural interpretations can occur. – Drucker, 2003 Discussions in Small Groups ELLs are sometimes intimidated into silence in whole-class settings. They are more likely to talk in small groups. Schedule small-group discussions with group make-up including both ELLs and native speakers. – Brock & Raphael, 2005 Shared Reading Teacher reads aloud an enlarged text that all students can see. Students can see text as it is discussed. Teacher can point to key words, etc. Paired Reading Teacher pairs ELLs with native speakers. Students read to each other, with native speaker providing support. Could be tied to repeated readings, where native speaker reads a brief passage and ELL reads the same passage. Building Prior Knowledge Teacher tries to anticipate limitations of prior knowledge. What does the author assume the child knows and that the child may not. Look for ways to build prior knowledge quickly and coherently. – Drucker, 2003 Audio Books Teacher provides a tape of the reading selection, perhaps in a listening center. ELLs follow along as they listen. A minimal level of reading ability is required for this approach to be effective. – Drucker, 2003 Teacher Read-Alouds Read-alouds can be planned with ELLs in mind. 5 steps used by Hickman et al.: 1. Preview story and 3 new words. Give Spanish equiivalents. 2. Read the book aloud. Focus on literal and inferential comprehension. 3. Reread, focusing on the 3 words. 4. Extend comprehension, focusing on deeper understanding of words. 5. Summarize the book. – Hickman, Pollard-Durodola, & Vaughn, 2004 Multicultural Books These are likely to require less background building. They build confidence and they value the ELLs’ home culture. Such books make good read-alouds! – Drucker, 2003 Selected Internet Resources Internet TESL Journal http://iteslj.org/ its-online http://www.its-online.com/ English-to-Go http://www.english-to-go.com/ Online Translator http://www.worldlingo.com/en/products_services/ worldlingo_translator.html More Internet Resources Barahona Center http://www.csusm.edu/csb/ Georgia ESOL Program http://public.doe.k12.ga.us/ci_iap_esol.aspx Office of English Language Acquisition (OELA) http://www.ed.gov/about/offices/list/oela/index.html?src=oc National Clearinghouse for English Language Acquisition (NCELA) http://www.ncela.gwu.edu/ Forming an Action Plan What can we do at the district, school, and classroom levels to meet the needs of ELLs? LEA Schools Teachers At the District Level Start (or improve) your record keeping system Stay updated on programs http://public.doe.k12.ga.us/ci_iap_esol.aspx Coordinate PD across schools that serve ELLs Lead efforts to attract bilingual teachers Explore transitional bilingual programs Establish active links with the Latino community Recommend that parents turn on captioning At the School Level Generally, foster cultural awareness Specifically, provide PD in culturally responsive teaching Acquire bilingual and multicultural books Hire bilingual teachers and paraprofessionals Host community-building activities for Latino parents Form teacher study groups Locate and disseminate professional resources At the Classroom Level Seek the Georgia ESOL Endorsement http://www.glc.k12.ga.us/pandp/esol/certif.htm Learn to apply scientifically-based instructional approaches Form needs-based groups with English proficiency in mind Learn conversational Spanish Who me? Learn Spanish? Why not? It will not only help you meet the needs of ELLs, but it will deepen your understanding of English. As the greatest writer in German once put it . . . Johann Wolfgang von Goethe (1749-1832) Those who know nothing of foreign languages know nothing of their own. Suggested Readings Brock, C.H., & Raphael, T.E. (2005). Windows to language, literacy, and culture: Insights from an English-language learner. Newark, DE: IRA. Drucker, M.J. (2003). What reading teachers should know about ESL learners. The Reading Teacher, 57, 22-29. Echevarria, J., & Graves, A. (2003). Sheltered content instruction: Teaching English-language learners with diverse abilities (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Echevarria, J., Vogt, M., & Short, D.J. (2004). Making content comprehensible for English learners: The SIOP model (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Helman, L.A. (2004). Building on the sound system of Spanish: Insights from the alphabetic spellings of English-language learners. The Reading Teacher, 57, 452-460. Helman, L.A. (2005). Using assessment results to improve teaching for Englishlanguage learners. The Reading Teacher, 58, 668-677. Hickman, P., Pollard-Durodola, S., & Vaughn, S. (2004). Storybook reading: Improving vocabulary and comprehension for English-language learners. The Reading Teacher, 57, 720-730. Suggested Readings Shanahan, T., & August, D. (Eds.). (2006). Developing literacy in Englishlanguage learners. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Tabors, P.O., & Snow, C.E. (2002). Young bilingual children and early literacy development. In S.B. Neuman & D.K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 1, pp. 159-178). New York: Guilford.