Running head: INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS Inhibition of Return Across Eye and Object Movements: The Role of Prediction Hannah M. Krüger and Amelia R. Hunt University of Aberdeen, UK INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 2 Abstract Responses are slower to targets appearing in recently-inspected locations, an effect known as Inhibition of Return (IOR). IOR is typically viewed as the consequence of an involuntary mechanism that prevents re-inspection of previously visited locations and thereby biases attention towards novel locations during visual search. For an inhibitory tagging mechanism to serve this function effectively, it should be robust against eye movements and the movements of objects in the environment. We investigated whether the predictability of motion supports the coding of inhibitory tags in spatiotopic coordinates across eye movements and object-based coordinates across object motion. IOR was observed in both retinotopic and spatiotopic coordinates across eye movements, regardless of the predictability of the eye movement direction. In a matching experiment, but with predictable or unpredictable object motion instead of eye movements, IOR was reduced in magnitude by object motion and was not observed in object-based coordinates, even when the motion was predictable. Together the results suggest inhibitory tags can track objects as they move across the retina, but only when this motion is selfgenerated. We conclude that efference copy, not prediction, plays a key role in maintaining inhibition on previously-attended objects across saccades. Keywords: Inhibition of Return, inhibitory tagging, visual attention, eye movements INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 3 Responses to targets appearing in the same location as an uninformative cue tend to be faster than in other locations (Posner, 1980), but only when the duration between the cue and target is short. When targets appear more than 200-300ms after the cue, responses to targets in the cued location are slower than the uncued location (Posner & Cohen, 1984). This delay in target detection at longer cue-target intervals is called Inhibition of Return (IOR). IOR is a robust effect that has been reproduced hundreds of times since the seminal study by Posner and Cohen in 1984. Posner and Cohen originally postulated that IOR reflects an attentional orienting bias. That is, the exogenous cue attracts attention reflexively, and if the target appears just briefly after the cue, attention is still in place and therefore facilitates a response. Over time, however, attention retreats from the cued location and this location is then inhibited in favour of new locations. In line with the assumption that IOR indexes inhibitory tags that orient attention towards novelty, Klein (1988) proposed that inhibitory tagging can facilitate visual search. One study that has delivered strong support for such a claim is that of Klein and MacInnes (1999), who recorded eye movements while participants searched for “Waldo” (a character in a red-striped shirt and glasses) in a cluttered “Where’s Waldo”TM scene. Participants were also instructed to move their eyes as quickly as possible to any probes (black disks) appearing on the display. These probes could appear at the previously fixated location, at the penultimate fixation, or in control locations that had not yet been fixated. Saccades back to previously fixated locations were slower than saccades to locations 180 degrees away from the previous fixation, and this result has been reproduced by others (Dodd, van Stigchel, & Hollingworth, 2009; MacInnes & Klein, 2003; Smith & Henderson, 2009; 2011). This is evidence that IOR is observed in a visual search INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 4 context, consistent with the interpretation that it reflects inhibitory tags that bias attention towards novel locations during visual search. The Reference Frame of IOR For an inhibitory tagging mechanism to efficiently facilitate foraging, it must be coded in spatiotopic (i.e., environmental or object- based) coordinates. If instead it were coded in retinotopic (i.e., gaze-centred) coordinates, inhibitory tags would shift spatially with the eye movements and would impede search in unexplored locations. Supporting a spatiotopic reference frame for IOR, the aforementioned studies investigating IOR in a visual search context have found IOR not only in the previously fixated location, but also in the location fixated two saccades previously (the 2-back location). This supports the idea that IOR indexes an attentionrelated phenomenon that is coded in spatiotopic locations, rather than reflecting either a change in sensitivity to a location on the retina, or a motor bias, such as an inhibited saccade vector or a location on the retina. IOR has also been observed in spatiotopic coordinates in cue-target experiments (Abrams & Pratt, 2000; Maylor & Hockey, 1985; Posner & Cohen, 1984; van Koningsbruggen, Gabay, Sapir, Henik & Rafal, 2009). For example, Posner and Cohen (1984) investigated the reference frame of IOR by cueing a location and then instructing observers to make eye movements to a series of three locations. Subsequently, a target appeared at either the retinotopic or the spatiotopic location of the cue. Participants were slower to respond to targets appearing in the cued spatiotopic coordinates than to targets in the cued retinotopic location. However, more recent evidence suggests that IOR is coded in both frames of reference. For example, Pertzov, Zohary, and Avidan (2010), using saccades to peripheral targets as a response, find that IOR is coded in spatiotopic coordinates just briefly after an eye movement, especially for cued targets INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 5 that appear in the same hemifield as the cue. Nonetheless, they also observed a retinotopic inhibition. Similarly, Hilchey, Klein, Satel and Wang (2012) and Mathot and Theeuwes (2010) report both spatiotopic and retinotopic IOR across eye movements for saccadic responses to targets and Sapir, Hayes, Henik, Danziger, and Rafal (2004) have shown a similar pattern for manual responses to targets. Abrams and Pratt have shown that IOR is spatiotopic with manual responses to a peripheral probe target, but IOR is retinotopic for saccadic responses to a central arrow target. Together these studies suggest that IOR can be observed in both the spatiotopic and retinotopic locations of cues, and that the factors that determine in which reference frame IOR will be more robust are not yet fully understood. A similar question to that about the reference frame of IOR concerns whether IOR is object-based or location-based. To bias attention away from previously-inspected objects, which can vary in size and can change location on the retina, inhibitory tagging would need to be applied not to a limited and static location on the retina, but to whole objects. There is substantial evidence from static displays that whole objects are inhibited (Chou & Yeh, 2008; Jordan & Tipper, 1998; Leek, Reppa, & Tipper, 2003). Jordan and Tipper (1998), for example, illustrate that if apparent objects (Kanisza squares) were cued, IOR was larger in magnitude than if a cue appeared in a similar location but without apparent objects. Furthermore, Tipper, Driver and Weaver (1991) showed that when an object is cued and subsequently moved, responses to targets appearing in the cued object are slower even though the object occupies a new location in space. It was also found that responses to targets appearing in the previous location of the moved object were slower than uncued locations, suggesting inhibitory tags are both object-based and locationbased (Tipper, Weaver, Jerreat, & Burak, 1994). These findings have been challenged by Müller and von Mühlenen (1996), who conducted a study with seven experiments in all of which IOR INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 6 was largely location-based rather than object-based. Indeed, cued objects were facilitated, rather than inhibited, when cued objects followed common motion patterns, for example, from left to right (as in reading) or from top to bottom (falling with gravity). Thus, similar to the conclusions made about the reference frame of IOR across eye movements, it appears as though IOR can be applied to cued, moving objects, but this inhibition is not always applied. What circumstances lead to robust object-based IOR is an open question. Both spatiotopic IOR and object-based IOR are assumed to reflect an inhibitory tag that disengages from the retinotopic location of the cue, and moves with objects as they move on the retina. Given these similarities between them, it seems plausible that spatiotopic and objectbased IOR could reflect the same general mechanism. Indeed, if inhibitory tags were entirely object-based then they would, by definition, also be spatiotopic. However, they also differ, in that when the eyes move, the motion of objects on the retina is produced internally by the observer. This motion is therefore expected and predictable. In the case of object-based IOR, the motion of the object is generated externally, and cannot be predicted with certainty. The ability to predict the upcoming motion with certainty may play an important role in whether inhibitory tags are preserved on objects that move on the retina. However, it is also possible that there is something special about retinal motion that is generated by an eye movement, over and above its predictability. Indeed, recently it has been suggested that spatiotopic IOR may be supported by a saccade-specific mechanism known as predictive remapping (Abrams & Pratt, 2000; Mathôt & Theeuwes, 2010; Pertzov et al., 2010; van Koningsbruggen et al., 2009). Visual cells in several brain areas, including lateral interparietal cortex (LIP) and frontal eye fields (FEF) have been shown to respond to visual stimuli shortly before an executed eye movement even though these stimuli will only fall into their receptive INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 7 field after the completion of this eye movement (Duhamel, Colby, & Goldberg, 1996). Remapping has been linked to signals coming from superior colliculus (SC), a midbrain structure associated with eye movement control, suggesting remapping is based on a copy of the efferent saccadic motor command (Sommer & Wurtz, 2002; 2006). In other words, cells in some visual areas of the brain use information about upcoming eye movements to prepare for expected changes in the retinotopic map. The same mechanism may support spatiotopic inhibitory tagging across saccades. Maintaining inhibitory tags on moving objects, however, cannot rely on exactly the same mechanism, because the changes on the retina are not self-generated and therefore lack an important aspect of predictive remapping: the efference copy. If object-based IOR differs from spatiotopic IOR even when object motion is perfectly predictable, it would suggest that efference copy plays an important role in maintaining inhibitory tags across saccades. We therefore examined whether a general prediction-based mechanism could explain both spatiotopic IOR across eye movements and object-based IOR across object motion. We manipulated predictability by either blocking the direction of motion (predictable) or randomly interleaving it within each block (unpredictable). We first examined the role of predictability in maintaining IOR across saccades. In Experiment 1a we tested IOR across eye movements when the direction of the eye movement was blocked and therefore predictable. In Experiment 1b we also tested IOR across eye movements but the direction of the eye movement was mixed and therefore unpredictable. If spatiotopic inhibitory tags rely on saccadic remapping alone then IOR should be spatiotopic to a similar extent under these two conditions. Alternatively, the inhibition of attended objects in their post-saccadic coordinates may depend to some extent on foreknowledge about where these objects will appear on the retina in the future, in which case the predictability of future eye movement directions may facilitate spatiotopic IOR. INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS In Experiment 2 we investigated location-based and object-based IOR by moving the objects externally across the retina in a manner that mimicked the saccadic displacement of Experiment 1. Again we manipulated predictability across object displacement, by running a predictable and an unpredictable displacement condition. If predictability of object motion is a crucial factor in object-based IOR then IOR should be object-based with predictable motion but remain location-based with unpredictable motion. Experiment 1a: Saccades with the direction blocked Method Participants Twenty-eight participants (19 female, average age 22.25) participated in this study. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision. Some participants participated for course credit (11), the rest volunteered. Apparatus An Eyelink 1000 (SR Research, Mississauga, Canada) was used for video based eye tracking with a sample rate of 1000Hz. Only the right eye was monitored. The camera was placed in the desktop mount, with a chin and forehead rest at a viewing distance of 50cm, in front of an 85Hz 17inch CRT monitor with a resolution of 1024x768. The experiment was programmed and run using Experiment Builder (SR Research) on a Macintosh Pro running Windows XP. Stimuli and procedure Throughout this experiment and the following ones, the participant's primary task was to perform a manual detection, that is, to press a specified button on a game controller as soon as 8 INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 9 the target stimulus (+) appeared anywhere on the screen. Participants were instructed to hold the game controller with both hands and use the right thumb to press the button. Each experiment and subsequently each block started with a nine-point calibration sequence. In some cases the nine-point calibration was substituted with a five-point calibration (only for participants with corrected vision). Each trial began with a drift correction: Participants were instructed to fixate a dot in the middle of a blank screen and press a specified button with their left thumb. If fixation was stable the trial would begin. Four boxes (outlined squares) 1.3˚ in size were presented at each corner of an invisible square, 3.4˚ in length/height (see Figure 1). The background was white. The top left and bottom right boxes were green [0, 150, 0] and the top right and bottom left boxes were red [250, 0, 0]. These boxes were surrounded by a larger black square, 15.6˚ in length/height1. All objects remained in this position throughout the experiment and there were three fixation dots spread evenly across the horizontal midline between the upper and lower two boxes all 3.4˚ apart from each other. After 500 ms the cue appeared: One of the boxes filled with light grey [200,200,200] for 50ms. At 750ms into the trial (200ms after cue offset) an auditory signal (female voice) said “stay” indicating that fixation was to be maintained on the fixation dot straight ahead. At 1550ms (1050ms after the cue) the target ‘+’ (0.6 degrees) appeared in one of the four boxes and remained visible for 500ms. The cue and target were equally likely to appear in any of the four locations. Ten percent of all trials were catch trials where no target appeared. In Experiment 2, when object motion is introduced, the motion could be seen as either a shift of all four boxes shifting together, or as two boxes jumping from one side to the other (as in the Ternus illusion). The uniquely coloured boxes and a surrounding larger square force the interpretation of four boxes shifting together. For consistency across the experiments this same display was used in Experiment 1. 1 INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 10 In motion trials the same voice said “right” or “left”, indicating that an eye movement to the fixation dot to the right or left of the central fixation dot should be made. The trial ended with the button press or 2750ms after trial onset. After each correct response a positive feedback sound was released. For each incorrect response (responding on catch trials, pressing the wrong button, or not pressing the response button within the given time-interval) a negative feedback sound was released. Each condition (stay, left, right) started with 12 training trials and consisted of 4 blocks with 36 trials in each block. Left and right conditions were collapsed and treated as one motion condition. The order of the conditions was counterbalanced so that the no motion condition occurred equally often as the first, second, and third block. >INSERT FIGURE 1 AROUND HERE < Targets were classified as 1) cued targets on no saccade trials; 2) targets appearing on the same retinotopic location as the cue (on motion trials) or 3) targets appearing in the cued spatiotopic location (on motion trials). As uncued control locations for each of these categories, we used the opposite box in the same column (for example, if the cued location is the upper left square, the uncued location would be the lower-left square, and vice versa). We chose to compare the cued location to its respective mirror uncued location (rather than comparing it to all other uncued locations) as this led to an equal number of trials within each pair of cued and uncued conditions and controlled for distance and direction relative to fixation. Trials falling into the above categories comprised ~60% of trials. The remaining trials were catch trials (11% of all trials) and trials where the cues and targets did not form any of the pairs described above (50% of no motion trials and 25% of motion trials). INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 11 Results and Discussion Trials were excluded from analysis if an eye movement was detected that moved further than 1.3° away from the saccade target (5.0%). Trials with manual reaction times faster than 150ms or slower than 1000ms were also excluded (2.2%). Saccades landed on the target accurately, although small under- and overshoots followed by corrective saccades were common (37.5%). As long as the second fixation was acquired, we included these trials in the analysis because remapping has been shown to be based on the intended, rather than the actual, saccade target (e.g. Bahcall & Kowler, 1999). The saccade-to-target interval (STI) was taken as the duration from the end of the saccade to the onset of the target. In the case of corrected saccades, we used the end time for the saccade that landed on the second fixation. Trials were excluded if STI was greater than 700ms or less than 180ms (17.4%)2. Subjects having less than 50% of trials (i.e. 15 observations) remaining after exclusions in any of the defined target type conditions, or those who pressed the button on more than 15% of catch trials (on average 5.8% erroneous responses were recorded after exclusion) were excluded. On this basis three subjects were excluded. Mean STI, saccadic latency and saccade to target onset asynchrony (STOA) are displayed in Table 1. >INSERT FIGURE 2 AND TABLE 1 AND TABLE 2 AROUND HERE < The results are shown in Figure 2 and Table 2. A 3x2 repeated measures ANOVA with Target Type (no saccade, retinotopic and spatiotopic) and Cueing (cued and uncued) revealed a significant main effect of Cueing, F (1, 24) = 21.612; p < .001; ηp2= .474. The main effect of 2 We decided to drop trials with an STI smaller than 180ms or larger than 700ms to make STI in experiments 1a and 1b more comparable. Target onsets were not time-locked to the end of the eye movements and due to the predictable direction in E1a, STIs were larger than in E1b. Additionally, dropping the extreme STI values in the eye movement experiments made the timing of the retinal motion relative to the target onset time in these two experiments more comparable to Experiment 2. INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 12 Target Type (F (2, 48) = 1.289; p = .286) and the interaction (F < 1) were not significant, indicating that IOR was present to a similar extent, regardless of the Target Type3. IOR was observed in both spatiotopic and retinotopic coordinates across eye movements, consistent with three recent studies (Hilchey et al., 2012; Mathot & Theeuwes, 2010; Pertzov et al., 2010). It is important to note that these three studies used saccadic responses, while we used manual responses. IOR was previously suggested to be more strongly spatiotopic for manual responses and more strongly retinotopic for saccadic responses (Abrams & Pratt, 2000), so we might have expected to produce predominantly spatiotopic IOR in our experiment. However, Sapir et al. (2004) also observed both retinotopic and spatiotopic IOR for manual responses, consistent with our current results. Unlike previous studies, our design also allowed us to directly compare the retinotopic and spatiotopic IOR effect to IOR in a no-eye movement baseline condition. We found no evidence of a significant reduction in the overall magnitude of IOR when the eyes move. In Experiment 1b we randomly interleave the trials of Experiment 1a, so that the directions are mixed and unpredictable at the onset of the cue. If predictability of the saccade direction is an important factor in maintaining inhibitory tags in spatiotopic coordinates, this experiment should produce weaker spatiotopic IOR. Experiment 1b: Saccades in mixed directions Method Participants 3 Unlike Pertzov et al., we did not observe a larger spatiotopic effect when the cue and target appear in the same hemifield. However, there were a number of differences between their experiment and ours that could explain the discrepancy, including response modality and the timing of the target onset relative to the saccade. INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 13 Thirty-one participants (20 female, average age 21.9) participated in this study. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision. Some participants participated for course credit (10), others volunteered. Apparatus, stimulus and procedure Apparatus, stimuli and procedure were the same as in Experiment 1a, with the following exceptions. First, the three different conditions of the separated blocks from Experiment 1a were here randomly interleaved within each block. Second, the word “straight” was used in place of the word “stay” to instruct subjects to maintain fixation on the no saccade trials. Third, the Eyelink 1000 tower mount was used instead of the desktop mount. Fourth, if any eye movements deviated further than 2° away from the horizontal midline (and towards any of the four object markers), an error message was displayed and the trials were recycled (7.4% of trials had to be repeated, but 28.2% of these were due to blinks). The experiment started with 12 practice trials followed by 8 blocks of 54 trials, between which participants could rest. Results and Discussion Trials were excluded from analysis if an eye movement was made in the wrong direction (7.2%), if responses were faster than 150ms or slower than 1000ms (3.5%), or if the wrong button was pressed (1.1%). Trials were also excluded if STI was larger than 700ms or smaller than 180ms (30.5%). Subjects with less than 50% of trials (i.e. 15 observations) in any of the defined target types or with more than 15% responses on catch trials were excluded from the analysis. On this basis nine participants were excluded. The average response rate on catch trials after exclusion was 8.0%. As in Experiment 1a, small corrective saccades were relatively common (22.1%) and trials where they occurred were not excluded. The mean STI was 317.12ms (SEM 5.32), and detailed saccade information can be found in Table 1. INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 14 The results are shown in Figure 2 and Table 2. The same analysis as for Experiment 1a revealed a main effect of Cueing, F (1, 21) = 11.081; p < .005; ηp2 = .345. The main effect of Target Type (F (2, 42) = 1.082; p = .348) and the interaction of Cueing and Target Type (F < 1) were not significant. Direct comparison of Experiment 1a and 1b. The pattern of Experiment 1b looks very similar to Experiment 1a. To statistically evaluate whether the two experiments produced different results, we directly compared Experiment 1a and Experiment 1b in A 2x3x2 ANOVA with Cueing (cued, uncued) and Target Type (no saccade, spatiotopic and retinotopic) as withinsubjects factors and with Experiment (Experiment 1a and 1b) as a between-subjects factor. The analysis revealed a main effect of Cueing, F (1, 45) = 31.597; p < .001; ηp2 = .418, again indicating the presence of IOR. The main effect of Target Type was not significant (F < 1). The between-subjects factor Experiment was significant, F (1, 45) = 5.253; p < .05; ηp2 = 105, reflecting slower RTs in Experiment 1a (predictable eye movement direction). None of the interactions were significant (Target Type and Experiment, F (2, 90) = 1.720; p > .05; Cueing and Experiment, F (1, 45) = 1.200; p > .05; Target Type and Cueing, F < 1), including the threeway interaction (F < 1), indicating that the predictability of eye movement direction had no significant effect on the magnitude of IOR or its spatial distribution. In summary, these findings indicate that IOR can be maintained across a saccade, and that it occurs in both spatiotopic and retinotopic frames of reference. Furthermore, spatiotopic IOR is not dependent on, or strengthened by, the ability to reliably predict the direction of an upcoming eye movement. These results together suggest efference copy could play a key role in maintaining inhibitory tags across changes in retinal position caused by saccades. Experiment 2 tests the role of efference copy by examining whether IOR can be maintained across similarly- INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 15 sized shifts in an object’s retinal location, but when the shifts are produced not by the participant’s own eye movements, but by external object motion. We examined both predictable and unpredictable object motion. Experiment 2: Object Shifts Method Participants Twenty-four participants (15 female, mean age 24.6 years) participated in this study. Participants volunteered (10) or were reimbursed £10 for their time. All participants were recruited through the University of Aberdeen and had normal or corrected to normal vision. Apparatus, stimulus and procedure The stimuli and procedure were the same as Experiment 1a with the following exceptions. There was only one fixation dot in the centre of the screen. Participants were instructed to fixate this fixation dot throughout the trial. In motion trials, the four boxes and the larger surrounding box shifted 3.4° to the left or to the right, 550ms after cue onset (the shift is roughly equal to the average saccade end-time of Experiment 1). This shift resulted in, for example, the left two boxes replacing the right two boxes and the right boxes shifting further into the periphery (and the other way around for a shift to the left). See Figure 3 for reference. In no motion trials, the objects remained in the same position throughout the trial. >INSERT FIGURE 3 AROUND HERE < All participants completed a blocked and a mixed condition. During the blocked condition, the objects would either not shift (72 plus 12 practice trials) or would shift to the right (72 plus 12 practice trials) or to the left (72 plus 12 practice trials) on all trials within a block. During the mixed condition the shifts of the objects was randomly interleaved such that on a INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 16 given trial they could shift to the left, shift to the right, or remain in their initial position (216 plus 12 practice trials). There was a break after every 36 trials throughout the different conditions. To counterbalance, each of the 24 possible orders of these four sets (no motion, left, right, and mixed) was completed by one subject. Subjects attended two experimental sessions and performed all four blocks in each one of them, so the total number of trials in the experiment was 864, not including practice. The sessions lasted 45-60 minutes and there was a minimum delay of four hours and a maximum delay of four days between the first and the second session. Targets were classified in a similar manner as in Experiment 1a and 1b. However, retinotopic targets are now referred to as location-based because they occupy the same location in space and the same location on the retina across object displacement, but are associated with a different object. Object-based targets, similar to spatiotopic targets in Experiment 1, appear on the same object as the previous cue, but after the object has shifted to a different location on the retina. Results and Discussion Trials were excluded from analysis if an eye movement larger than 1.3° away from fixation was detected (5.3%), or if manual reaction time was faster than 150ms or slower than 1000ms (3.3%). Subjects were excluded from analysis if they had less than 50% of trials (i.e. 15 observations) remaining in any one target type condition after these criteria were applied, or if they responded to more than 15% of catch trials. On this basis three participants were excluded. After exclusions, responses were made on 5.1% of catch trials. The results are shown in Figure 2 and Table 3. For simplicity, and to mirror the analyses in Experiment 1a and 1b, we first examined blocked and mixed sessions separately. INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 17 >INSERT TABLE 2 AROUND HERE< Blocked session. We ran a 2x3 repeated-measures ANOVA with Cueing (cued or uncued) and Target Type (no motion, location-based and object-based) as factors. The main effect of Cueing was significant, F (1, 20) = 11.462; p < .005; ηp2 = .364, reflecting the presence of IOR. The main effect of Target Type was also significant, F (3, 60) = 17.608; p < .001; ηp2 = .468, as motion trials had faster RTs than no motion trials. Lastly, Cueing and Target Type qualified for an interaction, F (2, 40) = 4.854; p <.05; ηp2 = .195. Planned comparison confirmed that IOR was present only in no motion trials (t (20) = 4.138; p < .001), and not in any of the motion conditions (ts < 1). Mixed session. The same ANOVA conducted on the mixed session revealed a significant effect of Cueing, F (1, 20) = 8.245; p < .001; ηp2 = .292. The main effect of Target Type was also significant, F (2, 40) = 195.940; p < .001; ηp2 = .907, reflecting slower RTs on no motion trials than motion trials. The interaction of Cueing and Target Type was also significant, F (2, 40) = 5.504; p < .01; ηp2 = .216, indicating that IOR was present in some Target Types but not in others. Planned comparison revealed that this interaction occurred because there was significant IOR in the no motion condition (t (20) = 2.744; p < .05) and in location-based coordinates (t (20) = 2.33; p < .05); but not object-based coordinates (t (20) = 1.716, p > .05; with cued targets having numerically faster RTs). Direct Comparison of blocked and mixed conditions. To evaluate whether predictability of object motion had an effect on the allocation of IOR, we conducted a 2x3x2 ANOVA with Cueing (cued and uncued), Target Type (no motion, location-based, object-based), and Predictability (blocked and mixed) as within-subjects factors. The main effect of Cueing was INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 18 significant, F (1, 20) = 20.756; p < .001; ηp2 = .509, reflecting IOR. The main effect of Target Type was significant, F (2, 40) = 118.452; p < .001; ηp2 = .856, reflecting faster RTs for motion trials. The main effect of Predictability was also significant, F (1, 20) = 7.640; p < .012; ηp2 = .276, reflecting faster RTs in the blocked condition. Target Type and Cueing showed a significant interaction, F (2, 40) = 8.817; p < .001; ηp2 = .306, again reflecting that IOR was present in no motion trials (t (20) = 4.871; p < .001), marginally significant in location-based coordinates (t (20) = 1.996; p = .060), and not significant in object-based coordinates (t (20) < 1). Predictability and Target Type also qualified for an interaction, F (2, 40) = 5.997; p < .005; ηp2 =.231, reflecting that RTs on no motion trials in the mixed condition were slower than in the blocked condition. The interaction of Predictability and Cueing and the three-way interaction were not significant (F < 1). In summary, Experiment 2 revealed significant IOR in the no motion condition, but this IOR effect was eliminated when the objects moved, especially in the blocked condition. In the mixed condition, there was a small but significant location-based IOR effect remaining when the objects moved, but there was no evidence of object-based IOR. The no motion condition produced overall slower RTs than when the objects moved, probably because the object motion acted as an additional temporal signal that the target was about to appear. It is unlikely that IOR was diminished in motion trials due to fast RTs alone, however, because RTs were no faster than in Experiment 1, in which reliable IOR was obtained. Experiments 1 and 2 together suggest that inhibitory tags are automatically updated into spatiotopic coordinates across eye movements, but they are not attached to an object shifting to a similar extent and with similar timing when the motion of the object across the retina is generated externally instead of internally. To directly assess the differences between internally- INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 19 and externally-generated motion we compared the saccade trials in Experiment 1a and the blocked motion condition of Experiment 2. We chose to compare these two conditions in particular because they are matched in terms of the predictability of changes on the retina, with the only difference between them being whether the motion is due to a predictable eye movement or due to the objects themselves shifting in a predictable direction. Comparison of Experiment 1a and 2. A 2x2x2 mixed measures ANOVA with Cueing (cued and uncued) and Reference Frame (retinotopic/ location-based and spatiotopic/ object-based) as within-subject factors and Experiment (Experiment 1a and 2) as the between subjects factor was conducted. The analysis revealed a main effect of Cueing, F (1, 44) = 8.678; p < .005; ηp2 = .165. The main effect of Reference Frame was also significant, F (1, 44) = 5.139; p < .05; ηp2 = .105, reflecting that targets in the spatiotopic reference frame had larger RTs. Cueing and Experiment qualified for an interaction; F (1, 44) = 4.477; p <.05; ηp2=.092; reflecting that IOR was present across eye movements, but not across object motion. No other interactions were significant. The analysis confirmed that the IOR effect was present in the eye movement condition, but eliminated in the predictable object motion condition. Despite the similarities of the changes on the retina and the predictability of these changes, IOR was maintained across self-generated motion, but not across externally-produced motion. General Discussion The present study examined the impact of eye movements and object motion on IOR to establish whether a general prediction-based mechanism facilitates the transfer of inhibitory tags into spatiotopic coordinates across eye movements (E1a and E1b) and into object-based coordinates across object motion (E2). The findings show that IOR is maintained across saccades, in both retinotopic and spatiotopic coordinates, regardless of whether the observer has INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 20 foreknowledge of the direction of an upcoming eye movement. In contrast, and under similar conditions, IOR was eliminated by the external motion of objects compared to trials where no motion occurred. There was no evidence that the IOR effect shifted with a moving object, even when the direction and size of the object’s motion was perfectly predictable. Direct comparison of the eye movement condition and the object motion condition confirmed that IOR is maintained across self-generated retinal motion but eliminated across object motion. The implications of these findings are discussed below. IOR across Eye Movements The current study confirmed that IOR can be reliably observed in the spatiotopic location of cues when a saccade intervenes between the cue and target onset. As such, the study offers further support that IOR represents a bias of attention towards novelty that facilitates visual search (Klein & MacInnes, 1999). However, it is important to note that, like several other studies to date (Abrams & Pratt, 2000; Mathot & Theeuwes, 2010; Pertzov et al., 2010; Sapir et al., 2004), we also found evidence that IOR is retinotopic. The difference in magnitude between spatiotopic and retinotopic IOR was not statistically significant, but it was numerically smaller in retinotopic than in spatiotopic coordinates, which is similar to trends in previous studies (Abrams & Pratt; Pertzov et al.; Sapir et al.). It is possible that restricted laboratory conditions in this and other standard cue-target paradigms encourages the presence of retinotopic IOR. Cue-target paradigms are, unlike real world sceneries, often highly simplified and symmetric. Perhaps the transfer from the originally retinotopic location of the inhibition (before the eye movement) is not a complete process and traces of the retinotopic inhibition remain activated by the imperfection of the orienting mechanism. In real world situations such as free visual search, other factors may overcome the retinotopic inhibition (such as search strategies and a richer and INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 21 more variable scene layout) and therefore the need to fully clear the “retinotopic buffer” of the inhibition may be small. Previous studies have indicated that the reference frame of IOR depends on a number of parameters, such as the nature of the interfering eye movement (Abrams & Pratt, 2000) and the extent of featural salience of the stimuli (Hilchey et al., 2012). As such, predictability might have been expected to play a role in facilitating the updating of inhibition into spatiotopic coordinates. However, our findings showed that spatiotopic IOR occurred regardless of whether the upcoming eye movement direction could be predicted with certainty at the time of the onset of the exogenous cue. This suggests a rapid, involuntary mechanism that remaps inhibitory tags into spatiotopic coordinates across saccades, consistent with saccadic remapping. Location-based and object-based IOR We measured the allocation of IOR across blocked and randomly interleaved object motion conditions to investigate whether the presence or strength of object-based IOR would depend on the ability to predict an object’s upcoming motion with certainty. We only observed consistent IOR in trials when the objects did not move; small but significant location-based IOR was observed on trials where the objects moved in unpredictable directions, that is, in the mixed rather than blocked conditions. This is perhaps surprising, because the blocked condition might have been expected to produce more robust IOR than the mixed condition, given that blocking the motion direction allows observers to predict the future location of the cued object, which better matches the eye movement condition, in which robust IOR was observed. . However, a decrease in the magnitude of IOR following sudden changes in the search environment has been observed previously (e.g. MacInnes & Klein, 2003). Perhaps having the foreknowledge that the objects will move, which observers would only have in the blocked condition, led to an INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 22 attenuation of the IOR effect relative to the mixed condition, in which the objects may or may not move on any given trial. Our findings stand in contrast to previous reports of IOR that is attached to moving objects (e.g. Tipper et al., 1991). However, it is not unprecedented to observe IOR in locationbased coordinates across object-motion (Abrams & Pratt, 2000; Müller & von Mühlenen, 1996), and the current design and that of Tipper et al. differ in many aspects. For example, our objects shifted abruptly from one location to another, rather than moving smoothly as in Tipper et al.. It is possible that the visual system treats sudden motion as a scene change and therefore clears the ‘inhibitory buffer’, as MacInnes and Klein (2003) reported. It is an interesting and open question whether the type of object motion can influence the robustness of object-based IOR. That said, we specifically used sudden object shifts in Experiment 2 to better match the sudden changes that occur on the retina across eye movements that would have been produced in Experiment 1. Direct comparison of the eye movement and object motion condition confirmed that IOR is eliminated across predictable object motion but maintained across eye movements. As such, the current study illustrates the importance of the efference copy signal in maintaining inhibitory tags in locations in space across eye movements: IOR was automatically updated into spatiotopic coordinates across eye movements, but not into object-based coordinates, even across highly predictable object motion. This suggests that predictability alone is not sufficient for the updating of inhibitory tags in dynamic environments and across changing retinotopic maps, supporting the notion that efference copy is a necessary precursor for updating visual space (or visual attention within this space, Cavanagh, Hunt, Afraz & Rolfs, 2010). The suggestion (Abrams & Pratt; Pertzov et al., 2010, Sapir et al., 2004) that the retinotopic locations of inhibitory tags are INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 23 updated rapidly and automatically with each eye movement is thus further supported by the current findings. Conclusion In Experiment 1 we showed that IOR across eye movements is maintained in both retinotopic and spatiotopic reference frames, irrespective of whether the upcoming eye movement direction can be predicted with certainty. In Experiment 2, which matched the conditions of Experiment 1 except in the respect that the objects moved rather than the eyes, object-based IOR was not observed. These results suggest the maintenance of inhibitory tags in spatiotopic coordinates across saccades is an automatic process, likely to be supported by an efferent signal from the oculomotor motor system. The results also support the idea that IOR reflects an inhibitory tagging mechanism that can orient attention towards novel locations during active, overt scanning of the visual environment. INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 24 References Abrams, R. A. & Pratt, J. (2000). Oculocentric coding of inhibited eye movements in recently attended locations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(2), 776-788. Bahcall, D. O. & Kowler, E. (1999). Attentional interference at small spatial separations. Vision Research, 39(1), 71- 86. Cavanagh, P., Hunt, A.R., Afraz, A., & Rolfs, M. (2010). Visual stability based on remapping of attention pointers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 147-153.Chou, W. L., & Yeh, S. L. (2005). Object-based inhibition of return: Evidence from overlapping objects. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 47, 1–13. Chou, W. L., & Yeh, S. L. (2008). Short article location-and object-based inhibition of return are affected by different kinds of working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(12), 1761-1768. Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1(1), 4–45. Dodd, M. D., Van der Stigchel, S., & Hollingworth, A. (2009). Novelty is not always the best policy: Inhibition of return and facilitation of return as a function of visual task. Psychological Science, 20(3), 333-339. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02294.x Duhamel, J. R., Colby, C. L., & Goldberg, M. E. (1992). The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. Science, 255(5040), 90-92. INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 25 Hilchey, M.D.; Klein, R.M.; Satel, J. & Wang, Z. (2012). Oculomotor inhibition of return: How soon is it „recoded“ into spatiotopic coordinates? Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. doi: 10.3758/s13414-012-0312-1 Jordan, H., & Tipper, S. P. (1998). Object-based inhibition of return in static displays. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), 504-509. Klein, R. (1988). Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search. Nature, 334 (6181). 430431. Klein, R. M., & MacInnes, W. J. (1999). Inhibition of return is a foraging facilitator in visual search. Psychological Science, 10(4), 346-352. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00166 Leek, E., Reppa, I., & Tipper, S. (2003). Inhibition of return for objects and locations in static displays. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 65(3), 388-395. Macinnes, J., & Klein, R. M. (2003). Inhibition of return biases orienting during the search of complex scenes. TheScientificWorldJOURNAL, 3, 75–86. Mathôt, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Gradual remapping results in early retinotopic and late spatiotopic inhibition of return. Psychological Science, 21(12), 1793-1798. Maylor, E. A., & Hockey, R. (1985). Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(6), 777-787. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.11.6.777 INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 26 Müller, H. J., & Mühlenen, A. (1996). Attentional tracking and inhibition of return in dynamic displays. Perception & Psychophysics, 58(2), 224-249. Pertzov, Y., Zohary, E., & Avidan, G. (2010). Rapid formation of spatiotopic representations as revealed by inhibition of return. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(26), 8882-8887. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3-25. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma and D. Bonwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X (531–556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Posner, M. I., Rafal, R. D., Choate, L. S., & Vaughan, J. (1985). Inhibition of return: Neural basis and function. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2(3), 211-228. Sapir, A., Hayes, A., Henik, A., Danziger, S., & Rafal, R. (2004). Parietal lobe lesions disrupt saccadic remapping of inhibitory location tagging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(4), 503-509. Smith, T. J., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). Facilitation of return during scene viewing. Visual Cognition, 17, 1027-1034. Smith, T. J., & Henderson, J. M. (2011). Looking back at Waldo: Oculomotor inhibition of return does not prevent return fixations. Journal of Vision, 11(1), 1-11. doi:10.1167/11.1.3. 3. Sommer, M. A., & Wurtz, R. H. (2002). A pathway in primate brain for internal monitoring of movements. Science, 296(5572), 1480-1482. INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 27 Sommer, M. A., & Wurtz, R. H. (2006). Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex. Nature, 444(5279), 374-377. Tipper, S. P., Driver, J., & Weaver, B. (1991). Short report: Object-centred inhibition of return of visual attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 43(2), 289298. Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Jerreat, L. M., & Burak, A. L. (1994). Object-based and environmentbased inhibition of return of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 478-498. van Koningsbruggen, M. G., Gabay, S., Sapir, A., Henik, A., & Rafal, R. D. (2010). Hemispheric asymmetry in the remapping and maintenance of visual saliency maps: A TMS study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1730-1738. INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 28 Table 1. Summary of the saccadic information of Experiment 1a and 1b Experiment 1a Experiment 1b Latency STOA STI Latency STOA STI Before 266.16 533.84 428.59 418.29 381.71 290.12 Exclusion (18.1) (18.8) (18.4) (12.6) (12.6) (10.32) After 262.92 537.11 435.64 392.27 407.73 317.12 Exclusion (14.4) (14.37) (13.1) (12.24) (12.2) (5.3) Note. The mean saccadic latency, saccade to target onset asynchrony (STOA) and saccade to target interval (STI) before and after exclusions of extreme STI (<180, >700). Values in brackets denote the standard error of the mean. INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS Table 2. The mean reaction times and standard deviations (in brackets) in Experiment 1a and 1b. Experiment 1a uncued Experiment 1b Target type cued cued No motion 333.5 (42.2) 321.5 (41.7) 314.1 (37.0) 305.9 (39.3) Retinotopic 336.7 (46.5) 328.6 (49.4) 304.1 (39.6) 298.3 (45.5) Spatiotopic 342.5 (51.6) 331.3 (49.5) 309.9 (36.3) 302.8 (41.5) Note. All cued/uncued pairs differed significantly from each other. Uncued 29 INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS 30 Table 3. The mean reaction times and standard deviations (in brackets) in Experiment 2 Experiment 2 Blocked Target type cued No motion 366.6 (92.4) Location-based Object-based Mixed uncued cued uncued 349.5 (86.5) ** 394.0 (65.4) 381.9 (71.2) * 315.6 (58.6) 311.6 (62.7) 325.4 (65.7) 318.6 (67) * 319.5 (59.5) 319.1 (60.4) 324.1 (62.7) 328.3 (61.9) Note. Pairs of cued and uncued targets that differed significantly from each other are in boldface ((*)p<.1, two tailed. *p<.05, two-tailed. **p<.001, two-tailed.). INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS Figure 1. A schematic representation of Experiment 1. Examples of a spatiotopic and retinotopic target are shown for an eye movement to the right. 31 INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS IOR (cued-uncued) in ms IOR (cued-uncued) in ms Object motion Eye movements Blocked 32 Mixed 25 25 20 20 15 15 10 10 5 5 0 0 -5 -5 -10 -10 25 25 20 20 15 15 10 10 5 5 0 0 -5 -5 -10 -10 No Saccade Retinotopic Spatiotopic No Motion Location-based Object-based Figure 2. IOR (cued-uncued RT) for four different target types in Experiment 1a (top left), Experiment 1b (top right), Experiment 2 blocked (bottom left) and Experiment 2 mixed (bottom right). IOR was significant when no motion occurred (dark gray) and across all target types when an eye movement was made (top row). However, no object-based IOR was observed (bottom row). Error bars indicate within subject variability in the 95% confidence interval (Cousineau, 2005). INHIBITION OF RETURN ACROSS EYE AND OBJECT MOVEMENTS Figure 3. Schematic representation of Experiment 2 trial events. In the example above the objects shifts to the left and the location-based (left) and object-based (right) target locations are shown. 33