(1) for the - University of Oxford Department of Education

advertisement
ESRC Workshop
Researcher Development Initiative
Prof. Herb Marsh
Ms. Alison O’Mara
Dr. Lars-Erik Malmberg
2 June 2008
Department of Education,
University of Oxford
1
 What is meta-analysis,
 When and why we use meta-analysis,
 Examples of meta-analyses,
 Benefits and pitfalls of using meta-analysis,
 Defining a population of studies and finding publications,
 Coding materials,
 Inter-rater reliability,
 Computing effect sizes,
 Structuring a database,
 A conceptual introduction to analysis and interpretation of
results based on fixed effects, random effects, and multilevel
models, and
 Supplementary analyses
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
2
Traditionally, education researchers collect and analyse their
own data (referred to as primary data). Secondary data
analysis is based on data collected by someone else (or,
perhaps, reanalysis of your own published data). There are at
least four logical perspectives to this issue:
1. Meta-analysis -- systematic, quantitative review of
published research in a particular field, the focus of this
presentation.
2. Systematic review -- systematic, qualitative review of
published research in a particular field
3. Secondary Data Analyses -- using large (typically public)
databases
4. Reanalyses of published studies -- (often in ways critical
of the original study).
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
3
Wilson & Lipsey (2001) synthesised 319 meta-analyses of
intervention studies. Across the studies, roughly equal
amounts of variance were due to:
 substantive features of the intervention (true differences),
 method effects (idiosyncratic study features and potential
biases – particularly research design and
operationalisation of outcome measures), and
 sampling error.
They concluded:
These results underscore the difficulty of
detecting treatment outcomes, the importance of
cautiously interpreting findings from a single
study, and the importance of meta-analysis in
summarizing results across studies (p.413).
4
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
 Meta-analysis is an increasingly popular tool for
summarising research findings
 Cited extensively in research literature
 Relied upon by policymakers
 Important that we understand the method, whether
we conduct or simply consume meta-analytic
research
 Should be one of the topics covered in all introductory
research methodology courses
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
5
 What is meta-analysis?
 When and why we use meta-analysis?
6
 Systematic synthesis of various studies on a particular research
question
Do boys or girls have higher self-concepts?
 Collect all studies relevant to a topic
Find all published journal articles on the topic
 An effect size is calculated for each outcome
Determine the size/direction of gender difference for each study
 “Content analysis”
Code characteristics of the study; age, setting, ethnicity, selfconcept domain (math, physical, social), etc.
 Effect sizes with similar features are grouped together and
compared; tests moderator variables
Do gender differences vary with age, setting, ethnicity, self-concept,
domain, etc?
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
7
 Coding: the process of extracting the information
from the literature included in the meta-analysis.
Involves noting the characteristics of the studies in
relation to a priori variables of interest (qualitative)
 Effect size: the numerical outcome to be analysed
in a meta-analysis; a summary statistic of the data
in each study included in the meta-analysis
(quantitative)
 Summarise effect sizes: central tendency,
variability, relations to study characteristics
(quantitative)
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
8
1904:
quant. lit.
review by
Pearson
1977: first
modern
metaanalysis
published by
Smith &
Glass (1977)
Mid-1980s,
methods
develop:
E.g.,
Hedges,
Olkin,
Hunter, &
Schmidt
1990s:
explosion
in
popularity,
esp. in
medical
research
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
9
Karl Pearson conducted what is reputed to be the first metaanalysis (although not called this) comparing effects of
inoculation in different settings.
10
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
Gene Glass coined the phrase meta-analysis in classic
study of the effects of psychotherapy. Because most
individual studies had small sample sizes, the effects
typically were not statistically significant.
 Results of 375 controlled evaluations of psychotherapy
and counselling were coded and integrated statistically.
The findings provide convincing evidence of the efficacy
of psychotherapy.
 On the average, the typical therapy client is better off
than 75% of untreated individuals.
 Few important differences in effectiveness could be
established among many quite different types of
psychotherapy (e.g., behavioral and non-behavioral).
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
11
• The essence of good science is replicable and
generalisable results.
• Do we get the same answer to important
research questions when we run the study
again?
• The primary aims of meta-analysis is to test the
generalisability of results across a set of
studies designed to answer the same research
question.
• Are the results consistent? If not, what are the
differences in the studies that explain the lack
of consistency?
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
12
 A primary aim is to reach a conclusion to a research
question from a sample of studies that is generalisable
to the population of all such studies.
 Meta-analysis tests whether study-to-study variation in
outcomes is more than can be explained by random
chance.
 When there is systematic variation in outcomes from
different studies, meta-analysis tries to explain these
differences in terms of study characteristics: e.g.
measures used; study design; participant
characteristics; controls for potential bias.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
13
 There exists a critical mass of comparable studies
designed to address a common research question.
 Data are presented in a form that allows the metaanalyst to compute an effect size for each study.
 Characteristics of each study are described in
sufficient detail to allow meta-analysts to compare
characteristics of different studies and to judge the
quality of each study.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
14
The number
of metaanalyses is
increasing
at a rapid
rate.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
15
Where are
metaanalyses
done?
All over the
world.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
16
16
All
disciplines
do metaanalyses,
but very
popular in
medicine
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
17
The number & frequency of
citations are increasing in
Education
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
18
The number & frequency of
citations are increasing in
Psychology
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
19
19
20
Amato, P. R., & Keith, B. (1991). Parental divorce and the well-being of
children: A meta-analysis . Psychological Bulletin, 110, 26-46. Times
Cited: 471
Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of
sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis . Child Development, 56,
1479-1498. Times Cited: 570
Johnson, D. W., & et al (1981). Effects of cooperative, competitive, and
individualistic goal structures on achievement: A meta-analysis .
Psychological Bulletin, 89, 47-62. Times Cited: 426
Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein, M. (1991). Personality measures
as predictors of job performance: A meta-analytic review . Personnel
Psychology, 44, 703-742 Times Cited: 387
 Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A
meta-analysis . Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53-69. Times Cited: 316
Iaffaldano, M. T., & Muchinsky, P. M. (1985). Job satisfaction and job
performance: A meta-analysis . Psychological Bulletin, 97, 251-273. Times
Cited: 263.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
21
De Wolff, M., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment:
A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment . Child
Development, 68, 571-591. Times Cited: 340
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theoryof-mind development: The truth about false belief . Child Development,
72, 655-684. Times Cited: 276
Cohen, E. G. (1994). Restructuring the classroom: Conditions for
productive small groups . Review of Educational Research, 64, 1-35.
Times Cited: 235
Hansen, W. B. (1992). School-based substance abuse prevention: A
review of the state of the art in curriculum, 1980-1990 . Health Education
Research, 7, 403-430. Times Cited: 207
Kulik, J. A., Kulik, C-L., Cohen, P. A. (1980). Effectiveness of ComputerBased College Teaching: A Meta-Analysis of Findings. Review of
Educational Research, 50, 525-544. Times Cited: 198.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
22
Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P. R. (1988). The theory of
reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past research with recommendations
for modifications and future research . Journal of Consumer Research,
15, 325-343. Times Cited: 515
Jackson, S. E., & Schuler, R. S. (1985). A meta-analysis and conceptual
critique of research on role ambiguity and role conflict in work settings .
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 36, 16-78.
Times Cited: 401
Tornatzky Lg, Klein Kj. (1994). Innovation characteristics and innovation
adoption-implementation - A meta-analysis of findings . IEEE
Transactions On Engineering Management, 29, 28-4. Times Cited: 269.
Lowe KB, Kroeck KG, Sivasubramaniam N. (1996). Effectiveness
correlates of transformational and transactional leadership: A metaanalytic review of the MLQ literature. Leadership Quarterly, 7, 385-425.
Times Cited: 203.
Churchill GA, Ford NM, Hartley SW, et al. (1985). Title: The determinants
of salesperson performance - A meta-analysis . Journal Of Marketing
Research, 22, 103-118. Times Cited: 189.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
23
 Jadad AR, Moore RA, Carroll D, et al. (1996). Assessing the quality
of reports of randomized clinical trials: Is blinding necessary?
Controlled Clinical Trials, 17, 1-12. Times Cited: 2,008
Boushey Cj, Beresford Saa, Omenn Gs, Et . Al. (1995). A
quantitative assessment of plasma homocysteine as a risk factor
for vascular-disease - Probable benefits of increasing folic-acid
intakes. JAMA-journal Of The American Medical Assoc, 274, 10491057. Times Cited: 2,128
 Alberti W, Anderson G, Bartolucci A, et al. (1995). Chemotherapy
in non-small-cell lung-cancer - A metaanalysis using updated data
on individual patients from 52 randomized clinical-trials. British
Medical Journal, 311, 899-909. Times Cited: 1,591
 Block G, Patterson B, Subar A (1992). Fruit, vegetables, and
cancer prevention - A review of the epidemiologic evidence.
Nutrition And Cancer-an International Journal, 18, 1-29. Times
Cited: 1,422
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
24
 Question: Does feedback from university students’
evaluations of teaching lead to improved teaching?
 Teachers are randomly assigned to experimental (feedback)
and control (no feedback) groups
 Feedback group gets ratings, augmented, perhaps, with
personal consultation
 Groups are compared on subsequent ratings and, perhaps,
other variables
 Feedback teachers improved their teaching
effectiveness by .3 standard deviations compared to
control teachers on the Overall Rating item; even larger
differences for ratings of Instructor Skill, Attitude
Toward Subject, Student Feedback
 Studies that augmented feedback with consultation
produced substantially larger differences, but other
methodological
variations had little effect.
25
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
Question: What is the correlation between university
teaching effectiveness and research productivity?
Based on 58 studies and 498 correlations:
 The mean correlation between teaching effectiveness
(mostly based on Students’ evaluations of teaching)
and research productivity was almost exactly zero;
 This near-zero correlation was consistent across
different disciplines, types of university, indicators of
research, and components of teaching effectiveness.
This meta-analysis was followed by Marsh & Hattie
(2002) primary data study to more fully evaluate
theoretical model
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
26
 Contention about global self-esteem versus multidimensional, domainspecific self-concept
 Traditional reviews and previous meta-analyses of self-concept
interventions have underestimated effect sizes by using an implicitly
unidimensional perspective that emphasizes global self-concept.
 We used meta-analysis and a multidimensional construct validation
approach to evaluate the impact of self-concept interventions for
children in 145 primary studies (200 interventions).
 Overall, interventions were significantly effective (d = .51, 460 effect
sizes).
 However, in support of the multidimensional perspective, interventions
targeting a specific self-concept domain and subsequently measuring
that domain were much more effective (d = 1.16).
 This supports a multidimensional perspective of self-concept
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
27
 Examined predictors of sexual, nonsexual violent,
and general (any) recidivism
 82 recidivism studies
 Identified deviant sexual preferences and antisocial
orientation as the major predictors of sexual
recidivism for both adult and adolescent sexual
offenders. Antisocial orientation was the major
predictor of violent recidivism and general (any)
recidivism
 Concluded that many of the variables commonly
addressed in sex offender treatment programs (e.g.,
psychological distress, denial of sex crime, victim
empathy, stated motivation for treatment) had little
or no relationship with sexual or violent recidivism28
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
 “Epidemiologic studies have suggested that folate intake
decreases risk of cardiovascular diseases. However, the
results of randomized controlled trials on dietary
supplementation with folic acid to date have been
inconsistent”.
 Included 12 studies with randomised control trials.
 The overall relative risks of outcomes for patients treated
with folic acid supplementation compared with controls
were non-significant for cardiovascular diseases, coronary
heart disease, stroke, and for all-cause mortality.
 Concluded folic acid supplementation does not reduce risk
of cardiovascular diseases or all-cause mortality among
participants with prior history of vascular disease.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
29
 In lekking species (those that gather for competitive
mating), a male's mating success can be estimated as the
number of females that he copulates with.
 Aim of the study was to find predictors of lekking species’
mating success through analysis of 48 studies.
 Behavioural traits such as male display activity, aggression
rate, and lek attendance were positively correlated with
male mating success. The size of "extravagant" traits, such
as birds tails and ungulate antlers, and age were also
positively correlated with male mating success.
 Territory position was negatively correlated with male
mating success, such that males with territories close to
the geometric centre of the leks had higher mating success
than other males.
 Male morphology (measure of body size) and territory size
showed small effects on male mating success.
30
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
31
 Compared to traditional literature reviews:
 (1) there is a definite methodology employed in the
research analysis (more like that used in primary
research); and
 (2) the results of the included studies are quantified to a
standard metric thus allowing for statistical techniques
for further analysis.
 Therefore process of reviewing research literature
is more objective, transparent, and replicable; less
biased and idiosyncratic to the whims of a
particular researcher
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
32
Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. D (1994). Reinforcement, reward, and intrinsic
motivation: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 64, 363423.
 Ryan, R., & Deci, E. L. (1996). When paradigms clash: Comments on
Cameron and Pierce's claim that rewards do not undermine intrinsic
motivation. Review of Educational Research, 66, 33-38
Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. D (1996). The debate about rewards and
intrinsic motivation: Protests and accusations do not alter the results.
Review of Educational Research, 66, 39-51.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. (2001). Extrinsic rewards and
intrinsic motivation in education: reconsidered once again. Review of
Educational Research, 71, 1-27.
Cameron, J. (2001). Negative effects of reward on intrinsic motivation: a
limited phenomenon: comment on Deci, Koestner, and Ryan. Review of
Educational Research, 71, 29-42.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
33
 Increased power: by combining information from
many individual studies, the meta-analyst is able
to detect systematic trends not obvious in the XLS
individual studies.
 Conclusions based on the set of studies are likely
to be more accurate than any one study.
 Improved precision: based on information from
many studies, the meta-analyst can provide a
more precise estimate of the population effect size
(and a confidence interval).
 Provides potential corrections for potential biases,
measurement error and other possible artefacts
 Identifies directions for further primary studies to
address unresolved issues.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
34
 Able to establish generalisability across many
studies (and study characteristics).
 Typically there is study-to-study variation in
results. When this is the case, the meta-analyst
can explore what characteristics of the studies
explain these differences (e.g., study design) in
ways not easy to do in individual studies.
 Easy to interpret summary statistics (useful if
communicating findings to a non-academic
audience).
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
35
 Studies that are published are more likely to report
statistically significant findings. This is a source of
potential bias.
 The debate about using only published studies:
 peer-reviewed studies are presumably of a higher quality
VERSUS
 significant findings are more likely to be published than
non-significant findings
 There is no agreed upon solution. However, one
should retrieve all studies that meet the eligibility
criteria, and be explicit with how they dealt with
publication bias. Some methods for dealing with
publication bias have been developed (e.g., Failsafe N, Trim and Fill method).
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
36
Meta-analyses are mostly limited to studies
published in English.
Juni et al. (2002) evaluated the implications of
excluding non-English publications in meta-analyses
of randomised clinical trials in 50 meta-analyses
 treatment effects were modestly larger in non-English
publications (16%).
 However, study quality was also lower in non-English
publications.
Effects were sufficiently small not to have much
influence on treatment effect estimates, but may
make a difference in some reviews.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
37
 Increasingly, meta-analysts evaluate the quality of
each study included in a meta-analysis.
 Sometimes this is a global holistic (subjective)
rating. In this case it is important to have multiple
raters to establish inter-rater agreement (more on
this later).
 Sometimes study quality is quantified in relation to
objective criteria of a good study, e.g.
 larger sample sizes;
 more representative samples;
 better measures;
 use of random assignment;
 appropriate control for potential bias;
 double blinding, and
 low attrition rates (particularly for longitudinal studies)
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
38
 In a meta-analysis of Social Science meta-analyses,
Wilson & Lipsey (1993) found an effect size of .50.
They evaluated how this was related to study quality;
 For meta-analyses providing a global (subjective)
rating of the quality of each study, there was no
significant difference between high and low quality
studies; the average correlations between effect size
and quality was almost exactly zero.
 Almost no difference between effect sizes based on
random- and non-random assignment (effect sizes
slightly larger for random assignment).
 Only study quality characteristic to make a difference
was positively biased effects due to one-group
pre/post design with no control group at all
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
39
Goldring (1990) evaluated the effects of gifted
education programs on achievement. She found a
positive effect, but emphasised that findings were
questionable because of weak studies;
21 of the 24 studies were unpublished and only one
used random assignment.
Effects varied with matching procedures:
 largest effects for achievement outcomes were for
studies in which all non-equivalent groups'
differences controlled by only one pretest variable.
 Effect sizes reduced as the number of control
variables increase and
 disappeared altogether with random assignment.
Goldring (1990, p. 324) concluded policy makers need
to be aware of the limitations of the GAT literature. 40
 Schulz (1995) evaluated study quality in 250 randomized
clinical trials (RCTs) from 33 meta-analyses. Poor quality
studies led to positively biased estimates:
 lack of concealment (30-41%),
 lack of double-blind (17%),
 participants excluded after randomization (NS).
Moher et al. (1998) reanalysed 127 RCTs randomized
clinical trials from 11 meta-analyses for study quality.
Low quality trials resulted in significantly larger effect sizes,
30-50% exaggeration in estimates of treatment efficacy.
Wood et al. (2008) evaluated study quality (1346 RCTs
from 146 meta-analyses.
subjective outcomes: inadequate/unclear concealment & lack
of blinding resulted in substantial biases.
objective outcomes: no significant effects.
conclusion: Systematic reviewers should assess risk of bias.
41
 Meta-analyses should always include subjective
and/or objective indicators of study quality.
 In Social Sciences there is some evidence that
studies with highly inadequate control for preexisting differences leads to inflated effect sizes.
However, it is surprising that other indicators of
study quality make so little difference.
 In medical research, studies largely limited to RCTs
where there is MUCH more control than in social
science research. Here there is evidence that
inadequate concealment of assignment and lack of
double-blind inflate effect sizes, but perhaps only
for subjective outcomes.
 These issues are likely to be idiosyncratic to
individual discipline areas and research questions.
42
 Defining a population of studies and finding
publications
 Coding materials
 Inter-rater reliability
 Computing effect sizes
 Structuring a database
43
Establish
research
question
Define
relevant
studies
Develop code
materials
Data entry
and effect size
calculation
Pilot coding;
coding
Locate and
collate studies
Main analyses
Supplementary
analyses
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
44
 Comparison of treatment & control groups?
What is the effectiveness of a reading skills program for
treatment group compared to an inactive control group?
 Pretest-posttest differences?
Is there a change in motivation over time?
 What is the correlation between two variables?
What is the relation between teaching effectiveness and
research productivity?
 Moderators of an outcome?
Does gender moderate the effect of a peer-tutoring
program on academic achievement?
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
45
 Do you wish to generalise your findings to other
studies not in the sample?
 Do you have multiple outcomes per study? e.g.:
 achievement in different school subjects
 5 different personality scales
 multiple criteria of success
 Such questions determine the choice of metaanalytic model
 fixed effects
 random effects
 multilevel
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
46
 Need to have explicit inclusion and exclusion criteria
 The broader the research domain, the more detailed
they tend to become
 Refine criteria as you interact with the literature
 Components of a detailed search criteria
 distinguishing features
 research respondents
 key variables
 research methods
 cultural and linguistic range
 time frame
 publication types
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
47
Search electronic databases (e.g., ISI,
Psychological Abstracts, Expanded Academic
ASAP, Social Sciences Index, PsycINFO, and
ERIC)
Examine the reference lists of included studies
to find other relevant studies
If including unpublished data, email researchers
in your discipline, take advantage of Listservs,
and search Dissertation Abstracts International
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
48
The following is one possible way to write up the
search procedure (see LeBlanc & Ritchie, 2001)
1. Electronic search strategy (e.g., PsycINFO &
Dissertation Abstracts). Provide years included in
database
2. Keywords and limitations of the search (e.g.,
language)
3. Additional search methods (e.g., mailing lists)
4. Exclusion criteria (e.g., must contain control group)
5. Yield of the search—number of studies found. Ideally
should also mention how many were excluded from
the meta-analysis and why
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
49
1
2
3
4
5
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
50
 Inclusion process
usually requires
several steps to
cull inappropriate
studies
 Example from Bazzano,
L. A., Reynolds, K.,
Holder, K. N., & He, J.
(2006).Effect of Folic
Acid Supplementation on
Risk of Cardiovascular
Diseases: A Metaanalysis of Randomized
Controlled Trials. JAMA,
296, 2720-2726
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
51
You can report the
inclusion/exclusion
process using text
rather than a flow
chart, but is not
as easy to follow if
it is an elaborate
process.
Should report
original sample and
final yield as a
minimum (in this
case, original = 139,
final = 22)
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
52
Code Sheet
__
1 Study ID
_99_
Year of publication
__
2
Publication type (1-5)
__
1
Geographical region (1-7)
_87_ _ _ Total sample size
_41_ _
Total number of males
_46_ _
Total number of females
Code Book/manual
Publication type (1-5)
1. Journal article
2. Book/book chapter
3. Thesis or doctoral
dissertation
4. Technical report
5. Conference paper
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
53
Mode of therapy,
Duration of therapy,
Participant characteristics,
Publication characteristics,
Design characteristics
Coding characteristics should be mentioned in the paper.
If the editor allows, a copy of the actual coding materials
can be included as an appendix
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
54
Random selection of papers coded by both
coders (e.g., 30% of publications are doublecoded)
Meet to compare code sheets
Where there is discrepancy, discuss to reach
agreement
Amend code materials/definitions in code book
if necessary
May need to do several rounds of piloting, each
time using different papers
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
55
Percent agreement: Common but not recommended
Cohen’s kappa coefficient
 Kappa is the proportion of the optimum improvement
over chance attained by the coders, where a value of 1
indicates perfect agreement and a value of 0 indicates
that agreement is no better than that expected by chance
 Kappa’s over .40 are considered to be a moderate level of
agreement (but no clear basis for this “guideline”)
Correlation between different raters
Intraclass correlation. Agreement among multiple
raters corrected for number of raters using
Spearman-Brown formula (r)
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
56
 The purpose of this exercise is to explore various
issues of meta-analytic methodology
 Discuss in groups of 3-4 people the following
issues in relation to the gender differences in
smiling study (LaFrance et al., 2003)
1. Did the aims of the study justify conducting a meta-
analysis?
2. Was selection criteria and the search process explicit?
3. How did they deal with interrater (coder) reliability?
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
57
1. Extend previous meta-analyses, include previously
2.
3.
untested moderators based on theory/empirical
observations
Search process: detailed databases and 5 other sources
of studies, search terms. Selection criteria: justification
provided (e.g., for excluding under the age of 13).
However, not clear how many studies were retrieved
and then eventually included (compare with flow chart
on slide 51)
Multiple coders (group of coders consisted of four
people with two raters of each sex coding each
moderator). Interrater reliability was calculated by
taking the aggregate reliability of the four coders at
each time using the Spearman–Brown formula
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
58
59
The effect size makes meta-analysis possible
 It is based on the “dependent variable” (i.e., the outcome)
 It standardizes findings across studies such that they can
be directly compared
Any standardized index can be an “effect size” (e.g.,
standardized mean difference, correlation coefficient,
odds-ratio), but must
 be comparable across studies (standardization)
 represent magnitude & direction of the relation
 be independent of sample size
Different studies in same meta-analysis can be based
on different statistics, but have to transform each to a
standardized effect size that is comparable across
different studies
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
60
Study 1
N
M
SD
t
p
d
Cntr
10
100
15
Exp
10
105
15
Study 3
N
M
SD
t
p
d
Study 2
N
M
SD
t
p
d
Cntr
100
100
15
Cntr
50
100
15
Exp
50
105
15
Exp
100
105
15
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
61
Study 1
N
M
SD
t
p
d
Cntr
10
100
15
Exp
10
105
15
-0.750
0.466
0.333
Study 3
N
M
SD
t
p
d
Study 2
N
M
SD
t
p
d
Cntr
100
100
15
Cntr
50
100
15
Exp
50
105
15
-1.667
0.099
0.333
Exp
100
105
15
-2.360
0.019
0.333
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
XLS
62
 O’Mara (2004)
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
63
 Within the one meta-analysis, can include studies
based on any combination of statistical analysis
(e.g., t-tests, ANOVA, multiple regression,
correlation, odds-ratio, chi-square, etc). However,
you have to convert each of these to a common
“effect size” metric.
 Lipsey & Wilson (2001) present many formulae for
calculating effect sizes from different information.
The “art” of meta-analysis is how to compute effect
sizes based on non-standard designs and studies
that do not supply complete data.
 However, need to convert all effect sizes into a
common metric, typically based on the “natural”
metric given research in the area. E.g. standardized
mean difference; odds-ratio; correlation, etc.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
64
 Standardized mean difference
 Group contrast research
 Treatment groups
 Naturally occurring groups
 Inherently continuous construct
 Odds-ratio
 Group contrast research
 Treatment groups
 Naturally occurring groups
 Inherently dichotomous construct
 Correlation coefficient
 Association between variables research
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
65
 Represents a standardized group contrast on an
inherently continuous measure
 Uses the pooled standard deviation (some
situations use control group standard deviation)
 Commonly called “d”
X  X G2
ES  G1
s pooled
If n1  n2 s pooled 
In a gender difference study,
the effect size might be:
In an intervention study with
experimental and control
groups, the effect size might be:
ES 
X Males  X Females
SD pooled
ES 
s12  s22
2
X Exper  X Control
SD pooled
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
66
Means and standard
deviations
Correlations
P-values
F-statistics
t-statistics
d
Almost all test
statistics can
be transformed
into an
standardized
effect size “d”
“other” test statistics
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
67
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
68
 Represents the strength of association between
two inherently continuous measures
 Generally reported directly as r (the Pearson
product moment coefficient)
ES  r
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
69
 The odds-ratio is based on a 2 by 2 contingency
table
 The odds-ratio is the odds of success in the
treatment group relative to the odds of success in
the control group
Frequencies
Success
Failure
Treatment Group
a
b
Control Group
c
d
ad
ES OR 
bc
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
70
265  75
ES OR 
 3.044
32  204
log e (3.044)  1.113
1.113 / 1.83  0.61
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
71
Alternatively: transform rs
into Fisher’s Zr-transformed
rs, which are more normally
distributed
r
0.90
0.80
0.70
0.60
0.50
0.40
0.30
0.20
0.10
0.00
-0.10
-0.20
-0.30
-0.40
-0.50
-0.60
-0.70
-0.80
-0.90
d
4.13
2.67
1.96
1.50
1.15
0.87
0.63
0.41
0.20
0.00
-0.20
-0.41
-0.63
-0.87
-1.15
-1.50
-1.96
-2.67
-4.13
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
72
Hedges proposed a correction for small sample size
bias (n < 20)
Must be applied before analysis
ES
'
sm
3 

 ES sm 1 
 4 N  9 
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
73
The effect sizes are weighted by the inverse of the
variance to give more weight to effects based on
large sample sizes
Variance is calculated as
d i2
(n1  n 2 )
vi 

(n1 n 2 )
2( n1  n 2 )
The standard error of each effect size is given by
the square root of the sampling variance
SE =  vi
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
74
Population
N - ‘size’
M - ‘mean’
d = effect size
Sample
n - ‘size’
m - ‘mean’
d = effect size
Interval estimates
The “likely” population
parameter is the sample
parameter ± uncertainty
 Standard errors (s.e.)
 Confidence intervals (C.I.)
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
75
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
76
Each study is one lineVariance
in
of the effect size
Sample sizes
the data base Effect size
DurationReliability of
the instrument
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
77
 Fixed effects model
 Random effects model
 Multilevel model
78
Includes the entire population of studies to be
considered; do not want to generalise to other
studies not included (e.g., future studies).
All of the variability between effect sizes is due to
sampling error alone. Thus, the effect sizes are only
weighted by the within-study variance.
Effect sizes are independent.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
79
There are 2 general ways of conducting a fixed
effects meta-analysis: ANOVA & multiple regression
The analogue to the ANOVA homogeneity analysis
is appropriate for categorical variables
 Looks for systematic differences between groups of
responses within a variable
Multiple regression homogeneity analysis is more
appropriate for continuous variables and/or when
there are multiple variables to be analysed
 Tests the ability of groups within each variable to predict
the effect size
 Can include categorical variables in multiple regression
as dummy variables. (ANOVA is a special case of
multiple regression)
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
80
ES i
The homogeneity (Q) test asks whether the different effect sizes
are likely to have all come from the same population (an
assumption of the fixed effects model). Are the differences
among the effect sizes no bigger than might be expected by
chance?

Q   wi ES iES

2
ES i = effect size for each study (i = 1 to k)
ES = mean effect size
wi
= a weight for each study based on the sample size
However, this (chi-square) test is heavily dependent on sample size. It is
almost always significant unless the numbers (studies and people in
each study) are VERY small. This means that the fixed effect model will
almost always be rejected in favour of a random effects model.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
81
Run MATRIX procedure:
*****
Meta-Analytic Results
*****
------- Distribution Description --------------------------------N
15.000
Min ES
.050
Max ES
1.200
Wghtd SD
.315
------- Fixed & Random Effects Model ----------------------------Mean ES
-95%CI
+95%CI
SE
Z
P
Fixed
.4312
.3383
.5241
.0474
9.0980
.0000
Random
.3963
.2218
.5709
.0890
4.4506
.0000
------- Random Effects Variance Component -----------------------v
=
.074895
------- Homogeneity Analysis ------------------------------------Q
df
p
44.1469
14.0000
.0001
Random effects v estimated via
------ END MATRIX -----
Significant heterogeneity in the
effect sizes therefore random
noniterative method of moments.
effects more appropriate and/or
moderators need to be modelled
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
82
 Model moderators by grouping effect sizes that are
similar on a specific characteristic
 For example, group all effect size outcomes that
come from studies using a placebo control group
design and compare with effect sizes from studies
using a waitlist control group design
 So in this example, ‘Design’ is a dichotomous
variable with the values 0 = placebo control and 1 =
waitlist control
Exp.
cond
ES
design
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
83
 On the next slide, we will look at the outcomes of a
study to show the importance of various moderator
variables
 Do Psychosocial and Study Skill Factors Predict
College Outcomes? A Meta-Analysis
 Robbins, Lauver, Le, Davis, Langley, & Carlstrom (2004).
Psychological Bulletin, 130, 261–288
 Aim:
 To examine the relationship between psychosocial and
study skill factors (PSFs) and college retention by metaanalyzing 109 studies
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
84
N = sample size for that
variable
k = number of correlation
coefficients on which each
distribution was based
r = mean observed
correlation
CIr 10% = lower bound of
the confidence interval for
observed r
CIr 90% = upper bound of
the confidence interval for
observed r
Institutional size
smallest effect size
Not statistically
significant because CI
Statistically significant contains zero
Academic related skills because CI does not
largest effect size ESRC RDI One
85
contain
zeroworkshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
Day Meta-analysis
Target self-concept domains are those that are directly
relevant to the intervention
Target-related are those that are logically relevant to
the intervention, but not focal
Non-target are domains that are not expected to be
enhanced by the intervention
Regression Coefficients and their
standard errors
B
SE
Target
.4892
.0552
Target-related .1097
.0587
Non-target
.0805
.0489
Sig?
yes
no
no
From O’Mara, Marsh, Craven, & Debus (2006)
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
86
Is only a sample of studies from the entire
population of studies to be considered. As a result,
do want to generalise to other studies not included
in the sample (e.g., future studies).
Variability between effect sizes is due to sampling
error plus variability in the population of effects.
Effect sizes are independent.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
87
 If the homogeneity test is rejected (it almost always
will be), it suggests that there are larger differences
than can be explained by chance variation (at the
individual participant level). There is more than one
“population” in the set of different studies.
 Now we turn to the random effects model to
determine how much of this between-study
variation can be explained by study characteristics
that we have coded.
 The total variance associated with the effect sizes
has two components, one associated with
differences within each study (participant level
variation) and one between study variance:
vTi  v  vi
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
88
 Do Self-Concept Interventions Make a Difference? A
Synergistic Blend of Construct Validation and MetaAnalysis
 O’Mara, Marsh, Craven, & Debus. (2006). Educational
Psychologist, 41, 181–206
 Aim:
 To examine what factors moderate the effectiveness of
self-concept interventions by meta-analyzing 200
interventions
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
90
 QB = between group homogeneity. If the QB value is
significant, then the groups (categories) are
significantly different from each other
 QW = within group homogeneity. If QW is significant,
then the effect sizes within a group (category) Note that
differ significantly from each other
the fixed
Only 2 variables had significant QB in the random effects model.
‘Treatment characteristics’ also had significant QW.
effects
are more
significant
than
random
effects
91
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
Run MATRIX procedure:
*****
Meta-Analytic
Significant heterogeneity in
Results *****
the effect sizes therefore
need to model moderators
Description ---------------------------------
------- Distribution
N
Min ES
15.000
.050
Max ES
1.200
Wghtd SD
.315
------- Fixed & Random Effects Model ----------------------------Mean ES
-95%CI
+95%CI
SE
Z
P
Fixed
.4312
.3383
.5241
.0474
9.0980
.0000
Random
.3963
.2218
.5709
.0890
4.4506
.0000
------- Random Effects Variance Component -----------------------v
=
.074895
------- Homogeneity Analysis ------------------------------------Q
df
p
44.1469
14.0000
.0001
v 
Random effects v estimated via noniterative method of moments.
------ END MATRIX -----
Q  ( k  1)
w 
i
 wi 2
 wi92
Meta-analytic data is inherently hierarchical (i.e.,
effect sizes nested within studies) and has random
error that must be accounted for
Effect sizes are not necessarily independent
Allows for multiple effect sizes per study
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
93
New technique that is still being developed
Provides more precise and less biased
estimates of between-study variance than
traditional techniques
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
94
 Level 1: outcome-level component
 Effect sizes
 Level 2: study component
 Publications
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
95
Intercept-only model, which incorporates both the
outcome-level and the study-level components
(similar to a random effects model)
Expand model to include predictor variables, to
explain systematic variance between the study
effect sizes
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
96
 Acute Stressors and Cortisol Responses: A
Theoretical Integration and Synthesis of Laboratory
Research
 Dickerson & Kemeny (2004). Psychological Bulletin, 130,
355–391.
 Aim:
 To examine methodological predictors of cortisol
responses in a meta-analysis of 208 laboratory studies of
acute psychological stressors
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
97
 Only 2 variables significant (Quad Time between
stress onset & assessment; Time of day). The
quadratic component is difficult to interpret as an
unstandardized regression coefficient, but the
graph suggests it is meaningfully large
Quadratic Function of
time since Onset
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
98
Fixed, random, or multilevel?
Generally, if more than one effect size per study is
included in sample, multilevel should be used
However, if there is little variation at study level, the
results of multilevel modelling meta-analyses are
similar to random effects models
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
99
 Do you wish to generalise your findings to other
studies not in the sample?
Yes – random
No – fixed
effects or
effects
multilevel
 Do you have multiple outcomes per study?
Yes –
multilevel
No – random
effects or
fixed effects
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
100
 The purpose of this exercise is to consider choice
of meta-analytic method
 Discuss in groups of 3-4 people the question in
relation to the gender differences in smiling study
(LaFrance et al., 2003)
 Is there independence of effect sizes? What are the
implications for model choice (fixed, random,
multilevel)?
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
101
Fail-safe N
Power analysis
Trim-and-fill method
102
The fail-safe N (Rosenthal, 1991) determines the
number of studies with an effect size of zero
needed to lower the observed effect size to a
specified (criterion) level.
For example, assume that you want to test the
assumption that an effect size is at least .20.
If the observed effect size was .26 and the fail-safe
N was found to be 44, this means that 44
unpublished studies with a mean effect size of zero
would need to be included in the sample to reduce
the observed effect size of .26 to .20.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
103
Power is a term used to describe the probability of
a statistical test committing Type II error. That is, it
indicates the likelihood that the test has failed to
reject the null hypothesis, which implicitly suggests
that there is no effect when in reality there is.
Power, sample size, significance level, and effect
size are inter-related.
A lower powered study has to exhibit a much larger
effect size to produce a significant finding. This has
ramifications for publication bias.
Muncer, Craigie, & Holmes (2003) recommend
conducting a power analysis on all studies included
in the meta-analysis
Compare the observed value (d) against a theoretical
value (includes information about sample size)
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
104
Trim and fill procedure (Duval & Tweedie, 2000a,
2000b) calculates the effect of potential data
censoring (including publication bias) on the
outcome of the meta-analyses.
Nonparametric, iterative technique examines the
symmetry of effect sizes plotted by the inverse of
the standard error. Ideally, the effect sizes should
mirror on either side of the mean.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
105
Examining the methods and output of published meta-analysis
106
 Discuss in groups of 3-4 people the following
question in relation to the gender differences in
smiling study (LaFrance et al., 2003)
 How did they deal with publication bias? Does this
seem appropriate?
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
107
 The purpose of this exercise is to practice reading
meta-analytic results tables.
 This study, by Reger et al. (2004), examines the
relationship between neuropsychological
functioning and driving ability in dementia.
1. In Table 3, which variables are homogeneous for
the “on-road tests” driving measure in the “All
Studies” column? What does this tell you about
those variables?
2. In Table 4, look at the variables that were
homogeneous in question (1) for the “on-road
tests” using “All Studies”. Which variables have a
significant mean ES? Which variable has the
largest mean ES?
108
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
1. Homogeneous variables (non-significant Q2.
values): Mental status–general cognition,
Visuospatial skills, Memory, Executive functions,
Language
All of the relevant mean effect sizes are
significant. Memory and language are tied as the
largest mean ESs for homogeneous variables (r =
.44)
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
109
110
 We established what meta-analysis is, when and
why we use meta-analysis, and the benefits and
pitfalls of using meta-analysis
 Summarised how to conduct a meta-analysis
 Provided a conceptual introduction to analysis and
interpretation of results based on fixed effects,
random effects, and multilevel models
 Applied this information to examining the methods
of a published meta-analysis
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
111
 Comparing apples and oranges
 Quality of the studies included in the meta-analysis
 What to do when studies don’t report sufficient
information (e.g., “non-significant” findings)?
 Including multiple outcomes in the analysis (e.g.,
different achievement scores)
 Publication bias
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
112
 With meta-analysis now one of the most popularly
published research methods, it is an exciting time
to be involved in meta-analytic research
 The hottest topics in meta-analysis are:
 Multilevel modelling to address the issue of
independence of effect sizes
 New methods in publication bias assessment (Trim-andfill method, post hoc power analysis)
 Also receiving attention:
 Establishing guidelines for conducting meta-analysis
(best practice)
 Meta-analyses of meta-analyses
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
113
Purpose-built
 Comprehensive Meta-analysis (commercial)
 Schwarzer (free, http://userpage.fuberlin.de/~health/meta_e.htm)
Extensions to standard statistics packages
 SPSS, Stata and SAS macros, downloadable from
http://mason.gmu.edu/~dwilsonb/ma.html
 Stata add-ons, downloadable from
http://www.stata.com/support/faqs/stat/meta.html
 HLM – V-known routine
 MLwiN
 Mplus
 Please note that we do not advocate any one programme over another, and cannot guarantee
the quality of all of the products downloadable from the internet. This list is not exhaustive.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
114
 Cooper, H., & Hedges, L. V. (Eds.) (1994). The
handbook of research synthesis (pp. 521–529). New
York: Russell Sage Foundation.
 Hox, J. (2003). Applied multilevel analysis.
Amsterdam: TT Publishers.
 Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of
meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in
research findings. Newbury Park: Sage
Publications.
 Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical metaanalysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
115
 Pick up a brochure about our intermediate and
advanced meta-analysis courses
 Visit our website
http://www.education.ox.ac.uk/research/resgroup/self/training.php
ESRC RDI One Day Meta-analysis workshop (Marsh, O’Mara, Malmberg)
116
Download